Hostname: page-component-cd9895bd7-fscjk Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-12-18T18:45:46.827Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Ejective stops in Hittite: evidence for a phonemic length distinction

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  05 August 2022

Alwin Kloekhorst*
Affiliation:
Leiden University Centre for Linguistics, Leiden, The Netherlands
Rights & Permissions [Opens in a new window]

Abstract

In this article, it is argued that Hittite did not only possess a series of long ejective stops, as has previously been proposed, but that it also knew a series of short ejective stops. In this way, the Hittite stop system can be analysed as consisting of two types of stops, plain and ejective ones, with both types showing a length opposition: plain short /t/ vs. plain long /tː/, and ejective short /t’/ vs. ejective long /t’ː/.

Type
Article
Creative Commons
Creative Common License - CCCreative Common License - BY
This is an Open Access article, distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution licence (https://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/), which permits unrestricted re-use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided the original work is properly cited.
Copyright
Copyright © The Author(s), 2022. Published by Cambridge University Press on behalf of SOAS University of London

1. Introduction

In several previous publications,Footnote 1 I have argued that Hittite knows in its synchronic stop system a series of ejective stops (/t’ː/, etc.),Footnote 2 which etymologically go back to earlier clusters of stops + laryngeals.Footnote 3 These ejective stops form a third series next to the two series that are traditionally called “fortis” and “lenis”, and which can be phonologically interpreted as plain long stops (/tː/, etc.) and plain short stops (/t/, etc.), respectively.Footnote 4 My postulation of a series of ejectives is primarily based on the fact that in the spelling of dental stops before the vowel a, in some environments three different orthographic practices can be discerned. In these cases, two of the three spelling patterns can be analysed as denoting the fortis and lenis stops, respectively, whereas the third orthographic pattern, which always involves spellings with the sign DA, correlate with the etymological presence of a cluster of dental stop + laryngeal, *TH. Since in the Old Babylonian version of the cuneiform script, which forms the source of the Hittite ductus, the sign DA can be used to designate the ejective stop [t’], I have proposed that in Hittite, too, these spellings with DA indicate the presence of ejective stops.Footnote 5

My proposal concerns the following three environments. First, in word-initial position, the dental ejective is marked by virtually consistent spelling with the sign DA (e.g. da-an-zi “they take” = [t'antsi] < *dh3enti). This spelling contrasts with consistent spelling with the sign TA, which marks the presence of [t] = fortis /tː/, and with alternating spelling of TA and DA, which marks the presence of [d] = lenis /t/.Footnote 6 Second, in intervocalic position, the dental ejective is predominantly indicated by geminate spelling with the sign DA (e.g. ud-da-a-ar “words” = [ut’ːaːr] < *uth2ōr).Footnote 7 This contrasts with consistent geminate spelling with the sign TA, which marks the presence of [tː] = fortis /tː/, and alternating single spelling with TA and DA, which marks the presence of [d] = lenis /t/.Footnote 8 And, finally, in post-consonantal position, the dental ejective is indicated by virtually consistent spelling with the sign DA (e.g. an-da “into” = [ənt'a]Footnote 9 < *h1ndhh2e). This contrasts with consistent spelling with the sign TA, which marks the presence of [t] = fortis /tː/, and alternating spelling of TA and DA, which marks the presence of [d] = lenis /t/.Footnote 10

Some colleagues have indicated to find my analysis of these spellings difficult to accept.Footnote 11 For instance, Kim (Reference Kim, Guzzo and Taracha2019: 2987) states that he is “not convinced” of my postulation of a three-way contrast in the Hittite stop system, but does not specify his problems with my analysis, and does not treat the data on which this analysis is based. In the same vein, Patri (Reference Patri2019: 1005) formulates some problems with my 2010 paper,Footnote 12 but does not mention my 2013 paper. Lastly, Melchert (Reference Melchert, Kim, Mynářová and Pavúk2020) does not specifically mention my postulation of ejective stops in Hittite, but he does list my 2010 and 2013 articles as examples of phonological studies that are based on “the widespread pernicious false premise that all non-random orthographic patterns must at all costs reflect linguistically real contrasts” (emphasis his), whereas to his mind such patterns may rather be due to “established norms, aesthetic considerations, and pure convention” (Melchert Reference Melchert, Kim, Mynářová and Pavúk2020: 259). I fundamentally disagree with this latter view: it is the task of historical linguists to explain the rationale behind specific spelling peculiarities. Especially when synchronic, statistically significant orthographic patterns correlate with a specific etymological phonological sequence (in this case, for instance, the fact that synchronic spellings of the type (-)Vd-da(-) correlate with Proto-Indo-European (PIE) clusters of the type *-TH-, whereas consistent spelling of the type (-)Vt-ta(-) corresponds to PIE *-t-), and one can make likely that this orthography could represent a synchronic phonation that would fit its etymological origins (in this case, in Old Babylonian the spelling (-)Vd-da(-) is used to write the ejective stop [t’ː], whereas (-)Vt-ta(-) in principle denotes plain [tː]), Occam's Razor demands that we should postulate this phonation for the synchronic stage of the language in question (in this case that Hitt. (-)Vd-da(-) represents [t’ː], which contrasts with (-)Vt-ta(-) = [tː]).Footnote 13 Assuming that such patterns are based on “established norms” or “convention”, as Melchert is implying, without explaining why these norms arose, is nothing more than saying that one has not been able to find a linguistic rationale, and therefore does not constitute an explanation at all.

As long as no alternative explanation is offered to explain the correlation between synchronic DA-spellings and etymological clusters of dental stops + laryngeals, I see no reason to abandon my postulation of ejective stops in Hittite.

