Hostname: page-component-cd9895bd7-mkpzs Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-12-25T02:49:41.712Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Food choices for weight loss: what dietary strategies would people use?

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  28 November 2023

Luana Giacone*
Affiliation:
ETH Zurich, Department of Health Sciences and Technology, Consumer Behavior, Universitaetstrasse 22, CH-8092 Zurich, Switzerland
Michael Siegrist
Affiliation:
ETH Zurich, Department of Health Sciences and Technology, Consumer Behavior, Universitaetstrasse 22, CH-8092 Zurich, Switzerland
Christina Hartmann
Affiliation:
ETH Zurich, Department of Health Sciences and Technology, Consumer Behavior, Universitaetstrasse 22, CH-8092 Zurich, Switzerland
*
*Corresponding author: Luana Giacone, email [email protected]
Rights & Permissions [Opens in a new window]

Abstract

Previous observational research showed that one of the most common strategies used to lose weight is to avoid or restrict the consumption of specific food items. However, the question of how people behave and implement strategies in actual decision-making situations involving food choices for weight loss purposes remains inconclusive. This experimental study using a food buffet aimed to examine people’s different dietary strategies and motives for selecting foods for an entire day for weight loss purposes compared with a normal-day (ND) food selection. A total of 111 participants (55 % women) had to choose foods for both a ND and a weight loss day (WLD) (within-study design). Kilocalories and nutrients were calculated based on the weights of the foods selected, and food choice motives were assessed using a questionnaire. The results showed that for weight loss purposes, the participants selected more vegetables (both sexes) and unsweetened beverages (only men) while reducing their choices of high-fat and high-energy products (both sexes). Participants’ food choices in both conditions (ND and WLD) differed from the official nutrition recommendations. They chose less carbohydrates and fibres and more fat and sugar than recommended. Health, kilocalories and nutrient content (carbohydrates, sugar, fat and protein) were more important food choice motives for weight loss purposes than for a ND food selection, while taste became less important. In conclusion, the participants appeared to be well capable of implementing several appropriate dietary strategies. Further research is needed to explore strategies to help them maintain these dietary changes over the long term.

Type
Research Article
Creative Commons
Creative Common License - CCCreative Common License - BY
This is an Open Access article, distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution licence (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/), which permits unrestricted re-use, distribution and reproduction, provided the original article is properly cited.
Copyright
© The Author(s), 2023. Published by Cambridge University Press on behalf of The Nutrition Society

For many years now, people’s desire to control their body weights seems to steadily increase, even in non-clinical settings and situations(Reference Ferraro, Patterson and Chaput1,Reference Kruger, Galuska and Serdula2) . More than 40 % of adults worldwide reported some diet attempts in their lives(Reference Santos, Sniehotta and Marques3). Nevertheless, a high percentage of the global population is still overweight or obese because of an imbalance between energy intake and energy expenditure(4), despite people’s increased desire to control their body weights. Therefore, a better understanding of people’s current dietary weight loss strategies may facilitate more effective weight management practices.

Weight loss can be achieved through different strategies. The most common weight loss strategies are changing one’s diet by restricting energy intake and increasing exercise(Reference Kheniser, Saxon and Kashyap5Reference Ramage, Farmer and Apps Eccles8). Further dietary strategies include increasing the fibre content of one’s diet, omitting certain food categories (e.g. sweets and alcohol) or increasing one’s intake of certain food groups (e.g. fruits and vegetables) (for a review, see Ramage et al. (Reference Ramage, Farmer and Apps Eccles8)). Changing the macronutrient composition of the diets (e.g. low-carbohydrate, high-protein or low-fat diets) is a further method used for weight management(Reference Kheniser, Saxon and Kashyap5,Reference Martinez, Navas-Carretero and Saris7,Reference Ramage, Farmer and Apps Eccles8) . It is not possible to state with certainty how promising the various approaches are, but it is assumed that variations in weight loss with different macronutrient diets are probably attributed to differences in adherence(Reference Kheniser, Saxon and Kashyap5,Reference Sacks, Bray and Carey9) . Previous studies primarily examined weight loss strategies by using overweight or obese adults as the participants, particularly those participating in weight loss interventions, or analysed samples predominantly consisting of women(Reference Ramage, Farmer and Apps Eccles8,Reference Barone Gibbs, Kinzel and Pettee Gabriel10Reference Mitchell, Yang and Ho13) .

Observational studies(Reference Kruger, Galuska and Serdula2,Reference Machado, Silveira and Silveira14Reference Yoong, Carey and Sanson-Fisher16) showed that the general population avoided or restricted their consumption of specific foods when trying to control their weights. For example, they drank less alcohol and ate less fatty and sugary foods, less junk/fast food and high-carbohydrate foods, or less meat while eating more fruits and vegetables and consuming more low-energy foods and beverages (for a review, see Santos et al. (Reference Santos, Sniehotta and Marques3)). Further strategies involve skipping meals entirely and/or drinking a lot of water, while men are more likely to skip meals and less likely to drink more water compared with women(Reference Kruger, Galuska and Serdula2,Reference Weiss, Galuska and Khan15) . Most of these observational studies of the general population revealed weight control strategies and practices which were collected mainly by using questionnaires and interviews(Reference Kruger, Galuska and Serdula2,Reference Santos, Sniehotta and Marques3,Reference Machado, Silveira and Silveira14Reference Yoong, Carey and Sanson-Fisher16) rather than observing actual food choice decision-making situations. For instance, one study collected data utilising a 24-h dietary recall and examined whether different weight loss strategies were associated with the consumption of sugar-sweetened beverages (SSB) and snacks, as well as values related to food consumption(Reference Bleich and Wolfson17). Another study that tested multiple weight management strategies found that some strategies were more prevalent, while others were less common in the general population(Reference Keller and Siegrist18). For example, the general population used the strategy of consuming low-energy beverages (water and unsweetened beverages) more often than substituting foods they craved with more nutritious ones (fruits or vegetables instead of sweets or salty snacks)(Reference Keller and Siegrist18). All these previous studies lead to a better understanding of consumers’ eating behaviour if they desire weight loss. However, it is not well understood how consumers implement dietary strategies for weight loss in real-life food choice decisions and how these choices differ from those made without weight loss intention. Therefore, little is known about the actual use and implementation of different weight loss strategies in real-life food choice decision-making situations. Furthermore, it is important to identify potential dietary misconceptions about proper weight loss strategies. Consequently, the primary aim of the present study was to fill this knowledge gap by investigating in a more applied manner which dietary weight loss strategies are used by people when choosing meals for the entire day (weight loss day (WLD)) compared with a normal-day (ND) food selection.

