Hostname: page-component-78c5997874-xbtfd Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-11-08T01:21:04.822Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Effects of carbohydrate sugars and artificial sweeteners on appetite and the secretion of gastrointestinal satiety peptides

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  24 January 2011

Robert E. Steinert*
Affiliation:
Division of Gastroenterology, Department of Biomedicine, Clinical Research Center, University Hospital Basel, CH-4031Basel, Switzerland
Florian Frey
Affiliation:
Division of Gastroenterology, Department of Biomedicine, Clinical Research Center, University Hospital Basel, CH-4031Basel, Switzerland
Antonia Töpfer
Affiliation:
Division of Gastroenterology, Department of Biomedicine, Clinical Research Center, University Hospital Basel, CH-4031Basel, Switzerland
Jürgen Drewe
Affiliation:
Division of Gastroenterology, Department of Biomedicine, Clinical Research Center, University Hospital Basel, CH-4031Basel, Switzerland
Christoph Beglinger
Affiliation:
Division of Gastroenterology, Department of Biomedicine, Clinical Research Center, University Hospital Basel, CH-4031Basel, Switzerland
*
*Corresponding author: R. E. Steinert, fax +41 612655352, email [email protected]
Rights & Permissions [Opens in a new window]

Abstract

In vitro, both carbohydrate sugars and artificial sweeteners (AS) stimulate the secretion of glucagon-like peptide-1 (GLP-1). It has been suggested that the gut tastes sugars and AS through the same mechanisms as the tongue, with potential effects on gut hormone release. We investigated whether the human gut responds in the same way to AS and carbohydrate sugars, which are perceived by lingual taste as equisweet. We focused on the secretion of gastrointestinal (GI) satiety peptides in relation to appetite perception. We performed a placebo-controlled, double-blind, six-way, cross-over trial including twelve healthy subjects. On separate days, each subject received an intragastric infusion of glucose, fructose or an AS (aspartame, acesulfame K and sucralose) dissolved in 250 ml of water or water only (control). In a second part, four subjects received an intragastric infusion of the non-sweet, non-metabolisable sugar analogue 2-deoxy-d-glucose. Glucose stimulated GLP-1 (P = 0·002) and peptide tyrosine tyrosine (PYY; P = 0·046) secretion and reduced fasting plasma ghrelin (P = 0·046), whereas fructose was less effective. Both carbohydrate sugars increased satiety and fullness (albeit not significantly) compared with water. In contrast, equisweet loads of AS did not affect gastrointestinal peptide secretion with minimal effects on appetite. 2-Deoxy-d-glucose increased hunger ratings, however, with no effects on GLP-1, PYY or ghrelin. Our data demonstrate that the secretion of GLP-1, PYY and ghrelin depends on more than the detection of (1) sweetness or (2) the structural analogy to glucose.

Type
Full Papers
Copyright
Copyright © The Authors 2011

There is strong evidence that taste-signalling mechanisms identified in the oral epithelium also operate in the gut. It has been suggested that open-type enteroendocrine cells directly sense nutrients via G-protein-coupled receptors to modulate the secretion of gastrointestinal (GI) peptides(Reference Theodorakis, Carlson and Michopoulos1Reference Dyer, Salmon and Zibrik5). One example supporting this idea is that a much greater insulin response occurs after an oral glucose load, i.e. after direct contact of glucose with the intestinal lumen, than after intravenous injection of an identical glucose load(Reference Creutzfeldt6). The so-called ‘incretin effect’ is attributed to glucagon-like peptide-1 (GLP-1) and glucose-dependent insulinotropic peptide (GIP); both are released from enteroendocrine K and L cells in the proximal and distal gut. In addition to this incretin effect, GLP-1 and other GI peptides including cholecystokinin and peptide tyrosine tyrosine (PYY) delay gastric emptying and dose-dependently reduce food intake in animals and humans(Reference Woods7Reference Gutzwiller, Goke and Drewe9). For GLP-1, long-acting analogues are already clinically available (exenatide and liraglutide) for the treatment of type 2 diabetes mellitus. In some of these studies, it has been shown that subcutaneous injections of exenatide, a stable GLP-1 receptor agonist, to patients with type 2 diabetes are associated with a gradual and linear weight loss with no signs of impaired efficacy with time(Reference Schmitz10). The question, therefore, is whether it is possible to increase the secretion of these peptides from endogenous stores using specific stimuli.

