No CrossRef data available.
Article contents
Perspective psycholinguistique de l’apprentissage de la grammaire
Published online by Cambridge University Press: 27 June 2016
Extract
Un modèle psycho-linguistique adéquat de la grammaire doit rendre compte de l’apprentissage des structures grammaticales (point de vue diachronique) et de l’encodage et du décodage de ces structures (point de vue séquentiel). Le premier est un problème d’ordre génétique, le deuxième d’ordre fonctionnel; notre discussion sera axée sur cette distinction.
- Type
- Research Article
- Information
- Canadian Journal of Linguistics/Revue canadienne de linguistique , Volume 14 , Issue 2 , Spring 1969 , pp. 123 - 132
- Copyright
- Copyright © Canadian Linguistic Association 1969
References
Bibliographie
Ausubel, D. P. (1963), The Psychology of Meaningful Verbal Learning. New York: Grune & Stratton.Google Scholar
Ausubel, D. P. (1964), “A cognitive structure view of word and concept meaning,” in Anderson, R. C., and Ausubel, D. P., Readings in the psychology of cognition. New York: Holt.Google Scholar
Barrutia, R. (1966), “Some pedagogical dangers in recent linguistic trends,” in International Review of Applied Linguistics, IV, 1966, 3, 157–64.Google Scholar
Belyayev, B. V. (1963), The Psychology of Teaching Foreign Languages. Trans. from the Russian. Oxford: Pergamon Press.Google Scholar
Bohlen, A. (1963), Methodik des neusprachlichen Unterrichts. Heidelberg: Quelle und Meyer.Google Scholar
Carroll, J. B. (1939), “Determining and numerating adjectives in children’s speech,” Child Development, 10, 215–29.Google Scholar
Carroll, J. B. (1958), “A factor analysis of two foreign language aptitude batteries,” Journal of General Psychology, 59, 3–19.Google Scholar
Carroll, J. B. (1961), “Language development in children,” in Saporta, S. (ed.), Psycholinguistics. New York: Holt.Google Scholar
Cousinet, R. (1950), “La grammaire et l’orthographe,” L’Ecole Nouvelle Française, Octobre, 13.Google Scholar
Fodor, J. A. (1966), “How to learn to talk: some simple ways,” in Smith, F., and Miller, G. A., The Genesis of Language. Cambridge, Mass.: M.I.T., 105–122.Google Scholar
Galichet, G. (1949), Physiologie de la langue française. Paris: Presses Universitaires de France.Google Scholar
Galichet, G. (1950), Essai de grammaire psychologique. Paris: Presses Universitaires de France.Google Scholar
Galichet, G. (1953), Méthodologie grammaticale. Etude psychologique des structures. Paris: Presses Universitaires de France.Google Scholar
Guillaume, p. (1927), Les débuts de la phrase dans le langage de l’enfant, Journal de Psychologie Normale et Pathologique, 24, 1–25.Google Scholar
Gunter, H., and Scherer, A. (1956), Grundfragen der Sprachwissenschaft. Heidelberg: Quelle & Meyer.Google Scholar
Jenkins, J. J. (1965), “Mediation theory and grammatical behavior,” in Rosenberg, S. (ed.), Directions in Psycholinguistics. New York: Macmillan, 66–96.Google Scholar
Johnson, F. S. (1967), Grammars: a working classification, Elementary English (Milwaukee), 44, 4, 349–52.Google Scholar
Kantor, J. R. (1922), An Objective Psychology of Grammar. Bloomington: Indiana Univ. Publications.Google Scholar
Lashley, K. S. (1951), “The problem of serial order in behavior,” in Jeffress, L. A. (ed.), Cerebral Mechanisms in Behavior. New York: Wiley, 112–36. Reprinted in Saporta, S. (ed.), Psycholinguistics. New York: Holt, 1961, 180-98.Google Scholar
Miller, G. A. (1965), “Some psychological studies of grammar,” in Anderson, R. C., and Ausubel, D. P., Readings in the Psychology of Cognition. New York: Holt, 312–49.Google Scholar
Wittwer, J. (1959), Les fonctions grammaticales chez l’enfant. Neuchâtel, Switzerland: Delachaux et Niestle.Google Scholar