Food choice motives

Food choices are influenced by a diverse range of motives, and weight control can be one of them(Reference Glanz, Basil and Maibach19,Reference Rankin, Bunting and Poínhos20) . However, for the general population, taste is the most important food choice motive, followed by costs, nutritional values and convenience(Reference Glanz, Basil and Maibach19). Previous research with overweight and obese individuals showed that the level of importance of food choice motives differed between weight-stable individuals and weight loss maintainers(Reference Young, Phelan and Alarcon21). For weight loss maintainers, health and the belief that food was low in kilocalories (kcal) were more important food choice motives than for the weight-stable group(Reference Young, Phelan and Alarcon21). As food choice motives shape which food decisions people make, a change in food choice motives should lead to a change in food decisions. Therefore, an understanding of the motives that may drive the general population’s food choices for weight loss purposes is critical for developing weight management strategies that promote long-term success. Following this, the second aim of this study was to examine whether the level of importance of different food choice motives would vary between a WLD and a ND without any weight loss intention.

Methods

Participants

The study participants were recruited through the Consumer Behavior Experimental Panel. The participants had to be at least 18 years of age and fluent in German. They should not suffer from any food allergies or intolerances, not follow a vegan diet, not have a nutritional background, and have the desire to lose weight or maintain it. Each participant was rewarded with 40 CHF for participating in this 40–60 min study. The required sample size for medium effects of 0·4 with a power of 0·8 consisted of fifty-four participants for each sex(Reference Faul, Erdfelder and Lang22). A total of 116 people took part in this study. The data from the participants who confused the two study conditions (n 3) or did not understand the instructions properly (n 2) were excluded from the analysis. Therefore, the data from 111 participants were analysed. Of these participants, 55 % were female, the mean age was 46 years (sd = 14, range = 19–70 years) and 65 % had a higher educational degree (Table 1).

Table 1. Descriptive characteristics of study participants (n 111), separated by sex and comparison to the general Swiss population

Educational level was split into three categories: low = no education, primary and lower secondary school; middle = vocational school; high = higher secondary school, college and university.

Energy needs per day were calculated by multiplying the BMR by physical activity levels.

All participants were informed about the tasks and had to give their written consent before starting the experiment. This study was conducted according to the guidelines laid down in the Declaration of Helsinki, and all procedures involving human subjects were approved by the ethics committee of ETH Zurich (EK 2022-N-61). Written informed consent was obtained from all participants.

Experimental procedure

The study was conducted between May and July 2022 in Zurich, Switzerland. The participants were individually invited to the study room, where they were introduced to the experimental food buffet. The buffet consisted of replica (fake food) and the packaging of real food items (e.g. yogurt cups or cereals). Fake foods are 3D models of real foods moulded from plastic which can be reused, avoid food waste and create a controlled buffet for every participant as the food always looks the same. Previous studies have proven the fake food method as valid and reliable for assessing food choices in a well-controlled environment(Reference Bucher, van der Horst and Siegrist23,Reference Bucher, van der Horst and Siegrist24) .

The experiment included two conditions with a within-study design – a ‘normal day’ (ND) and a ‘weight loss day’ (WLD). Each participant completed both conditions in a quasi-randomised design. The participants were instructed to serve themselves breakfast, lunch, dinner and snacks as they would eat on a usual day (ND). In the other condition, they were instructed to serve themselves breakfast, lunch, dinner and snacks as they would eat if they had the wish to lose weight (WLD). Between the two conditions, the participants were asked to fill out the first part of a questionnaire on a tablet in a separate room. After the second condition, they were asked to finish the questionnaire. Meantime, the investigators took pictures of the assembled meals (see Fig. 1), weighed the continuous components (e.g. rice, pasta and vegetables) and counted the pieces of the single food items (e.g. bread, meat and sweets).

Fig. 1. Food selection of a female participant, with 2004 kcal for the ‘normal day’ (top) and 1609 kcal for the ‘weight loss day’ (bottom).

Food buffet

The food buffet comprised 152 different food items (see Fig. 2). Of these, ninety-two items were fake foods(Reference Bucher, van der Horst and Siegrist23), and sixty items were packages of real foods, presented as single package portions (e.g. yogurt), which were purchased from local retailers. Except for alcoholic beverages, all food categories were represented (e.g. beverages, starchy foods, vegetables, fruits, meat and fish, oils and fats, sweets, and salty snacks). To estimate the total energy and macronutrients of the meals more precisely, sauces (e.g. tomato sauce and basil pesto) in three different sizes, salad dressings (French and Italian) and cold sauces (ketchup, mayonnaise, mustard and tartar) were also provided. Additionally, multiplication cards (e.g. 0·25×, 2×, 3·75×) were put at the participants’ disposal so that they could precisely choose the amount of a single food item (e.g. for tea, coffee or apples). The authenticity of the whole fake food was assessed, ranging from 1 = not realistic at all to 6 = very realistic. Overall, the food buffet was rated as authentic (M = 4·91, sd = 1·02).

Fig. 2. Food buffet containing ninety-two fake food items and sixty real food items.

