Hostname: page-component-586b7cd67f-l7hp2 Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-11-23T21:32:55.011Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Liquid calories, energy compensation and weight: what we know and what we still need to learn

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  28 October 2013

David B. Allison*
Affiliation:
Office of Energetics, Nutrition Obesity Research Center, University of Alabama at Birmingham, Birmingham, AL35294-0022, USA email [email protected]
Rights & Permissions [Opens in a new window]

Abstract

Type
Invited Commentary
Copyright
Copyright © The Author 2013 

Roughly 10 000 years ago, sugar was first domesticated in New Guinea. Roughly 8000 years ago, it was transplanted to India. Sometime around the seventh century, cultivation and some industrial production began in southern Europe, and the crusades subsequently acquainted more Europeans with sugar imported from Arab lands. Until the sixteenth century, sugar was often viewed by Europeans as having medicinal properties. Colonisation of the New World led to mass production and distribution of sugar as a major foodstuff( 1 Reference Galloway 5 ). By 1713, a writer in a scholarly journal was extolling the health virtues of high levels of sugar consumption, including in beverages( Reference Slare 6 ). In 1893, Harley( Reference Harley 7 ) conducted self-experiments and concluded that consumption of 250 g (approximately 4184 kJ or approximately 1000 kcal) of sugar greatly increased muscular work capacity. In 1899, a controlled trial involving soldiers reported that those given a ration of sugar were in better health, felt more vigorous and gained more weight (presumably judged to be a good thing at the time)( 8 ). As the century turned, Gardner( Reference Gardner 9 ) described sugar as a nutritional necessity that increased the health and vigour of populations. Yet, the positive health halo of sugar could not last. A generation later, authors of scientific papers did write about ‘The social problem growing out of the overconsumption of sugar’ and described school-based programmes to teach children to consume less sugar( Reference Sollins 10 ).

Sugar consumed in liquid form has come to be seen by some as especially deserving of scrutiny. In 1990, Tordoff & Alleva( Reference Tordoff and Alleva 11 ) published seminal trial results showing that persons required to consume additional sugar in the form of a beverage gained more weight than did a control group given a non-energetic beverage. After 13 years, suspicion was increasing that metabolisable energy, perhaps especially sugar, consumed as liquids promoted less satiety, less energy compensation and more weight gain than did the same energy consumed in solid form( Reference Almiron-Roig, Chen and Drewnowski 12 ). The topic has become controversial to say the least( Reference Slavin 13 ), and there is substantial evidence that the strength of the supporting data has often been exaggerated and distorted( Reference Cope and Allison 14 , Reference Kaiser, Shikany and Keating 15 ).

Newspaper articles offer statements such as ‘People who drink sugary soft drinks do not appear to compensate by reducing calories somewhere else in their diets, so they tend to pack on extra pounds’( Reference Hellmich 16 ) and ‘Study after study has shown that like experimental animals, people do not compensate for extra liquid calories by eating less food’( Reference Brody 17 ). This concept that people do not adjust their energy intake (or expenditure) to compensate for energy consumed as liquids is at the heart of the matter. Yet, is it true? Although opinions on matters of energy compensation in response to various forms of sugar intake and/or liquid energy have been offered for over 70 years( 18 , 19 ), convincing data on these issues have been scarce.

In this issue of the British Journal of Nutrition, Reid et al. ( Reference Reid, Hammersley and Duffy 20 ) offer a new and valuable piece of evidence on this question. In a study of obese adult women, those consuming sugar in liquid form at a level of 1800 kJ (approximately 430 kcal) per d gained far less weight than expected and no more weight than did women in a control group drinking zero-energy beverages. The study has several strengths. It was a controlled trial that was run for long enough to observe weight changes and that was at least partially conducted in a blinded fashion. It also has several limitations, including a modest sample size, incomplete blinding and the fact that it was not strictly randomised. I will not belabour those points here as Reid and colleagues discuss them in their article. It should also be noted that the study concerns only adult women and cannot necessarily tell us about the effects in men or children.

What does the study show?