2. The problem: a single long ejective series

The geminate spelling (-)Vd-da(-) that is used to denote the intervocalic ejective stops in e.g. uddār “words”, padda-i “to dig”, etc., indicates that they were long consonants. This is not only the case when they etymologically go back to a PIE voiceless stop + laryngeal (e.g. uth2ōr > uddār [ut’ːaːr] “words”), but also when they reflect a cluster of a PIE voiced (aspirated) stop + laryngeal (e.g. *bhodhh2-V° > padda-i “to dig” [pat’ːa-]). I have therefore argued that in the latter case, the original lenis (short) outcome of PIE *d(h) underwent fortition (lengthening) before the laryngeal, which by that time had developed into a glottal stop, after which the fusion of the cluster of fortis (long) stop + glottal stop yielded a long ejective stop: e.g. PIE *bhodhh2 > pre-Hitt. [potʔV-], which underwent fortition to *[potːʔV-], resulting into Hitt. [pat’ːV-], spelled pád-da- “to dig” (Kloekhorst Reference Kloekhorst2013: 130–1). In the case of word-initial and post-consonantal ejective stops, there is no indication in spelling that these consonants were long, however, and it is therefore best to assume that they were phonetically short, [t’]. In Kloekhorst Reference Kloekhorst, Kim, Mynářová and Pavúk2020: 173, I argued that intervocalic long [t’ː] and word-initial and post-consonantal short [t’] may phonologically be regarded as allophones of a single ejective phoneme, for which I assumed the basic shape /t’ː/.Footnote 14 This allophony would thus be similar to the one found in the case of the plain fortis stop /tː/, which is realized as a long stop [tː] in intervocalic position, but as short [t] in word-initial and post-consonantal position. I did note a problematic aspect of this analysis, however, namely that “[o]ne could argue […] that in this way [the phoneme /t’ː/] is redundantly marked vis-à-vis the fortis and the lenis stops (/tː/ and /t/, respectively)” and that, when it comes to segmental features, it would be more economic to interpret this phoneme as an underlying short ejective stop /t’/. However, “since in intervocalic position the consonantal length is relevant for whether the preceding vowel stands in an open or closed syllable, I rather keep the long character of the ejective stop expressed in my phonemic representation of it” (Kloekhorst Reference Kloekhorst, Kim, Mynářová and Pavúk2020: 173).

Nevertheless, I kept feeling uneasy about this situation: long ejective stops are cross-linguistically rare, and, as far as I am aware, only occur in phoneme inventories in which they contrast with a series of short ejective stops.Footnote 15 In the following paragraphs I will present a solution to this problem: I will argue that Hittite also knew a series of short ejective stops.

3. Intervocalic short ejective stops?

The Hittite verbs pēda-i / pēd- “to bring (away)” and uda-i / ud- “to bring (here)” are transparent univerbations of the verb dā-i / d- “to take” with the preverbs pē- “thither” and u- “hither”, respectively. Both verbs contain an intervocalic single spelled dental stop, and in the case of uda-i, the spelling of this stop is remarkable.

Normally, in Old Script (OS) texts, intervocalic single spelled dental stops always show interchange between spellings with DA and with TA (e.g. a-da-an-zi / a-ta-an-zi “they eat”), which, as I have argued, denotes the presence of a voiced stop [d], the intervocalic allophone of lenis /t/ < PIE *d(h).Footnote 16 However, as noted in Kloekhorst Reference Kloekhorst2013: 13960, in the case of uda-i we find in OS texts consistent spelling with the sign DA (31x ú-da-), but no spelling with the sign TA (never **ú-ta-). In this way, this verb deviates in spelling from the words that contain an intervocalic [d] = /t/. However, since the corresponding verb pēda-i does show in OS texts an interchange between spellings with DA (30x pé(-e)-da(-)) and with TA (14x pé(-e)-ta(-)), which does more or less match the spelling practices of intervocalic [d], I decided to brush aside the consistent spelling of uda-i as ú-da-, and assumed for both verbs the presence of an intervocalic [d]. I did remark, however, that “I do not want to exclude the possibility […] that in these words the use of the sign DA in the sequence °V-da(-) represents the presence of a short glottalized stop, [-Vtʔa-], which then must have been taken over from the base verb dā-i/d- “to take”, which had the shape [tʔ(ā)-]” (Kloekhorst Reference Kloekhorst2013: 13960).

In the meantime I have come to the conclusion that this latter interpretation is the more likely one. This is based on the fact that it is very difficult to envisage a historical scenario that would account for the presence of a lenis stop /t/ = [d] in pēda-i and uda-i.

4. The prehistory of pēda-i and uda-i

In the older literature, it is assumed that the initial consonant of dā-i / d-, which reflects PIE *d- (from the root *deh3-), for a long time during the prehistory of Hittite had the value of a lenis stop. Only in recent pre-Hittite times (after the assibilation of *ti̯- > [ts-] and *d(h)i̯- > [s-] had taken place), word-initial fortis and lenis dental stops merged into a single, short voiceless stop [t-], which phonologically can be viewed as a fortis consonant. Within this framework, it was easy to account for a lenis /t/ in pēda-i and uda-i: one would just have to assume that the univerbation of pē- and u- + dā-i took place when its initial stop was still lenis.Footnote 17

However, with the recognition that, because of its consistent spelling with the sign DA (e.g. 1sg.pres. da-a-aḫ-ḫi, 3sg.pres. da-a-i, 3pl.pres. da-an-zi, etc.), the verb dā-i / d- must have had an initial ejective stop, [t’ā-, t’-], this scenario can no longer be upheld.

4.1. The prehistory of dā-i / d-

As mentioned above, the Hittite ejective stops are the result of a fusion of original clusters of stop + laryngeal, in this case *dh3-. It should be noted that in the verb *deh3- / *dh3- this cluster was originally only present in the weak stem *dh3-, which implies that at some moment in time a paradigmatic levelling has taken place. As Norbruis (Reference Norbruis2021: 423, fn. 2) argues on the basis of Luwian evidence,Footnote 18 both the fusion of *dh3- to a monophonemic stop and its levelling throughout the paradigm must have taken place in pre-Proto-Anatolian times. Moreover, there is another relevant pre-Proto-Anatolian development that needs to be taken into account, namely the contact-induced fortition of lenis stops when standing before laryngeals, which in intervocalic position is attested both in Hittite (e.g. mekki- “much, many” < *meǵh2-i-) and in Luwian (CLuw. 2pl.midd. -dduu̯ar < PIE *-dhh2uo°). Since this development must have taken place when the laryngeal was still an independent phoneme, it follows that it must have preceded the fusion of such clusters into ejective consonants. Although it cannot be excluded that this fortition only took place in word-internal, postvocalic position, it is possible that it also affected word-initial clusters.Footnote 19 All in all, we may envisage two possible pathways of developments of PIE *deh3- / *dh3- in pre-Proto-Anatolian (see Table 1): pathway I (without fortition of *[tʔ-] > *[tːʔ-]), which would yield PAnat. *[t’ō-, t’-]; and pathway II (with fortition of *[tʔ-] > *[tːʔ-]), which would yield PAnat. *[t’ːō-, t’ː-].

Table 1 The two possible pathways of developments of PIE *deh3- / *dh3- in pre-Proto-Anatolian.

As was mentioned earlier, in the prehistory of Hittite the plain dental stops, fortis *[tː-] and lenis *[t-], merged in word-initial position into a single short voiceless [t-]. Although this [t-] phonologically should be regarded as a fortis stop (Kloekhorst Reference Kloekhorst, Kim, Mynářová and Pavúk2020: 166), phonetically the merger is caused by the shortening of fortis *[tː-] to [t-]. We may thus assume that such a shortening would have affected PAnat. word-initial *[t’ː-] as well, yielding [t’-]. So in the case of pathway II, in which the PAnat. shape of this verb is *[t’ːō-, t’ː-], with long ejectives, we may now add another, specifically pre-Hittite development: *[t’ːō-, t’ː-] > *[t’ō-, t’-] (shortening of *[t’ː-] to [t’-]). Together with the specifically pre-Hittite colouring of PAnat. */ō/ to Hitt. /ā/, we end up with Hitt. [t’ā-, t’-]. In the case of pathway I, in which the PAnat. shape of this verb is *[t’ō-, t’-], no shortening needs to be assumed, only the colouring of the vowel of the strong stem, after which the result is Hitt. [t’ā-, t’-]. In this way, the outcome of both pathways is the same, Hitt. [t’ā-, t’-], which is spelled dā-i / d-.

4.2. The creation of pēda-i and uda-i

As said above, the verbs pēda-i / pēd- and uda-i / ud- are the result of a univerbation of the verb dā-i / d- with the preverbs pē- and u-, which phonetically were [pḗ] and [ʔū́], respectively.Footnote 20 The exact moment of univerbation is not fully clear, but there are no indications that point to a Proto-Anatolian origin of these verbs.Footnote 21 It is therefore best to assume that they are specifically Hittite formations. Within pathway I, this means that at the moment of univerbation their base verb had the shape *[t’ā-, t’-]. Within pathway II, there are two stages: before the shortening of word-initial *[t’:-] to [t’-], which means that at that moment in time the base verb had the shape *[t’ːō-. t’ː-]; or after the shortening, which is equal to pathway I.

Within pathway II, if the univerbation took place in its initial stage, i.e. before the shortening of *[t’:-] to [t’-], we would expect the outcomes of the univerbations to have been [pḗt’ːa-, pḗt’ː-] and [ʔū́t’ːa-, ʔū́t’ː-], respectively.Footnote 22 According to the spelling rules of Hittite, these verbs would then have been spelled with geminate spelling: **pé-e-ed-da- and **ú-ud-da-, respectively. Since these spellings do not occur, we can safely rule out this scenario.Footnote 23

If these verbs were formed with the base verb [t’ā-, t’-] (within pathway I, and during the second stage of pathway II), the expected outcomes would be [pḗt'a-, pḗt’-] and [ʔū́t'a-, ʔū́t’-], respectively. We would expect that the short ejective consonant in these forms would be spelled with single spelling, °V-Ca(-). In the case of the long ejective stop /t’ː/, we have seen that it is predominantly spelled with geminate spelling with the sign DA, (-)Vd-da(-), although spellings with the sign TA, (-)Vt-ta(-), do occasionally occur as well.Footnote 24 This would predict that a short ejective stop would be predominantly spelled with the sign DA as well, °V-da(-), next to some spellings with the sign TA, °V-ta(-). These predictions are a perfect match with the way pēda-i and uda-i are spelled. Both show single spelling of their dental stop, and both show predominant spellings with the sign DA. In the case of pēda-i, CHD (P: 345–6) lists 171 attestations with the sign DA, vs. 30 with the sign TA (a ratio of 85.1% DA vs. 14.9% TA). In the case of uda-i, I have found in my files over 470 attestations with the sign DA, vs. only oneFootnote 25 with the sign TA (a ratio of 99.8% DA vs. 0.2% TA). These ratios correspond almost exactly to the ratios between DA and TA spellings that are found in the lexemes that contain a long ejective stop [t’ː] (which ranged from 100% DA vs. 0% TA (padda-i “to dig”) to 94.6% DA vs. 5.4% TA (uddar / uddan- “word, thing”), 91.4% DA vs. 8.6% TA (paddar / paddan “basket”), 89.3% DA vs. 10.7% TA (apadda(n) “there, thither”), and 86.1% DA vs. 13.9% TA (piddae-zi “to flee”), cf. Kloekhorst Reference Kloekhorst, Kim, Mynářová and Pavúk2020: 150–3).