The weights of the fake food items were converted by a conversion factor to obtain the real weights of the foods. For further information about the conversion factors, see previous publications(Reference Bucher, van der Horst and Siegrist23,Reference Mötteli, Keller and Siegrist25) . If a food item was not chosen by a participant, a value of 0 was assigned. Furthermore, for all items selected by the participants, the energy content, macronutrients, saturated fatty acids (SFA), sugar, dietary fibre and salt content were calculated based on the chosen amounts. The data were obtained from the Swiss food composition database version 6.4 (https://naehrwertdaten.ch) and from the retailer information. To compare the served amounts of carbohydrates, sugar, fat, SFA, fibre, protein and salt between the two experimental conditions, the percentages of the total energy served from these nutrients were calculated for every individual. The amount of carbohydrates, sugar and protein (in grams) was multiplied by 4 kcal, and the amount of fat and SFA (in grams) was multiplied by 9 kcal (https://www.dge-sh.de/energiegehalt.html). Lastly, the energy served from these nutrients were divided by the total energy content (in kcal) and multiplied by 100.

The food items were further classified into food groups (e.g. vegetables, dairy products and sauces) by summing up the energy content of the individual food items.

Questionnaire

The questionnaire included self-reported information on demographics, such as sex, age and education, and dieting attempts. Besides the demographic variables, self-reported weight and height were assessed to calculate the participants’ BMI (kg/m2). Additionally, the participants rated their hunger status from 1 = not hungry at all to 6 = very hungry. Overall, the participants were not hungry (M = 2·62, sd = 1·27).

Energy needs

The participants’ energy needs (EN) per d (in kilocalories (kcal)) were calculated by multiplying their basal metabolic rates by their physical activity levels. Their basal metabolic rates were calculated based on the Mifflin–St. Jeor equation(Reference Mifflin, St Jeor and Hill26), and their physical activity levels were calculated according to Johansson and Westerterp(Reference Johansson and Westerterp27).

Perceived tastiness, perceived filling and food choice motives

The questionnaire further included questions about the participants’ specific food choices. The participants rated their perceived tastiness of their food selections in both conditions separately by answering the question ‘How tasty would you find your meal choices?’ from 1 = not tasty at all to 5 = very tasty (in German: ‘Wie gut würden Ihnen Ihre ausgewählten Mahlzeiten schmecken?’, 1 = gar nicht schmecken, 5 = sehr gut schmecken). Furthermore, participants rated their perceived filling of their food selections by answering the question ‘How filling would you find your meal choices?’ from 0 = not at all filling to 100 = very filling (in German: ‘Wie sättigend würden Sie die Auswahl ihrer Mahlzeiten empfinden?’, 0 = gar nicht sättigend, 100 = sehr sättigend). They were also asked how important each food choice motive was for them when assembling their foods. The included motives were taste, naturalness, product category (e.g. meat, dairy products and fruit), kilocalories, carbohydrate content, sugar content, protein content, fat content, health and familiarity. Each item was rated from 1 = not important at all to 6 = very important.

Data analysis

An ANOVA with Bonferroni-adjusted post hoc tests was used to assess the difference between EN and total kcal in the ND and the WLD conditions. Furthermore, paired t tests were used to evaluate the differences between the two conditions regarding the participants’ tastiness and filling perceptions of their chosen foods. A repeated-measures ANOVA with Bonferroni-adjusted pairwise comparisons was performed to compare the effects of the two conditions (ND and WLD) on the importance of food choice motives.

The assumptions for parametric testing were not fulfilled for the food items and the nutritional properties of the foods. Therefore, Wilcoxon signed-rank tests were used to evaluate the differences between the two study conditions regarding nutrient contents. McNemar tests were performed to determine whether the participants skipped meals in one or the other study condition and to explore which food groups were chosen or not chosen in either of the two study conditions. Analyses were conducted separately by sex. To assess the differences between the sexes, independent t tests for tastiness and filling perceptions, as well as for food choice motives, were performed. All statistical analyses were performed using the IBM SPSS version 28.0. A significance level of P ≤ 0·05 was used in this study.

Results

Energy needs and served kilocalories

An ANOVA showed that the participants’ energy needs (EN) and the total kilocalories (kcal) from the ‘normal day’ (ND) and the ‘weight loss day’ (WLD) food selections differed significantly for men (F(2,96) = 84·95, P < 0·001, ω 2 = 0·64) and women (F(2,118) = 72·78, P < 0·001, ω2 = 0·55) (see Fig. 3). Post hoc analyses revealed no difference between EN and ND energy selection, neither for men (P = 0·261) nor for women (P = 0·318). Post hoc analyses further revealed a significant difference between EN and WLD, and between ND and WLD for both men (P = <0·001) and women (P = <0·001). Overall, men’s food selection, on average, amounted to 2704 kcal (sd = 945) in the ND condition and 1537 kcal (sd = 532) in the WLD condition. Women’s food selection, on average, amounted to 2034 kcal (sd = 766) in the ND condition and 1266 kcal (sd = 496) in the WLD condition. Moreover, 95 % of the participants selected a WLD food composition whose total kcal were below their EN. In other words, the participants selected approximately as many kcal for the ND and fewer kcal for the WLD as they physiologically would need to maintain their current weights. However, on average, men reduced the energy by 1166 kcal (sd = 936), while women reduced the energy by 768 kcal (sd = 667) in the WLD food selection compared with the ND food selection.

Fig. 3. Differences between energy needs, total kilocalories selection for the ‘normal day’ and for the ‘weight loss day,’ separated by sex.

Note. Two participants did not report their heights and weights; therefore, their energy needs could not be calculated.

Meal skipping and meal ratings

When comparing the main meals and snack frequencies between the two study conditions (ND v. WLD), McNemar tests showed that women were significantly more likely to skip their snacks on the WLD compared with the ND (P = 0·04, OR = 29·38). In particular, eight women chose a snack for the ND but no snack for the WLD. Five men skipped their snacks for the WLD, although they chose a snack for the ND (P > 0·05). There were no significant main meal skips for both men and women. In other words, men and women chose the same main meals (breakfast, lunch and dinner) for the ND as for the WLD.

The participants further indicated their perceived tastiness and perceived filling of their food selection in both conditions separately. They perceived their WLD food selection as significantly less tasty (M = 4·6, sd = 1, P < 0·001, Cohen’s |d|= 0·56) and less filling (M = 66, sd = 20, P < 0·001, Cohen’s |d|= 0·88) compared with their ND food selection (tastiness: M = 4·2, sd = 1, filling: M = 83, sd = 16). Nevertheless, women found their WLD food selection (M = 4·3, sd = 1) significantly tastier than men did (M = 4·0, sd = 1, P = 0·04, Cohen’s |d| = 0·40).