In this regard, Jang et al. (Reference Jang, Kokrashvili and Theodorakis2) demonstrated that glucose-stimulated GLP-1 and GIP secretion is impaired in knockout mice lacking α-gustducin (a taste-specific G-protein subunit) or T1R3 (part of the sweet-taste receptor heterodimer T1R2/T1R3)(Reference Jang, Kokrashvili and Theodorakis2). In addition, in vitro, both carbohydrate sugars and artificial sweeteners (AS) were capable of stimulating GLP-1 and GIP release from enteroendocrine cell lines (GLUTag, NCI-H716)(Reference Jang, Kokrashvili and Theodorakis2, Reference Margolskee, Dyer and Kokrashvili4). It is, therefore, currently concluded that the gut directly senses glucose or other sweet compounds by sweet-taste G-protein-coupled receptors and gustducin and that this leads to GLP-1 release from enteroendocrine cells(Reference Kokrashvili, Mosinger and Margolskee11). We, therefore, sought to investigate whether carbohydrate sugars and AS (aspartame, acesulfame K and sucralose), which are perceived by lingual taste as equisweet, have equivalent effects on the release of GI peptides and appetite perception in human subjects.

Subjects and methods

Subjects

The study included twelve healthy, non-smoking, volunteers (six males and six females, mean age 23·3 (sem 0·7; range 19–29) years). Body weight of all subjects was in the normal range for age, sex and height and stable for at least 3 months (mean BMI 23·0 (sem 0·5; range 20·5–24·7) kg/m2). The criteria for exclusion were smoking, substance abuse, chronic medical or psychiatric illness, and any abnormalities detected on physical examination or screening blood tests. None of the subjects had a history of food allergies or dietary restrictions. The study was conducted according to the guidelines laid down in the Declaration of Helsinki, and all procedures involving human subjects were approved by the State Ethical Committee of Basel. Written informed consent was obtained from all subjects.

Study design

Administration of artificial sweeteners or carbohydrate sugars

The study was performed as a randomised, placebo-controlled, double-blind, six-way cross-over trial. The six treatments were separated by at least 3–5 d. Subjects were instructed to abstain from alcohol, caffeine and strenuous exercise for 24 h before each treatment. On each test day, subjects reported to the research unit at 08.00 hours after a 10 h overnight fast. After arrival at the research unit, a radio-opaque polyvinyl feeding tube (external diameter 8 French) was inserted into the stomach through an anaesthetised nostril. Its intragastric position was confirmed by rapid injection of 10 ml of air and auscultation of the upper abdomen. The feeding tube was firmly attached to the skin behind the ear to prevent further progression during the treatment. An intravenous cannula was inserted into a forearm vein for blood sample collection. A baseline blood sample was taken, before each subject received an intragastric infusion of 250 ml of the test solution over 2 min (t = 0–2 min). Solutions were equated for sweetness intensity with each other in preliminary psychophysical tests. Healthy volunteers rated the sweetness of different test solutions and equated them with a standard 50 g of glucose solution (positive control). The results largely confirmed previous estimates by Rogers et al. (Reference Rogers, Carlyle and Hill12) and Bellisle & Drewnowski(Reference Bellisle and Drewnowski13), and the concentrations were comparable with the amounts found in commercially available beverages and soft drinks (Table 1).

Table 1 Nutritional composition/sweetening power of the test solutions

CHO, carbohydrates.

* Amount of each sweetener was dissolved in 250 ml of tap water.