For the sake of argument, we may also discuss the possibility that the univerbations of pē- and u- with the base verb dā-i / d- took place in pre-Proto-Anatolian times, that is, before the spread of the initial consonant of the weak stem *[t’-] (in pathway I) or *[t’ː-] (in pathway II) throughout the paradigm. The phonologically regular outcome of the strong stem, [pḗ] / [ʔū́] + *[tō-], would then be [pḗda-] and [ʔū́da-], respectively, with a lenis dental [d]. In the weak stem, however, we would expect an outcome [pḗt’-] and [ʔū́t’-], with a short ejective stop [t’] (according to pathway I), or [pḗt’ː-] and [ʔū́t’ː-], with a long ejective stop [t’ː]Footnote 26 (according to pathway II). The result would be paradigms with consonantal alternation between strong and weak stem: [pḗda-, pḗt’(ː)-] and [ʔū́da-, ʔū́t’(ː)-]. In other verbs for which we can reconstruct a similar consonantal alternation between a strong and a weak stem, it is always the weak stem that is generalized. For instance, PIE *ti-ne-h1-ti / *ti-nh1-énti should regularly have yielded Hitt. **zinizzi / zinnanzi ([tsini-, tsinː-]), with lenis -n- in the strong stem and fortis -nn- in the weak stem, but in the prehistory of Hittite this has been levelled out to zinnizzi / zinnanzi, with generalization of the fortis -nn- of the weak stem. On the basis of this and many other examples, we would have to assume that the original paradigms **[pḗda-, pḗt’(ː)-] and **[ʔū́da-, ʔū́t’(ː)-] would have been levelled out either to [pḗt'a-, pḗt’-] and [ʔū́t'a-, ʔū́t’-] (according to pathway I), or to [pḗt’ːa-, pḗt’ː-] and [ʔū́t’ːa-, ʔū́t’ː-] (according to pathway II), in both cases with the ejective stop of the weak stem being generalized throughout the paradigm. As we have seen above, the outcomes with a long ejective stop (according to pathway II), should in Hittite have been spelled **pé-e-ed-da- and **ú-ud-da-, respectively, which is not what we find. The outcomes with a lenis ejective stop (according to pathway I) would formally be identical to the outcomes of the pre-Hittite univerbations of pathway I (and the second stage of pathway II), for which the spellings pé-e-da- and ú-da- are a perfect match.

4.3. The synchronic interpretation of pēda-i and uda-i

All in all, we can conclude that the only way to combine the fact that pēda-i and uda-i show single spelling of their dental stop with the recognition that their base verb, dā-i / d- contained an ejective stop, is by assuming that the dental stop of pēda-i and uda-i was a short ejective stop [t’]: [pḗt'a-] and [ʔū́t'a-]. This is the only possible outcome of these univerbations, whether they were created in pre-Hittite times (pathway I or during the second stage of pathway II) or in pre-Proto-Anatolian times (pathway I). There simply is no scenario by which the dental stop of pēda-i and uda-i could be a plain lenis dental stop [d] = /t/, and I therefore regard the presence of a short ejective stop [t’] in these verbs as certain.

5. A revision of the Hittite stop system: two series of ejective stops

Since the intervocalic ejective [t’] of pēda-i and uda-i is in spelling consistently distinguished from its long counterpart [t’ː] (e.g. ud-da-a-ar [ut’ːaːr] “words”, pád-da- [pat’ːa-] “to dig”), we must assume that they were two different phonemes, /t’/ and /t’ː/, respectively. As a consequence, we should enlarge the Hittite phoneme inventory – at least for the dental place of articulation – with a series of short ejective stops, which contrast with their long counterparts.Footnote 27

A major advantage of this analysis, and in fact an extra argument in favour of it, is that in this way we solve the problem that was formulated in section 2: the length of the intervocalic ejective stops of words like uddār, padda-i, etc., which originally seemed phonologically redundant, can now be seen as a distinctive feature that contrasts with the absence of length in the newly discovered ejective stops of pēda-i and uda-i.

The distinction between the dental short and long ejective stops seems to have been made in intervocalic position only: pēda-i “to bring away” = /pḗt'a-/, uda-i “to bring here” = /ʔū́t'a-/ vs. uddār “words” = /ut’ːā́r/, padda-i “to dig” = /pat’ːa-/, etc. As far as I am aware, in word-initial position and post-consonantal position, no contrast between long and short ejective stops can be discerned, and given the fact that in these positions the ejective stops phonetically are probably short, it is best to phonemically interpret them as short as well: dā-i / d- “to take” = /t’ā-, t’-/, dai-i / ti- “to put” = /t'ai-, t'i-/, daššu- “dense” = /t'asːu-/; anda “into” = /ənt'a/, andan “inside” = /ənt'an/.Footnote 28

As argued in Kloekhorst Reference Kloekhorst2010: 216–17, also for the velar place of articulation there is direct evidence for ejective stops, namely in the verb kinu-zi, ginu-zi “to open up” = [k'inu-]Footnote 29 < PIE *ǵhh2i-neu-. Etymologically, we would expect the presence of intervocalic long ejective velar stops as well, for instance in mekki- “much, many”, which reflects PIE *meǵh2-i- and therefore synchronically probably was [mek’ːi-].Footnote 30 Thus far, however, no specific spelling practice has been identified with which these sounds can be distinguished from plain fortis stops, so their existence must, for the time being, remain hypothetical. Nevertheless, in analogy to the situation in the dental series, I regard it likely that also in the velar series the word-initial short ejective [k’-] can now be interpreted as a separate phoneme vis-à-vis the long ejective [k’ː] that probably was present in words like mekki-.

Unfortunately, for the labial and labiovelar place of articulation we have at the moment no secure evidence for any ejective stops, so here it is best to remain agnostic.