Dietary weight loss strategies

Table 2 (men) and Table 3 (women) provide insights into the differences in food selection between the ND and the WLD conditions. Table 4 displays the changes in kcal by food group between the ND and the WLD. Detailed information on which food items are classified under which food groups can be found in the supplementary material.

Table 2. Food group selection for the ‘normal day’ and the ‘weight loss day’ for men (n 50) and corresponding McNemar tests

SSB, sugar-sweetened beverages.

* No test statistics can be calculated if two cells are empty.

Table 3. Food group selection for the ‘normal day’ and the ‘weight loss day’ for women (n 61) and corresponding McNemar tests

SSB, sugar-sweetened beverages.

* No test statistics can be calculated if two cells are empty.

Table 4. Comparisons of food groups’ kilocalories between the ‘normal day’ and the ‘weight loss day’, and Wilcoxon signed-rank tests, separated by sex

SSB, sugar-sweetened beverages.

Mdn and IQR indicate only 50 % and 25–75 % of the values. However, some products were selected only by a few people; therefore, zero values can occur and still result in significant test results.

McNemar tests showed that on a product level (taken v. not taken, independent of amount), there were significant differences between the ND and the WLD for women and men. A significant number of men and women omitted SSB, margarine and butter, sauce, processed meat, fast food, and sweets (P < 0·05) for weight loss purposes, meaning that they chose these food groups for the ND food selection but did not choose them for the WLD food selection. Men additionally omitted bread and croissants (P = 0·002), red meat (P = 0·003), and salty snacks (P = 0·02) for the WLD but chose them for the ND. An increase in poultry consumption was observed among women. Of the women who did not choose poultry for the ND, 38 % did so for the WLD. No significant effects were found for vegetables, fruits and pulses for either man or woman.

The analyses of energy changes within food groups showed that both men and women significantly selected more kcal from vegetables in the WLD condition. Men also significantly increased the amount (in millilitres) of unsweetened beverages (P = 0·02) and the kcal from poultry (P < 0·001) for weight loss purposes. Additionally, the kcal were significantly decreased for dairy products, pasta, rice and potato, and salty snacks for the WLD food selection compared with that for the ND in both sexes. Women further selected significantly less millilitres from zero beverages (P = 0·04) and fewer kcal from bread and croissants (P < 0·001). There were no statistically significant changes in the selection of kcal from fruits, pulses, cereals, eggs, fish and fish sticks, and vegetarian alternatives (P > 0·05) in the ND and the WLD conditions.

Importance of food choice motives

A repeated-measures ANOVA with Bonferroni-adjusted pairwise comparisons was performed to compare the effects of the two conditions (ND and WLD) on the importance of food choice motives. There was a significant interaction effect between the conditions and the importance of food choice motives (F(9, 990) = 31·68, P < 0·001, ηp 2 = 0·22). Pairwise comparisons showed that taste was significantly less important for the WLD food selection than that for the ND (M normal = 5·32, se = 0·90, M weight loss = 4·72, se = 1·03, F(1, 110) = 32·11, P < 0·001, ηp 2 = 0·23). In contrast, naturalness, kilocalories, carbohydrate content, sugar content, protein content, fat content and health were significantly more important for the WLD food selection than that for the ND (P < 0·01). There was no difference between ND and WLD regarding the importance of the product category and familiarity (P > 0·05) for food choice (see Fig. 4).

Fig. 4. Motives important for food choice.

Note. Each motive was rated separately for each condition (ND and WLD) on a scale ranging from 1 = not important at all to 6 = very important. Means for every food choice motive is displayed separately for the ND and the WLD condition, and P-values from ANOVA with Bonferroni-adjusted post hoc tests are shown.

For their food choices, men assigned less importance to health (P = 0·03, Cohen’s |d| = 0·43), which accounted for the only sex difference for the ND selection. For the WLD selection, taste (P = 0·002, Cohen’s |d| = 0·60), naturalness (P = 0·01, Cohen’s |d| = 0·50) and health (P = 0·004, Cohen’s |d| = 0·56) were more important food choice motives for women than for men.

Proportion of nutrients and dietary recommendations

Differences in nutrient relations between the ND condition and the WLD condition are summarised in Table 5. In the WLD condition, the participants served themselves a significantly higher percentage of total energy from protein and sugar, while choosing a lower percentage of total energy from total fat and saturated fatty acids (SFA).

Table 5. Nutrient comparison between the ‘normal day’ and the ‘weight loss day’, and Swiss dietary recommendations, separated by sex

TE, total energy served; SFA, saturated fatty acids.

* German Society for Nutrition (DGE) reference values(41).

WHO recommendations(42).

Sixth Swiss Nutrition Report(Reference Keller, Battaglia and Beer40).

As shown in Table 5, the average nutrient selection for both days deviated from the Swiss dietary recommendations for both sexes. The recommended intake levels for carbohydrates and fibre were not reached on either day, whereas the portions for sugar, total fat and SFA exceeded the recommendations on either day. The recommended protein intake levels were met or were even slightly exceeded on both days and for both sexes. The salt intake recommendations for women were only exceeded on the ND food selection, while those for men were exceeded on both days.

Discussion

The goal of the present study was to investigate consumers’ ability to select foods for an entire day when imagining their desire to lose weight. The study further examined whether their choices followed the official dietary recommendations and what food choice motives drove their overall selections. The results indicated that the participants composed meals for the entire day that provided less energy than their daily energy needds (EN). However, energy reduction was on average very high (1166 kcal and 768 kcal, for men and women, respectively). The weight loss strategies that were used consisted of increasing the amounts of vegetables and unsweetened beverages (only men), while reducing high-fat and high-energy products. Dietary recommendations were not met in both the ‘normal day’ (ND) and the ‘weight loss day’ (WLD) conditions. Health, kilocalories and nutrient content were more important food choice motives for a WLD than for an ND.

Generally, participants’ energy selection for the ND condition was almost the same as their calculated EN. This means that the participants were able to meet their EN for a full day quite well. Considering that neither their EN nor the kilocalories of the food items were displayed to them, this is surprising. It shows that people are capable of making food choices to maintain their weight. However, compared with the ND, participants reduced their energy selection on average up to 768 kcal (women) and 1166 kcal (men) per day on the WLD. Given that a daily calorie reduction of 500 kcal leads roughly to a weight reduction of up to 0·5 kg per week(Reference Koliaki, Spinos and Spinou28 and 29), the female participants of this study would roughly lose up to 0·7 kilogram body weight per week and the male participants roughly up to 1·1 kilogram body weight per week if they maintained the calorie deficit on a daily basis. This is a quite high energy deficit and body weight reduction(Reference Mero, Huovinen and Matintupa30) and a potential problem, as a large energy deficit will likely make participants less compliant in their weight control behaviours over the long term. However, unplanned in-between consumption is common, especially when people keep their energy intake on a low level. Therefore, the actual energy deficit will probably be smaller than the one found in this study, since no unplanned in-between consumption could be measured.

Dietary weight loss strategies

Different dietary weight loss strategies have been identified in previous research through questionnaires and interviews. A common strategy involves individuals’ avoidance or restricted intake of specific foods (for a review, see Santos et al. (Reference Santos, Sniehotta and Marques3)), such as desserts, SSB and fried foods(Reference Barone Gibbs, Kinzel and Pettee Gabriel10), consistent with the present study’s results. This study’s participants decreased or even avoided the selection of high-fat and high-energy products, including sauces (e.g. cream, carbonara and gravy), fast food (e.g. cheeseburger and pizza), sweets and sugar-sweetened beverages (SSB) for a WLD compared with a ND. Although some participants omitted sweets entirely for weight loss purposes, sweets did not seem to be replaced with fruits, since no change in the amount of fruits selected was observed. Therefore, reducing high-fat and high-energy foods seems to be an energy-saving strategy that people know and implement.

A further strategy used by both sexes involved increasing the amount of vegetables in the WLD condition compared with the ND condition. Previous studies have shown that increased consumption of vegetables helps people lose weight(Reference Ramage, Farmer and Apps Eccles8 and Reference Mitchell, Yang and Behr31) because vegetables have a low energy density, contain essential vitamins and minerals, and are sources of water and fibre(Reference Mitchell, Yang and Behr31Reference Slavin and Lloyd33). Therefore, increasing the amount of vegetables is a key strategy to facilitate weight loss, which was employed by the participants of the present study.

Regarding the consumption of animal protein, the results showed no change in the total meat consumption between the study conditions, neither for men nor for women. Only for the subcategories processed meat and poultry, less of the first one and more of the second one was selected in the WLD condition. This strategy seems reasonable, as poultry is one of the leanest types of meat, with high-protein, low-fat and low-energy contents(Reference Pereira and Vicente34). In contrast, processed meat generally has the highest fat content of up to 25 %(Reference Cosgrove, Flynn and Kiely35). Therefore, decreasing processed meat consumption and increasing poultry consumption could facilitate weight loss by reducing energy intake, which was a strategy applied by the participants of the present study in a simulated setting. Fish is another source of animal protein, is part of a healthy diet and is suggested to support weight reduction(Reference Barone Gibbs, Kinzel and Pettee Gabriel10 and Reference Thorsdottir, Tomasson and Gunnarsdottir36 and Reference Abete, Astrup and Martínez37). However, the participants of the present study did not select more fish in the WLD condition, and only a few of them selected it in the ND condition. A possible explanation is the generally low level of fish consumption in the German-speaking part of Switzerland(Reference Matthes, Zuberbuehler and Rohrmann38). Alternative plant-based protein sources are pulses (e.g. peas and green beans). The participants selected pulses in both conditions but did not increase the portion size in either condition. This finding seems to indicate the need to raise awareness about pulses(Reference Henn, Bøye Olsen and Goddyn39) and fish as substitutes for meat and the importance of reducing high amounts of processed meat to facilitate weight loss.

Decreasing or avoiding the intake of SSB is a further way to limit energy intake by decreasing the sugar intake. Furthermore, drinking a lot of water is a diet strategy too as it has been suggested to suppress hunger pangs and therefore minimise energy intake, especially if SSB are replaced by water(Reference Weiss, Galuska and Khan15). In the present study, men used these weight loss strategies by selecting more unsweetened beverages and omitting SSB in the WLD condition. However, this result contradicts another study’s finding that women reported drinking a lot of water more often than men did(Reference Weiss, Galuska and Khan15). However, in the current study, women chose a higher volume of unsweetened beverages than men did, regardless of the condition. Although the effectiveness of water as a strategy is unknown(Reference Weiss, Galuska and Khan15), switching from SSB to unsweetened beverages is an easily implementable strategy to decrease energy intake by decreasing sugar intakes.

Overall, participants were aware that avoiding SSB and high-energy foods while increasing the consumption of vegetables and unsweetened beverages are reasonable dietary changes to lose weight. Moreover, these strategies are also in line with public nutrition recommendations(29,Reference Keller, Battaglia and Beer40) . Therefore, further efforts should be made to help the population make favourable dietary changes and maintaining them, not only to lose weight but also to maintain a healthy weight.

Dietary recommendations

The participants’ food selection for weight loss had a lower percentage of total energy from total fat and saturated fatty acids (SFA) but a higher percentage of total energy from protein. Except for protein, participants’ choices differed from the Swiss dietary recommendations in both conditions. Moreover, the percentage of total energy from carbohydrates was not altered between the study conditions and was lower than the Swiss nutrition recommendations(Reference Keller, Battaglia and Beer40). This lower amount of carbohydrates could reflect the popularity of low-carbohydrate diets, even in normal eating conditions(Reference Mötteli, Keller and Siegrist25).