The study's essential finding concerns the question of compensation for liquid energy. The sucrose group gained no appreciable weight. This shows that over an extended period, at least in conditions similar to those of this study, women do compensate for additional energy consumed in the form of a sugar-sweetened beverage (SSB). Moreover, that the weight gained in the sucrose group was significantly less than that predicted by an established mathematical model based on the amount of energy consumed in the form of SSB further indicates that the vast majority of the energy consumed was compensated for. Reid et al. state that ‘Obese women who received 1800 kJ sucrose per day in soft drinks for four weeks gained a mean of 1·72 kg less than predicted by the model.’ Interestingly, the model predicted a total weight gain for a woman with the average characteristics listed in Reid et al.'s Table 1 of only about 1·8 kg.

Are the findings consistent with those of other studies?

Yes. Kaiser et al. ( Reference Kaiser, Shikany and Keating 15 ) meta-analysed other studies in which adults were required to consume additional energy in SSB in randomised controlled trials (RCT), and found that, on average, such required SSB consumption did indeed cause weight gain, but that the amount of weight gained was far less than half the amount one would have predicted to be gained by use of the same mathematical model used by Reid et al. (see Kaiser et al.'s Fig. 2). This indicates that, as Reid et al. found, over extended periods of time, the majority of the energy consumed as SSB is indeed compensated for.

Do the findings inform us about the effects of reducing sugar-sweetened beverage consumption among adult women?

No. Though tempting, we cannot necessarily infer the effects of reducing SSB consumption from studies of the effects of increasing SSB consumption. That said, as Kaiser et al. ( Reference Kaiser, Shikany and Keating 15 ) reported, no RCT of adults reported to date has found a statistically significant effect of reducing SSB consumption on weight.

Do the findings inform us about the differential effects (if any) of consuming liquid v. solid energy on weight?

No. The results of Reid et al. only show what happens with SSB. From these data alone, we have no way of knowing whether the same results would have been obtained if the women were required to consume 1800 kJ of food in some solid form. Returning to the literature at large, there is evidence from a recent meta-analysis that in short-term (typically single-day) studies with food intake as the outcome, liquid energy is less well compensated for than is solid energy( Reference Almiron-Roig, Palla and Guest 21 ). Yet, we cannot assume that individuals will not adapt to dietary changes over time. Long-term effects on weight cannot be reliably inferred from short-term effects on food intake. Indeed, to my knowledge, there are only two human RCT comparing the effects of liquid v. solid foods on weight over an extended period of time, and neither found a statistically significant difference between the liquid and solid conditions when the entire samples were analysed( Reference DiMeglio and Mattes 22 , Reference Houchins, Burgess and Campbell 23 ).

In conclusion, what we know from the overall literature is that when adults are required to consume additional energy in the form of SSB, on average, they gain some weight. What we also know from the overall literature and this new study is that, on average, adults gain far less weight than they would be expected to gain if they did not compensate. Thus, people clearly do compensate for liquid energy, although they do so incompletely. What we do not know, despite all the drama and vituperation surrounding SSB, is whether, over extended periods of time, people compensate any differently for liquid v. solid energy. It is high time we learned.

Acknowledgements

The present work was supported in part by National Institutes of Health (NIH) grant P30DK056336. The opinions expressed are those of the author and not necessarily those of the NIH or any other organisation.

The author received grants and gifts to his university and consulting fees from multiple for-profit and not-for-profit organisations with interests in obesity, sugar and SSB.