All in all, an updated overview of the Hittite stop system should look as follows:

I also present here an updated version of the table of the phonetic realizations of the dental stop phonemes in different environments as presented in Kloekhorst Reference Kloekhorst, Kim, Mynářová and Pavúk2020: 172 (originally given with only three phonemes, but here with four, and with different ordering; moreover, I have added the environment -TS-, based on the outcome of Kloekhorst Reference Kloekhorst2019).

Acknowledgements

I would like to thank Sasha Lubotsky as well as the anonymous reviewer from BSOAS for useful comments on an earlier version of this article.

Footnotes

2 In these articles I noted down this consonant as /tːʔ/, but in this article I will refer to it as /t’ː/, which is in line with other phonological literature on long (geminate) ejectives.

3 At the relevant moment, these laryngeals had developed into a glottal stop /ʔ/. Cross-linguistically, the rise of ejectives through the fusion of clusters of a stop + glottal stop is commonplace, cf. Fallon Reference Fallon1998: 410–45. An anonymous reviewer remarks that clusters of stops + laryngeals could in principle also have yielded other types of stops, and (s)he therefore asks: “How can we prove that the Hittite ejective stops were indeed ejective and not, for instance, aspirated or pharyngealized […]?” Here, the following two language universals are relevant: (1) “if [in a given language] there are aspirated stops, then there is /h/” (Hyman Reference Hyman2008: 114, with reference to Hagège Reference Hagège, Hattori and Inoue1982: 936); and (2) “pharyngealization is only noted in languages in which primary pharyngeal consonants occur” (Maddieson Reference Maddieson2009). Since Hittite knows neither /h/ nor pharyngeal consonants (note that the phonemes spelled -ḫḫ- and -ḫ- are now commonly regarded as uvular fricatives, cf. Kümmel Reference Kümmel2007: 331; Simon Reference Simon and Taracha2014; Weiss Reference Weiss, Byrd, DeLisi and Wenthe2016; Kloekhorst Reference Kloekhorst2018), we can rule out the possibility that the stops that I interpret as ejectives were aspirated or pharyngealized.

4 For the postulation of a synchronic phonological length difference between the fortis and lenis series, see Melchert Reference Melchert1994: 14–21 (in nuce); Kloekhorst Reference Kloekhorst2008: 21–5; 2014: 546–7; Reference Kloekhorst2016: 213–17; Yates Reference Yates2019. This interpretation challenges the traditional view that the phonological distinction between the fortis and lenis series was voice (/t/ vs. /d/, etc.), a concept that in many recent treatments of Hittite is still mentioned as the default interpretation: e.g. Luraghi Reference Luraghi1997: 3–4; Kimball Reference Kimball1999: 54; Watkins Reference Watkins and Woodard2004: 556; Vanséveren Reference Vanséveren2006: 39–40; Weiss Reference Weiss2009: 90; van den Hout Reference van den Hout2011: 64; Francia and Pisaniello Reference Francia and Pisaniello2019: 19; but note the remarks by Hoffner and Melchert Reference Hoffner and Melchert2008: 35 (“For the sake of simplicity we here describe the contrast in stops as one of voicing, but we do not mean thereby to take a definitive stance on this issue”) and Rieken Reference Rieken2011: 39 (“Es ist aber nicht klar, ob es sich bei der genannten phonemischen Distinktion tatsächlich auch phonetisch um einen Kontrast zwischen stimmhaft und stimmlos handelt […]. Der Konvention entsprechend ist im Folgenden stets von stimmhaften und stimmlosen Plosiven die Rede”). Recently, Simon (Reference Simon, Serangeli and Olander2020) has argued specifically in favour of the traditional idea that the distinction between the fortis and lenis series was voice but does so on false grounds (cf. my discussion of Simon Reference Simon, Serangeli and Olander2020 in Kloekhorst Reference Kloekhorst2021). According to Patri (Reference Patri2019: 275; see also Patri Reference Patri2009), the fortis series was aspirated (/th/ etc.), and the lenis series voiced (/d/, etc.), which is an interpretation that does not match the language universal cited in the previous footnote (cf. also Kloekhorst Reference Kloekhorst2021: 343–9).

5 My postulation of ejective stops is not only based on spelling patterns concerning the signs TA and DA. In Kloekhorst Reference Kloekhorst2010: 216–17, I argued that we find a similar case for the signs KI and GI in word-initial position: alternating spelling of KI and GI denote the presence of an ejective stop [k’] (kinu-zi, ginu-zi “to open up” = [k'inu-] < PIE *ǵhh2i-neu-), which contrasts with consistent spelling with the sign KI, which marks the presence of [k] = fortis /kː/, and with consistent spelling with GI, which marks the presence of [g] = lenis /k/.

6 See Kloekhorst Reference Kloekhorst2010: 202–7; 2020: 165–8 for details. Note that next to the two examples of word-initial ejective stops discussed there (dā-i / d- “to take” and dai-i / ti- “to put”), Lubotsky (Reference Lubotsky, Kloekhorst and Pronk2019: 153–4) has in the meantime found a third example: Hitt. daššu- “strong; heavy; difficult”. As Lubotsky cogently argues, this word, which is virtually consistently spelled with the sign DA (cf. Kloekhorst Reference Kloekhorst2008: 853–4), can be etymologized as *dh1ens-u-, with an initial cluster *TH-. Both facts would perfectly fit a synchronic interpretation with an initial ejective stop: [t'asːu-].

7 Note that in this word geminate spelling with TA occasionally occurs as well, ut-ta-a-ar. However, since the relative numbers of (-)Vt-ta(-) spellings of the relevant words is so low (ranging from 14% TA vs. 86% DA to 0% TA vs. 100% DA, cf. Kloekhorst Reference Kloekhorst, Kim, Mynářová and Pavúk2020: 150–1), I will from now on cite them with their (-)Vd-da(-) spelling only.

8 See Kloekhorst Reference Kloekhorst2013: 127–31; 2020: 148–55 for details.

9 For the postulation of an initial /ə/ in words spelled aC-, cf. Kloekhorst Reference Kloekhorst2014: 337–41.

10 See Kloekhorst Reference Kloekhorst2013: 131–9; 2020: 155–65 for details.

11 Note that in the recent papers of Yates (Reference Yates2019) and Simon (Reference Simon, Serangeli and Olander2020), who both offer critical discussions of some aspects of my analysis of the Hittite stop system, my postulation of ejective stops is not commented on.

12 Patri's main problem with my 2010 paper seems to be the analogy I assume for the verb dā-i “to take” (for which see section 4.1 below), and the fact that the Old Hittite 3pl.pres. form ta-an-zi “they take” (KBo 17.36+ i 7 (OS)) is spelled with TA, whereas it reflects *dh3enti and thus, according to my theory, should have yielded /t'antsi/, spelled da-an-zi, with DA. He does not discuss the fact, however, that this is the only Old Hittite attestation of this word spelled with TA, and that in all 26 other Old Hittite attestations this word is indeed spelled da-an-zi, with DA. Moreover, the same situation is found in MS and NS texts, where according to my files more than 300 attestations of da-an-zi, with DA, can be found vs. only two attestations ta-an-zi, with TA (KUB 15.34 iv 42 (MH/MS), KUB 41.28 ii 11 (NS)). The forms spelled ta-an-zi should therefore not be used as an argument.

13 See e.g. Rieken Reference Rieken, Archi and Francia2008 and Reference Rieken, Cohen, Gilan and Miller2010 for other illuminating and successful applications of this method.

14 In the original publication: /tːʔ/.

15 See also fn. 27.

16 Kloekhorst Reference Kloekhorst2013: 139–40.

17 In theory, a later univerbation may also be possible, if one would assume that the long accented vowels of the preverbs pē- and u- (which are /pḗ/ < *h1pói and /ʔū́/ < *h2óu, respectively, cf. Kloekhorst Reference Kloekhorst2008: 660–1, 909–10; 2014: 505) at that moment in time were still able to lenite a following fortis consonant. This depends, however, on the question of how long in the prehistory of Hittite the Proto-Anatolian lenition rules were still productive, which is not easy to answer.

18 His argument starts with the observation that the Luwian verb lā-i / l- “to take”, which is generally seen as the direct cognate to Hitt. dā-i / d-, has an initial l- that thus far is unexplained from PIE *deh3- / *dh3-. After noting that “Hittite evidence points to a generalization of a monophonemic outcome of *dH- throughout the paradigm”, Norbruis (Reference Norbruis2021: 243, fn. 2) states that “the rather unexpected Luwian outcome l- may be explained by the same development, which suggests that it had already happened by Proto-Anatolian”.

19 An argument against this idea may be the following. If in all positions in the word lenis stops before a laryngeal would undergo fortition, it follows that when the fusion of such clusters took place, the result would be that all ejectives stops were long. There would thus be only a single series of ejective stops, and we may assume that, with its length being non-contrastive, this would soon be given up, yielding a single short ejective series /t’/, etc. It may therefore be best to assume that in word-initial position no lengthening of stops before laryngeals took place, and that, after the fusion of stops + laryngeals into ejective stops, in word-initial position a phonemic distinction between long ejective stops, /t’ː/, etc., and short ejective stops, /t’/, etc., existed, which later was extended to the word-internal position as well. When, later on, the distinction between word-initial plain fortis and lenis stops was lost, we may then assume that also the long and short ejective stops merged into a single series, which was short. Note that in the sections to follow, which discuss the possible prehistories of pēda-i and uda-i, there is nothing that would speak against such a scenario.

20 See fn. 17.

21 The idea that Hitt. pēda-i “to bring” has a cognate in CLuw. “padda-”, allegedly “to carry” (thus Melchert Reference Melchert1994: 34), has to be given up, cf. Kloekhorst Reference Kloekhorst2014: 575–7.

22 One could theoretically argue that the long accented vowels of the preverbs may have caused a lenition of the following consonant. However, we know that in other forms, original clusters of consonants + laryngeals have not been subject to lenition, e.g. šākki < *sókh1ei. It should be noted that in the discussion of this latter verb (Kloekhorst Reference Kloekhorst2014: 555–6) I explained the lack of lenition by the assumption that the cluster *-kH- was still present as such in Proto-Anatolian times because “the assimilation of laryngeals to preceding stops was a post-Proto-Anatolian, specific Hittite development”. Since we have now seen that the fusion of stop + laryngeal was in fact a pre-Proto-Anatolian process, this idea needs to be adapted. In order to explain the absence of lenition in e.g. šākki < *sókh1ei, we would now have to assume that fortis ejective stops were not subject to lenition.

23 Another theoretically possible way to explain the absence of geminate spelling in these verbs is to assume that it is caused by a wish to retain the original spelling of the forms of the base verb (e.g. 3pl.pres. da-an-zi) in the univerbated verbs (hence pé-e-da-an-zi and ú-da-an-zi). However, in other univerbated verbs with pē- and u- geminate spellings are tolerated: compare e.g. pé-en-na-i “he drives (there)” and u-un-na-i “he drives / sends (here)”, with geminate spelling -nn-, next to the base verb nāi “he turns, sends”.

24 See fn. 7.

25 2pl.imp.act. ú-ta-a[t-tén] (KUB 15.34 ii 7 (MH/MS)). It is interesting that this tablet also contains one of the attestations of 3pl.pres. ta-an-zi “they take” with TA, cf. fn. 12. This strengthens the idea that both attestations are exceptions that do not reflect normal spelling practices.