Participants fibre quantity differed from the recommended quantity of > 30 g per day(Reference Keller, Battaglia and Beer4042) in either condition. This is in line with previous studies showing that the fibre content in meals was too low(Reference Mötteli, Keller and Siegrist25 and Reference Stephen, Champ and Cloran43). Dietary fibres have been shown to facilitate weight loss(Reference Ramage, Farmer and Apps Eccles8) by increasing satiety(Reference Bellissimo and Akhavan44), and they provide various health benefits, such as lowering the risks for diabetes, obesity or coronary heart diseases(Reference Anderson, Baird and Davis45). However, the present study’s participants did not choose more pulses for weight loss purposes, which are high in fibre and protein and low in fat(Reference Chen, McGee and Vandemark46). Therefore, it is important to raise more awareness about the benefits of pulses and vegetables and how they can be integrated into daily meals.

Even though the participants reduced their total fat intake in this experimental setting, and the study neglected food preparation (e.g. frying with oil or butter), the amount was still above Swiss recommendations(Reference Keller, Battaglia and Beer40). The participants chose more SFA in both conditions than recommended, confirming the findings of a previous study(Reference Mötteli, Keller and Siegrist25). However, reducing fat intake helps to lose weight by decreasing the total energy intake, as fat has double the number of kcal for the same amount than carbohydrates or proteins have(Reference Kim6). Thus, the participants of the current study seemed to understand that they should reduce their fat intake to decrease their energy consumption. However, there seems to be a lack of knowledge about how much fat and especially how much SFA different food items have. Therefore, greater awareness about the fat content of different food items should be promoted, not only for weight loss purposes.

Overall, reducing energy intake to a minimum and avoiding or restricting the consumption of single food groups are insufficient strategies, since long-term adherence to this dietary regime will probably be low. Instead, it is necessary to modify people’s diets in a sustainable way, especially to comply with dietary guidelines.

Food choice motives

Besides the dietary strategies, this study’s results show that if people are asked to put together meals for an entire day to lose weight (WLD), taste as a food choice motive becomes less important to them compared with an ND composition. Perceived tastiness also decreased for the weight loss meals compared with the ND meals, even though the participants could choose whatever they liked. Nonetheless, taste is generally the most important food choice motive(Reference Glanz, Basil and Maibach19); therefore, sacrificing taste for weight loss purposes could lead to compliance problems, which might result in relapses into old dietary behaviour patterns over time.

The more important food choice motives for weight loss purposes were kilocalories and nutrient contents (carbohydrates, sugar, protein and fat). This partially supports a previous study’s finding that nutrition information was more important for food decision-making by people with a weight loss intention than for people without that intention(Reference Bleich and Wolfson47). It is reasonable that people focus more on kilocalories and nutrient contents of various foods when trying to lose weight since they must attain und maintain a negative energy balance.

Limitations

The participants were required to select foods for an entire day; however, it is unclear whether they would have eaten everything they chose or would have consumed even more in reality. Furthermore, by using a food buffet, the experimental setting was highly controlled and did not take into account other factors, such as price, cooking skills or food availability, which are important determinants for food choice(Reference Story, Kaphingst and Robinson-O’Brien48). Since food preparation (frying and seasoning) was not considered in this study, underestimations, especially of fat and salt intake, cannot be ruled out. However, the participants’ energy selection for the ND was almost the same as their calculated EN. Therefore, their choices seemed to represent good guesses of their daily energy intake.

The authors were interested in the participants’ food selections and the changes in their food selections for weight loss. This was a hypothetical WLD and did not show food selections over a longer period. Furthermore, the food buffet excluded alcoholic beverages, which is a further limitation of this study. However, the authors decided not to offer alcoholic beverages based on the assumption that the participants would not drink alcohol daily but rather in social situations. Alcohol is also highly susceptible to underreporting(Reference Livingston and Callinan49), which would have affected this study’s results. Furthermore, the direction of the importance of each food choice factor in this study is not clear. For example, it could be that participants thought it was important to reduce carbohydrates to lose weight, but it could also be that they thought it was important to increase carbohydrates to lose weight. Therefore, further research is needed to identify the direction of the importance of different food choice motives.

It is easier for people to reduce their daily energy intake than to fundamentally increase their physical activities in daily life. Furthermore, most individuals fail to achieve adequate energy expenditure through exercise, or they even compensate for the energy burned by eating afterwards(Reference Jakicic, Clark and Coleman50). Thus, the current study focused on food selection and neglected physical activity. The authors cannot rule out the possibility of the participants’ attempts to compensate for their lack of physical activity (i.e. not included as a strategy) by an even higher reduction in their food selection.

Compared with the general Swiss population(5153), our study had more participants with a higher educational level (65 % v. 45 %) and included more people with a normal BMI range of 18·5–24·9 kg/m2 (62 % v. 55 %). This may have had an impact on the results of this study; thus, the results should be interpreted with caution.

Conclusion

This study examined the dietary strategies that people would tend to implement in a real-life decision-making food choice situation for weight loss purposes and the motives behind their food selections. The results suggest that people use the appropriate strategy of increasing their selection of low-energy products (e.g. vegetables) while decreasing their choice of high-energy and high-fat products (e.g. sweets). Furthermore, their food selections for weight loss are based mostly on their food choice motives – taste, kilocalories and nutrient content. Therefore, this study shows that people are capable of choosing an adequate number of kilocalories to maintain their body weights, as well as implementing a lower energy intake for weight loss. While reducing energy intake to a great extent, they would probably still consume too much fat and sugar, as well as too little carbohydrates and fibres, and taste would become less important. As a result, the new diet would more likely be abandoned over the long term, leading to a weight regain. However, it is encouraging that the participants knew how to make appropriate dietary changes to lose weight. Further studies should examine strategies that can support consumers in implementing and sustaining these dietary modifications over an extended period.

Acknowledgements

The authors thank Nadia Ricklin, Livia Thoma and Natalie Birnbaum for their help in data collection.

This research received no specific grant from any funding agency, commercial or not-for-profit sectors.

L. G, C. H. and M. S. designed the study. L. G. formulated the research questions, collected, analysed and interpreted the data, and wrote the manuscript. L. G., C. H., and M. S. revised the manuscript and approved the final version.

The study was approved by the Ethics Committee of ETH Zurich (EK 2022-N-61).