References

1 Anonymous (1853) History of sugar. The Illustrated Magazine of Art, vol. 2, no. 2, p. 147. http://www.jstor.org/stable/20538093 (accessed accessed August 2013).Google Scholar
2 Baru, S (1987) Sugar in history: sweetness and power. Review of Sweetness and Power: The Place of Sugar in Modern History by Sidney W. Mintz. Economic and Political Weekly vol. 22, no. 33 (15 August 1987), pp. 1391–1393. http://www.jstor.org/stable/4377370 (accessed accessed August 2013).Google Scholar
3 Levi, L (1864) On the Sugar Trade and Sugar Duties. A Lecture Delivered at King's College, London, February 29, 1864. London: Effingham Wilson, Royal Exchange. http://www.jstor.org/stable/60101270 (accessed accessed August 2013).Google Scholar
4 US Department of Agriculture, Economic Research Service (1971) A History of Sugar Marketing. Agriculture Economic Report No. 197 . Washington, DC: US Government Printing Office.Google Scholar
5 Galloway, JH (1977) The Mediterranean sugar industry. Geographic Rev 67, 177194. http://www.jstor.org/stable/214019 (accessed accessed August 2013).Google Scholar
6 Slare, F (1713) Part of a letter from Dr. Fred Slare to Dr. Hans Sloane; concerning a person who had a new set of teeth after 80 years of age; with some observations upon the virtues and properties of sugar. Phil Trans R Soc (1683–1775) 28, 273274.Google Scholar
7 Harley, V (1893) Sugar as a food in the production of muscular work. Proc R Soc Lond 54, 480487.Google Scholar
8 Anonymous (1899) Sugar as a ration. Br Med J 1, 105.Google Scholar
9 Gardner, HW (1901) The dietetic value of sugar. Br Med J 1, 10101013.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
10 Sollins, IV (1930) Sugar in diet: an experiment in instruction in candy consumption. J Educ Sociol 3, 546555.Google Scholar
11 Tordoff, MG & Alleva, AM (1990) Effect of drinking soda sweetened with aspartame or high-fructose corn syrup on food intake and body weight. Am J Clin Nutr 51, 963969.Google Scholar
12 Almiron-Roig, E, Chen, Y & Drewnowski, A (2003) Liquid calories and the failure of satiety: how good is the evidence? Obes Rev 4, 201212.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
13 Slavin, J (2012) Beverages and body weight: challenges in the evidence-based review process of the Carbohydrate Subcommittee from the 2010 Dietary Guidelines Advisory Committee. Nutr Rev 70, Suppl. 2, S111S120.Google Scholar
14 Cope, MB & Allison, DB (2010) White hat bias: examples of its presence in obesity research and a call for renewed commitment to faithfulness in research reporting. Int J Obes (Lond) 34, 8488.Google Scholar
15 Kaiser, KA, Shikany, JM, Keating, KD, et al. (2013) Will reducing sugar-sweetened beverage consumption reduce obesity? Evidence supporting conjecture is strong, but evidence when testing effect is weak. Obes Rev 14, 620633.Google Scholar
16 Hellmich, N (2007) Soda drinkers consume more calories. USA Today . http://usatoday30.usatoday.com/news/health/2007-03-25-soda-drinkers_N.htm (accessed accessed August 2013). Google Scholar
17 Brody, JE (2010) A tax to combat America's sugary diet. New York Times . http://www.nytimes.com/2010/04/06/health/06brod.html?_r = 0 (accessed accessed August 2013).Google Scholar
18 Anonymous (1942) Sugar rationing called a “Godsend” to national health. Science News-Letter vol. 41, no. 11 (14 March 1942), p. 164. http://www.jstor.org/stable/3918542 (accessed accessed August 2013).Google Scholar
19 Anonymous (1944) Advice given to go easy on use of chocolate milk. Science News-Letter vol. 45, no. 25 (17 June 1944), p. 398. Google Scholar
20 Reid, M, Hammersley, R, Duffy, M, et al. (2014) Effects on obese women of the sugar sucrose added to the diet over 28 days, a quasi-randomised, single-blind, controlled trial. Br J Nutr 111, 563570.Google Scholar
21 Almiron-Roig, E, Palla, L, Guest, K, et al. (2013) Factors that determine energy compensation: a systematic review of preload studies. Nutr Rev 71, 458473.Google Scholar
22 DiMeglio, DP & Mattes, RD (2000) Liquid versus solid carbohydrate: effects on food intake and body weight. Int J Obes Relat Metab Disord 24, 794800.Google Scholar
23 Houchins, JA, Burgess, JR, Campbell, WW, et al. (2012) Beverage vs. solid fruits and vegetables: effects on energy intake and body weight. Obesity (Silver Spring) 20, 18441850.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed