Hostname: page-component-78c5997874-ndw9j Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-11-05T05:33:22.220Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Contextual Motivation and Working Memory

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  27 June 2016

Ronald H. Smyth*
Affiliation:
Carleton University

Extract

This study examines certain properties of human memory which impose limits on topic accessibility in discourse. While the discussion is centred on so-called “dative” and “cleft” sentences in English, the work is motivated by more general concerns which have been variously referred to as “topicworthiness” (Thompson 1988), “information flow” (Chafe 1987; Kuno, 1987), “topic continuity” (Givón 1983), “degree of presuppositionality” (Givón 1979), “pragmatic perspective” (Dik 1978), “packaging phenomena” (Chafe 1976), “staging” (Grimes 1975), “communicative dynamism” (Firbas 1967), “the theme system” (Halliday 1967), and “linear modification” (Bolinger 1952). The term adopted here is “information flow”.

One important aspect of the study of information flow is the attempt to determine the conditions under which speakers make selections from among alternative syntactic expressions of identical propositional meaning. While some of the motivation for such choices lies in sentence-internal factors such as the animacy or definiteness of NPs (e.g., Ransom 1979), I shall restrict the discussion here to the issue of contextual motivation as originally developed in Smyth (1977) and in Smyth et al (1979). A particular syntactic choice is said to be contextually motivated if the information flow features which govern it are external to the sentence, and the alternative structure is contextually inappropriate. Where contextual motivation is absent, I claim that either of the candidate syntactic forms is appropriate, although sentence-internal information flow factors may then exert a weaker influence on syntactic choice.

Type
Research Article
Copyright
Copyright © Canadian Linguistic Association 1988

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

Akmajian, Adrian 1970 On Deriving Cleft Sentences from Pseudo-Cleft Sentences. Linguistic Inquiry 1:149168.Google Scholar
Bolinger, Dwight 1952 Linear Modification. Proceedings of the Modern Language Association 67:11171144.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bolkestein, A. Machtelt 1985 Cohesiveness and Syntactic Variation: Quantitative vs. Qualitative Grammar. Pp. 114 in Syntax and Pragmatics in Functional Grammar. Bolkestein, A. Machtelt, Groot, Casper de, and Mackenzie, J. Lachlan eds. Dordrecht: Foris Publications.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bower, Gordon H., and Cirilo, Randolph K. 1985 Cognitive Psychology and Text Processing. Pp. 71106 in Handbook of Discourse Analysis, Vol. 1: Disciplines of Discourse. Dijk, Teun A. Van, ed. New York: Academic Press.Google Scholar
Chambers, Jack 1970 Focused Noun Phrases in English Syntax. Ph.D. dissertation, University of Alberta.Google Scholar
Chafe, Wallace 1976 Givenness, Contrastiveness, Definiteness, Subjects and Topics. Pp. 2556 in Subject and Topic, Li, Charles, ed. New York: Academic Press.Google Scholar
Chafe, Wallace 1987 Cognitive Constraints on Information Flow. Pp. 2151 in Coherence and Grounding in Discourse. Tomlin, Russell, ed. Amsterdam: John Benjamins.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Chomsky, Noam 1971 Deep Structure, Surface Structure, and Semantic Interpretation. In Semantics: An Interdisciplinary Reader in Philosophy, Linguistics, Anthropology, and Psychology. Steinberg, Danny, and Jakobovitz, Leon, eds. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Culicover, Peter 1977 Some Observations Concerning Pseudo-Clefts. Linguistic Analysis 3:347375.Google Scholar
Dik, Simon C. 1978 Functional Grammar. Amsterdam: North Holland.Google Scholar
Emonds, Joseph 1976 A Transformational Approach to English Syntax: Root, Structure Preserving, and Local Transformations. New York: Academic Press.Google Scholar
Erteschik-Shir, Nomi 1979 Discourse Constraints on Dative Movement. Pp. 441467 in Discourse and Syntax (Syntax and Semantics 12). Givón, Talmy, ed. New York: Academic Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Fillmore, Charles 1965 Indirect Object Constructions in English and the Ordering of Transformations. The Hague: Mouton.Google Scholar
Firbas, Jan 1967 It was yesterday that … Sbornick Praci Filosoficke Fakulty Brnenske University A15:141146.Google Scholar
Fletcher, Paul 1973 Experimental Approach to Linguistic Paraphrase. Ph.D. dissertation, University of Alberta.Google Scholar
Fox, John 1979 Comment on Harris, Luginbuhl, and Fishbein: Loglinear and Logit Models for Tabular Experimental Data. Social Psychology Quarterly 42:431433.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Givón, Talmy 1975 Promotion, Accessibility and Case-Marking: Toward Understanding Grammar. Working Papers in Language Universals, Vol. 19. Stanford University. Cited in Givón (1983).Google Scholar
Givón, Talmy 1979 On Understanding Grammar. New York: Academic Press.Google Scholar
Givón, Talmy 1984a Syntax: A Functional-Typological Introduction, Vol. 1. Amsterdam: John Benjamins.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Givón, Talmy 1984b Direct Object and Dative Shifting: Semantic and Pragmatic Case. Pp. 151182 in Objects: Towards a Theory of Grammatical Relations. Plank, Frans, ed. New York: Academic Press.Google Scholar
Givón, Talmy, ed. 1983 Topic Continuity in Discourse. Amsterdam: John Benjamins.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Green, Georgia 1974 Semantics and Syntactic Regularity. Bloomington: Indiana University Press.Google Scholar
Halliday, Michael 1967 Notes on Transitivity and Theme in English: I. Journal of Linguistics 3:3781.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Haviland, Susan, and Clark, Herbert 1974 What’s New? Acquiring New Information as a Process of Comprehension. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior 13:512521.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hornby, Peter 1971 The Role of Topic-Comment in the Recall of Cleft and Pseudocleft Sentences. Proceedings of the Chicago Linguistic Society 7:445453.Google Scholar
Jespersen, Otto 1933 Essentials of English Grammar. London: Allen and Unwin.Google Scholar
Kuno, Susumu 1987 Functional Syntax: Anaphora, Discourse and Empathy. Chicago: The University of Chicago Press.Google Scholar
Kuroda, S.-Y. 1968 Review of Fillmore Indirect Object Constructions in English and the Ordering of Transformations (1965). Language 44:374378.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Li, Charles, ed. 1976 Subject and Topic. New York: Academic Press.Google Scholar
Oehrle, Richard 1976 The Grammatical Status of the English Dative Alternation. Ph.D. dissertation, MIT.Google Scholar
Perlmutter, David 1980 Relational Grammar. Pp. 195229 in Current Approaches to Syntax (Syntax and Semantics 13). Moravcsik, Edith, and Wirth, Jessica, eds. New York: Academic Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Plank, Frans, ed. 1985 Objects: Towards a Theory of Grammatical Relations. New York: Academic Press.Google Scholar
Prince, Elizabeth 1978 A Comparison of WH-Clefts and It-Clefts in Discourse. Language 54:883906.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ransom, Evelyn 1979 Definiteness and Animacy Constraints on Passive and Double-Object Constructions in English. Glossa 13:215240.Google Scholar
Rochemont, Michael 1986 Focus in Generative Grammar. Amsterdam: John Benjamins.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Sachs, Jacqueline 1967 Recognition Memory for Syntactic and Semantic Aspects of Connected Discourse. Perception and Psychophysics 2:437442.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Sachs, Jacqueline 1974 Memory in Reading and Listening to Discourse. Memory and Cognition 2:95100.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Smyth, Ronald 1977 Context Effects in Memory for Information Focus. M.Sc. thesis, University of Alberta.Google Scholar
Smyth, Ronald 1986 Cognitive Aspects of Anaphora Judgment and Resolution. Bloomington: Indiana University Linguistics Club.Google Scholar
Smyth, Ronald, Prideaux, Gary, and Hogan, John 1979 The Effect of Context on Dative Position. Lingua 47:2742.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Thompson, Sandra 1988 Information Flow and ‘Dative Shift’ in English Discourse. Technical Report No. 8819. Santa Barbara: Center for Cognitive Science Research, University of California.Google Scholar
Thompson, Sandra, and Koide, Yuka 1987 Iconicity and ‘Indirect Objects’ in English. Journal of Pragmatics 11:399406.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Werth, Paul 1984 Focus, Coherence and Emphasis. London: Croom Helm.Google Scholar