There are no conflict of interest

Supplementary material

For supplementary material/s referred to in this article, please visit https://doi.org/10.1017/S0007114523002726

References

Ferraro, ZM, Patterson, S & Chaput, JP (2015) Unhealthy weight control practices: culprits and clinical recommendations. Clin Med Insights Endocrinol Diabetes 8, 711.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kruger, J, Galuska, DA, Serdula, MK, et al. (2004) Attempting to lose weight. Am J Prev Med 26, 402406.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Santos, I, Sniehotta, FF, Marques, MM, et al. (2017) Prevalence of personal weight control attempts in adults: a systematic review and meta-analysis: weight control attempts in adults. Obes Rev 18, 3250.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
WHO (2021) Obesity and Overweight (Internet). https://www.who.int/news-room/fact-sheets/detail/obesity-and-overweight (accessed March 2023).Google Scholar
Kheniser, K, Saxon, DR & Kashyap, SR (2021) Long-term weight loss strategies for obesity. J Clin Endocrinol Metab 106, 18541866.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kim, JY (2021) Optimal diet strategies for weight loss and weight loss maintenance. J Obes Metab Syndr 30, 2031.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Martinez, JA, Navas-Carretero, S, Saris, WHM, et al. (2014) Personalized weight loss strategies—the role of macronutrient distribution. Nat Rev Endocrinol 10, 749760.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ramage, S, Farmer, A, Apps Eccles, K, et al. (2014) Healthy strategies for successful weight loss and weight maintenance: a systematic review. Appl Physiol Nutr Metab 39, 120.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Sacks, FM, Bray, GA, Carey, VJ, et al. (2009) Comparison of weight-loss diets with different compositions of fat, protein, and carbohydrates. N Engl J Med 360, 859873.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Barone Gibbs, B, Kinzel, LS, Pettee Gabriel, K, et al. (2012) Short- and long-term eating habit modification predicts weight change in overweight, postmenopausal women: results from the WOMAN study. J Acad Nutr Diet 112, 13471355.e2.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Collins, CE (2011) Dietary strategies for successful weight loss and maintenance: more evidence required. J Am Diet Assoc 111, 18221825.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Elfhag, K & Rossner, S (2005) Who succeeds in maintaining weight loss? A conceptual review of factors associated with weight loss maintenance and weight regain. Obes Rev 6, 6785.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Mitchell, ES, Yang, Q, Ho, AS, et al. (2021) Self-reported nutritional factors are associated with weight loss at 18 months in a self-managed commercial program with food categorization system: observational study. Nutrients 13, 1733.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Machado, EC, Silveira, MF & Silveira, VMF (2012) Prevalence of weight-loss strategies and use of substances for weight-loss among adults: a population study. Cad Saúde Pública 28, 14391449.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Weiss, EC, Galuska, DA, Khan, LK, et al. (2006) Weight-control practices among U.S. adults, 2001–2002. Am J Prev Med 31, 1824.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Yoong, SL, Carey, ML, Sanson-Fisher, RW, et al. (2012) A cross-sectional study assessing the self-reported weight loss strategies used by adult Australian general practice patients. BMC Fam Pract 13, 48.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bleich, SN & Wolfson, JA (2014) Weight loss strategies: association with consumption of sugary beverages, snacks and values about food purchases. Patient Educ Couns 96, 128134.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Keller, C & Siegrist, M (2015) The weight management strategies inventory (WMSI). Development of a new measurement instrument, construct validation, and association with dieting success. Appetite 92, 322336.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Glanz, K, Basil, M, Maibach, E, et al. (1998) Why Americans eat what they do. J Am Diet Assoc 98, 11181126.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Rankin, A, Bunting, BP, Poínhos, R, et al. (2018) Food choice motives, attitude towards and intention to adopt personalised nutrition. Public Health Nutr 21, 26062616.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Young, J, Phelan, S, Alarcon, N, et al. (2021) Factors associated with food choice among long-term weight loss maintainers. J Hum Nutr Diet 35 , 110.Google ScholarPubMed
Faul, F, Erdfelder, E, Lang, AG, et al. (2007) G*Power 3: a flexible statistical power analysis program for the social, behavioral, and biomedical sciences. Behav Res Methods 39, 175191.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bucher, T, van der Horst, K & Siegrist, M (2012) The fake food buffet – a new method in nutrition behaviour research. Br J Nutr 107, 15531560.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bucher, T, van der Horst, K & Siegrist, M (2013) Fruit for dessert. How people compose healthier meals. Appetite 60, 7480.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Mötteli, S, Keller, C, Siegrist, M, et al. (2016) Consumers’ practical understanding of healthy food choices: a fake food experiment. Br J Nutr 116, 559566.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Mifflin, MD, St Jeor, ST & Hill, LA (1990) A new predictive equation for resting energy expenditure in healthy individuals. Am J Clin Nutr 51, 241247.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Johansson, G & Westerterp, KR (2008) Assessment of the physical activity level with two questions: validation with doubly labeled water. Int J Obes 32, 10311033.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Koliaki, C, Spinos, T, Spinou, M, et al. (2018) Defining the optimal dietary approach for safe, effective and sustainable weight loss in overweight and obese adults. Healthcare 6, 73.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
U.S. Department of Health and Human Services & U.S. Department of Agriculture (2015) 2015–2020 Dietary Guidelines for Americans (Internet). 8th Edition. https://health.gov/our-work/nutrition-physical-activity/dietary-guidelines/previous-dietary-guidelines/2015 (accessed March 2023).Google Scholar
Mero, AA, Huovinen, H, Matintupa, O, et al. (2010) Moderate energy restriction with high protein diet results in healthier outcome in women. J Int Soc Sports Nutr 7, 4.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Mitchell, ES, Yang, Q, Behr, H, et al. (2021) Adherence to healthy food choices during the COVID-19 pandemic in a U.S. population attempting to lose weight. Nutr Metab Cardiovasc Dis 31, 21652172.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Darmon, N, Darmon, M, Maillot, M, et al. (2005) A nutrient density standard for vegetables and fruits: nutrients per calorie and nutrients per unit cost. J Am Diet Assoc 105, 18811887.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Slavin, JL & Lloyd, B (2012) Health benefits of fruits and vegetables. Adv Nutr 3, 506516.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Pereira, PM & Vicente, AF (2013) Meat nutritional composition and nutritive role in the human diet. Meat Sci 93, 586592.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cosgrove, M, Flynn, A & Kiely, M (2005) Consumption of red meat, white meat and processed meat in Irish adults in relation to dietary quality. Br J Nutr 93, 933942.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Thorsdottir, I, Tomasson, H, Gunnarsdottir, I, et al. (2007) Randomized trial of weight-loss-diets for young adults varying in fish and fish oil content. Int J Obes 31, 15601566.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Abete, I, Astrup, A, Martínez, JA, et al. (2010) Obesity and the metabolic syndrome: role of different dietary macronutrient distribution patterns and specific nutritional components on weight loss and maintenance. Nutr Rev 68, 214231.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Matthes, KL, Zuberbuehler, CA, Rohrmann, S, et al. (2022) Selling, buying and eating – a synthesis study on dietary patterns across language regions in Switzerland. Br Food J 124, 15021518.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Henn, K, Bøye Olsen, S, Goddyn, H, et al. (2022) Willingness to replace animal-based products with pulses among consumers in different European countries. Food Res Int 157, 111403.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Keller, U, Battaglia, RE, Beer, M, et al. (2012) Sechster Schweizerischer Ernährungsbericht (6th Swiss Nutrition Report). Bern: Bundesamt für Gesundheit (Federal Office for Public Health). pp. 302.Google Scholar
German Society for Nutrition (2022) Deutsche Gesellschaft für Ernährung (German Society for Nutrition). Referenzwerte (Reference Values). https://www.dge.de/wissenschaft/referenzwerte/ (accessed March 2023).Google Scholar
WHO World Health Organization (2020) Healthy Diet. https://www.who.int/news-room/fact-sheets/detail/healthy-diet (accessed March 2023).Google Scholar
Stephen, AM, Champ, MMJ, Cloran, SJ, et al. (2017) Dietary fibre in Europe: current state of knowledge on definitions, sources, recommendations, intakes and relationships to health. Nutr Res Rev 30, 149190.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bellissimo, N & Akhavan, T (2015) Effect of macronutrient composition on short-term food intake and weight loss. Adv Nutr 6, 302S308S.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Anderson, JW, Baird, P, Davis, RH Jr, et al. (2009) Health benefits of dietary fiber. Nutr Rev 67, 188205.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Chen, Y, McGee, R, Vandemark, G, et al. (2016) Dietary fiber analysis of four pulses using AOAC 2011.25: implications for human health. Nutrients 8, 829.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bleich, SN & Wolfson, JA (2015) Differences in consumer use of food labels by weight loss strategies and demographic characteristics. BMC Public Health 15, 1275.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Story, M, Kaphingst, KM, Robinson-O’Brien, R, et al. (2008) Creating healthy food and eating environments: policy and environmental approaches. Annu Rev Public Health 29, 253272.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Livingston, M & Callinan, S (2015) Underreporting in alcohol surveys: whose drinking is underestimated? J Stud Alcohol Drugs 76, 158164.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Jakicic, JM, Clark, K, Coleman, E, et al. (2001) Appropriate intervention strategies for weight loss and prevention of weight regain for adults. Med Sci Sports Exerc 33, 21452156.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Swiss Federal Statistical Office (2022) Education Level of the Population – Indicator Data (Internet). https://www.bfs.admin.ch/bfs/de/home/statistiken/wirtschaftliche-soziale-situation-bevoelkerung/gleichstellung-frau-mann/bildung/bildungsstand.assetdetail.24485179.html (accessed January 2023).Google Scholar
Swiss Federal Statistical Office (2022) Body Mass Index (BMI) by Gender, Age, Education Level, Language Area (Internet). https://www.bfs.admin.ch/bfs/de/home/statistiken/gesundheit/determinanten/uebergewicht.assetdetail.6466017.html (accessed January 2023).Google Scholar
Swiss Federal Statistical Office (2022) Average Age of the Permanent Resident Population by Citizenship Category, Gender and Canton, 2010–2022 (Internet). https://www.bfs.admin.ch/bfs/de/home/statistiken/bevoelkerung/stand-entwicklung/alter.assetdetail.27225505.html (accessed January 2023).Google Scholar
Figure 0