26 Clusters of stops + laryngeals and their outcomes are not subject to lenition, cf. fn. 22.

27 Although the number of languages that have a phonemic opposition between short and long ejective stops is small, they certainly exist: PHOIBLE 2.0 lists the following 16 languages (out of a total of 2,186): the Cushitic languages Alaba-K'abeena, Arbore, Eastern Oromo, Hadiyya, Kambaata and Tsamai (all from Ethiopia), the Omotic languages Anfillo, Dime, Koorete and Wolayta (all from Ethiopia), the Semitic languages Amharic and Silt'e (from Ethiopia) and Tigre (from Eritrea), the North-East Caucasian languages Andi and Hunzib (both from Dagestan), and the language isolate Zuni (from New Mexico, USA). To these can be added the North-East Caucasian languages Avar (/k/ vs. /kː/ vs. /k’/ vs. /k’ː/), spoken in Dagestan (e.g. Forker Reference Forker and Polinskyforthcoming), and Tsova-Tush (Batsbi), spoken in Georgia (cf. Hauk and Hakim Reference Hauk, Hakim, Calhoun, Esudero, Tabain and Warren2019). Another relevant language that knew this opposition is Akkadian: its so-called “emphatic” stops, which in fact were ejectives (cf. Kouwenberg Reference Kouwenberg2003: 81–2), know a phonemic distinction between short and long (geminate) variants, a distinction that is found for all consonants of Akkadian.

28 Cf. fn. 9 for the postulation of /ə/ in the latter two words.

29 In the original publication the initial consonant was noted down as “[gʔ-]”.

30 The fact that PIE *ǵ underwent fortition indicates that the laryngeal was still present at the relevant moment, which enhances the chance that it, too, was reinterpreted as a glottalic element of the preceding stop.

31 The postulation of a glottal stop is controversial, but to my mind justified: see the discussion in Kloekhorst Reference Kloekhorstforthcoming.

References

CHD = Güterbock, Hans G., Hoffner, Harry A. and van den Hout, Theo P.J. (eds). 1983–. The Hittite Dictionary of the Oriental Institute of the University of Chicago. Chicago: The Oriental Institute.Google Scholar
PHOIBLE 2.0 = Moran, Steven and McCloy, Daniel (eds). 2019. PHOIBLE 2.0. Jena: Max Planck Institute for the Science of Human History. http://phoible.org (accessed 14 January 2020).Google Scholar
Fallon, Paul D. 1998. The Synchronic and Diachronic Phonology of Ejectives. PhD diss., Ohio State University.Google Scholar
Forker, Diana. Forthcoming. “Avar”, to appear in Polinsky, Maria (ed.), The Oxford Handbook of Languages of the Caucasus. Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Francia, Rita and Pisaniello, Valerio. 2019. La Lingua degli Ittiti. Milano: Hoepli.Google Scholar
Hagège, Claude. 1982. “Linguistic universals as general tendencies”, in Hattori, Shirô and Inoue, Kazuko (eds), Proceedings of the XIIIth International Congress of Linguists, August 29th–September 4th 1982, Tokyo, 936–40. Tokyo: Tokyo Press.Google Scholar
Hauk, Bryn and Hakim, Jakob. 2019. “Acoustic properties of singleton and geminate ejectives in Tsova-Tush”, in Calhoun, Sasha, Esudero, Paola, Tabain, Marija and Warren, Paul (eds), Proceedings of the 19th International Congress of Phonetic Sciences, Melbourne, Australia 2019, 3483–7. Canberra, Australia: Australasian Speech Science and Technology Association Inc.Google Scholar
Hoffner, Harry A. and Melchert, H. Craig. 2008. A Grammar of the Hittite Language. 2 vols. Winona Lake: Eisenbrauns.Google Scholar
van den Hout, Theo P.J. 2011. The Elements of Hittite. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hyman, Larry M. 2008. “Universals in phonology”, The Linguistic Review 25, 81135.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kim, Ronald I. 2019. “The 2pl. middle ending in Proto-Indo-European”, in Guzzo, Natalia Bolatti and Taracha, Piotr (eds), “And I Knew Twelve Languages”: A Tribute to Massimo Poetto on the Occasion of His 70th Birthday, 295314. Warsaw: Agade.Google Scholar
Kimball, Sara E. 1999. Hittite Historical Phonology. Innsbruck: Institut für Sprachwissenschaft der Universität Innsbruck.Google Scholar
Kloekhorst, Alwin. 2008. Etymological Dictionary of the Hittite Inherited Lexicon. Leiden Indo-European Etymological Dictionary Series, vol. 5. Leiden: Brill.Google Scholar
Kloekhorst, Alwin. 2010. “Initial stops in Hittite (with an excursus on the spelling of stops in Alalaḫ Akkadian)”, Zeitschrift für Assyriologie 100, 197241.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kloekhorst, Alwin. 2013. “The signs TA and DA in Old Hittite: evidence for a phonetic difference”, Altorientalische Forschungen 40, 125–41.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kloekhorst, Alwin. 2014. Accent in Hittite: A Study in Plene Spelling, Consonant Gradation, Clitics, and Metrics. Studien zu den Boğazköy-Texten, vol. 56. Wiesbaden: Harrassowitz.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kloekhorst, Alwin. 2016. “The Anatolian stop system and the Indo-Hittite hypothesis”, Indogermanische Forschungen 121, 213–47.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kloekhorst, Alwin. 2018. “Anatolian evidence suggests that the Indo-European laryngeals *h 2 and *h 3 were uvular stops”, Indo-European Linguistics 6, 6994.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kloekhorst, Alwin. 2019. “The spelling of clusters of dental stop + sibilant in Hittite”, Münchener Studien zur Sprachwissenschaft 73, 5572.Google Scholar
Kloekhorst, Alwin. 2020. “The phonetics and phonology of the Hittite dental stops”, in Kim, Ronald I., Mynářová, Jana and Pavúk, Peter (eds), Hrozný and Hittite: The First Hundred Years. Proceedings of the International Conference Held at Charles University, Prague, 11–14 November 2015. (Culture and History of the Ancient Near East, vol. 107), 147–75. Leiden: Brill.Google Scholar
Kloekhorst, Alwin. 2021. “The phonetics and phonology of Hittite intervocalic fortis and lenis stops”, Bibliotheca Orientalis 78, 327–52.Google Scholar
Kloekhorst, Alwin. Forthcoming. “Evidence for a phonemic glottal stop in Hittite: a reassessment”, (unpublished manuscript).Google Scholar
Kouwenberg, Norbert J.C. 2003. “Evidence for post-glottalized consonants in Assyrian”, Journal of Cuneiform Studies 55, 7586.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kümmel, Martin J. 2007. Konsonantenwandel. Wiesbaden: Reichert.Google Scholar
Lubotsky, Alexander. 2019. “The Indo-European suffix *-ens- and its Indo-Uralic origin”, in Kloekhorst, Alwin and Pronk, Tijmen, The Precursors of Proto-Indo-European: The Indo-Anatolian and Indo-Uralic hypotheses. (Leiden Studies in Indo-European, vol. 21), 151–62. Leiden: Brill/Rodopi.Google Scholar
Luraghi, Silvia. 1997. Hittite. Munich: Lincom Europa.Google Scholar
Maddieson, Ian. 2009. “Typology and occurrence of pharyngeals and pharyngealization around the world”. Abstract for a paper presented at the International Workshop on Pharyngeals and Pharyngealisation, March 26–7, Newcastle University. Available online at https://web.archive.org/web/20110617064432/http://www.ncl.ac.uk/linguistics/assets/documents/PharyngealsMaddieson.pdf.Google Scholar
Melchert, H. Craig. 1994. Anatolian Historical Phonology. (Leiden Studies in Indo-European, vol. 3.) Amsterdam: Rodopi.Google Scholar
Melchert, H. Craig. 2020. “Hittite historical phonology after 100 years (and after 20 years)”, in Kim, Ronald I., Mynářová, Jana and Pavúk, Peter (eds), Hrozný and Hittite: The First Hundred Years. Proceedings of the International Conference Held at Charles University, Prague, 11–14 November 2015. (Culture and History of the Ancient Near East, vol. 107), 258–76. Leiden: Brill.Google Scholar
Norbruis, Stefan. 2021. Indo-European Origins of Anatolian Morphology and Semantics. Innovations and Archaisms in Hittite, Luwian and Lycian. Amsterdam: LOT.Google Scholar
Patri, Sylvain. 2009. “L'adaptation des consonnes hittites dans certaines langues du XIIIe siècle”, Zeitschrift für Assyriologie 99, 87126.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Patri, Sylvain. 2019. Phonologie hittite. Leiden: Brill.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Rieken, Elisabeth. 2008. “Die Zeichen <ta>, <tá> und <tà> in den hieroglyphen-luwischen Inschriften der Nachgroßreichszeit”, in Archi, Alfonso and Francia, Rita (eds), VI Congresso Internazionale di Ittitologia, Roma, 5-9 settembre 2005, Parte II. Studi Micenei ed Egeo-Anatolici, vol. 50, 637–48.Google Scholar
Rieken, Elisabeth. 2010. “Das Zeichen <tá> im Hieroglyphen-Luwischen”, in Cohen, Yoram, Gilan, Amir and Miller, Jared L. (eds), Pax Hethitica. Studies on the Hittites and Their Neighbours in Honour of Itamar Singer. (Studien zu den Boğazköy-Texten, vol. 51), 301–10. Wiesbaden: Harrassowitz.Google Scholar
Rieken, Elisabeth. 2011. Einführung in die hethitische Sprache und Schrift. Münster: Ugarit Verlag.Google Scholar
Simon, Zsolt. 2014. “Der phonetische Wert der luwischen Laryngale”, in Taracha, Piotr (ed.), Proceedings of the Eighth International Congress of Hittitology. Warsaw, 5–9 September 2011, 873–95. Warsaw: Agade.Google Scholar
Simon, Zsolt. 2020. “The Anatolian stop system and the Indo-Hittite hypothesis – revisited”, in Serangeli, Matilde and Olander, Thomas (eds), Dispersals and Diversification. Linguistic and Archaeological Perspectives on the Early Stages of Indo-European, 236–50. Leiden: Brill.Google Scholar
Vanséveren, Sylvie. 2006. Nisili. Manuel de Langue Hittite. Vol. I. Leuven: Peeters.Google Scholar
Watkins, Calvert. 2004. “Hittite”, in Woodard, Roger D. (ed.), The Cambridge Encyclopaedia of the World's Ancient Languages, 551–75. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Weiss, Michael. 2009. Outline of the Historical and Comparative Grammar of Latin. Ann Arbor: Beech Stave.Google Scholar
Weiss, Michael. 2016. “The Proto-Indo-European laryngeals and the name of Cilicia in the Iron Age”, in Byrd, Andrew M., DeLisi, Jessica and Wenthe, Mark (eds), Tavet Tat Satyam. Studies in Honor of Jared S. Klein on the Occasion of His Seventieth Birthday, 331–40. Ann Arbor: BeechStave.Google Scholar
Yates, Anthony. 2019. “The phonology, phonetics, and diachrony of Sturtevant's Law”, Indo-European Linguistics 7, 241307.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Figure 0

Table 1 The two possible pathways of developments of PIE *deh3- / *dh3- in pre-Proto-Anatolian.