Table 1. Descriptive characteristics of study participants (n 111), separated by sex and comparison to the general Swiss population

Figure 1

Fig. 1. Food selection of a female participant, with 2004 kcal for the ‘normal day’ (top) and 1609 kcal for the ‘weight loss day’ (bottom).

Figure 2

Fig. 2. Food buffet containing ninety-two fake food items and sixty real food items.

Figure 3

Fig. 3. Differences between energy needs, total kilocalories selection for the ‘normal day’ and for the ‘weight loss day,’ separated by sex.Note. Two participants did not report their heights and weights; therefore, their energy needs could not be calculated.

Figure 4

Table 2. Food group selection for the ‘normal day’ and the ‘weight loss day’ for men (n 50) and corresponding McNemar tests

Figure 5

Table 3. Food group selection for the ‘normal day’ and the ‘weight loss day’ for women (n 61) and corresponding McNemar tests

Figure 6

Table 4. Comparisons of food groups’ kilocalories between the ‘normal day’ and the ‘weight loss day’, and Wilcoxon signed-rank tests, separated by sex

Figure 7

Fig. 4. Motives important for food choice.Note. Each motive was rated separately for each condition (ND and WLD) on a scale ranging from 1 = not important at all to 6 = very important. Means for every food choice motive is displayed separately for the ND and the WLD condition, and P-values from ANOVA with Bonferroni-adjusted post hoc tests are shown.

Figure 8

Table 5. Nutrient comparison between the ‘normal day’ and the ‘weight loss day’, and Swiss dietary recommendations, separated by sex

Supplementary material: File

Giacone et al. supplementary material

Giacone et al. supplementary material
Download Giacone et al. supplementary material(File)
File 224.5 KB