The control solution was 250 ml of tap water only. Solutions were freshly prepared each morning of the study and were at room temperature when administered. The feeding tube was removed directly after the infusion was completed, and blood was drawn at regular time intervals at 5, 10, 15, 20, 30, 45, 60, 75, 90 and 120 min. Blood samples were collected on ice into tubes containing EDTA (6 μmol/l), aprotinin (500 kIU/l; 0·07 mg aprotinin/ml blood) and a dipeptidyl peptidase IV inhibitor. After centrifugation at 4°C, plasma samples were processed into different aliquots and kept frozen at − 70°C until analysis. The appetite profile (hunger, satiety and fullness) was assessed using visual analogue scales, 100 mm (or 10 cm) in length with words anchored at each end, expressing the most positive and most negative rating(Reference Flint, Raben and Blundell14, Reference Raben, Tagliabue and Astrup15). For example, a score of 0 for hunger indicated that the subject was not hungry at all, 2 indicated slightly hungry, 5 indicated moderately hungry, 8 indicated very hungry and 10 indicated absolutely ravenous. Subjects had no exposure to food cues during evaluation, and they were allowed to talk, relax and read with the exception that they could not discuss or compare their ratings. Vital signs (blood pressure and heart rate) were continuously measured while subjects were sitting in a comfortable armchair.

Administration of 2-deoxy-d-glucose

In an additional experiment, four subjects (two males and two females) received an intragastric infusion of 2-deoxy-d-glucose (2DG), a non-sweet, non-metabolisable structural analogue of glucose. In total, 3·75 g of 2DG were dissolved in 250 ml of tap water; the same experimental set-up was used, and the effects on GI peptide secretion, appetite and glucose homeostasis were compared with the data of the first experiment. Blood samples were collected before and 30, 60 and 120 min after intragastric infusion. The appetite profile was assessed using visual analogue scales, and vital signs were continuously measured.

Laboratory analysis

Active GLP-1 was measured, as has been described recently(Reference Steinert, Poller and Castelli16), using a commercially available ELISA kit (Linco Research, Inc., St Charles, MO, USA). This kit is for non-radioactive quantification of biologically active forms of GLP-1 (i.e. 7–36 amide and 7–37) in the plasma and other biological media. Total PYY, total ghrelin, insulin and glucagon were measured with commercially available RIA kits (Linco Research, Inc.; Cisbio International, Bagnols, France; Siemens Medical Solution Diagnostics, Los Angeles, CA, USA); the methods have been described recently in more detail(Reference Steinert, Poller and Castelli16). Blood glucose was measured using the glucose oxidase method.

Materials

Glucose monohydrate and fructose were purchased at Hänseler (Herisau, Switzerland); aspartame was purchased at Fagron (Barsbuettel, Germany); sucralose and 2DG were purchased at Sigma-Aldrich (Buchs, Switzerland). Acesulfame K was a friendly gift of Merisant Company (Neuchatel, Switzerland).

Statistical analysis

Descriptive statistics was used for demographic variables such as age, weight, height and BMI. Individual hormone concentrations v. time data were used to obtain GLP-1, PYY and ghrelin metrics, including maximum/minimum plasma concentrations (C max/C min), the time of maximal/minimal peptide occurrence (T max/T min) and the area under the plasma concentration–time curve (AUC) calculated by the trapezoidal method. Differences between water and either carbohydrate sugars or AS were assessed using the non-parametric Friedman test due to non-normal data distribution. In the case of significant differences, pairwise comparison was performed using the Wilcoxon signed-rank test, followed by Bonferroni's correction to account for multiple comparisons. Visual analogue scales were analysed by calculating return to baseline values (interception with the y-axis) using linear interpolation; differences between water v. single treatment groups were assessed using the non-parametric Friedman test due to distinct data variation and non-normal distribution. All statistical analyses were done using SPSS for Windows software (version 15.0; SPSS, Chicago, IL, USA). Differences were considered to be significant with P < 0·05. Data are presented as means with their standard errors.

Results

Administration of artificial sweeteners or carbohydrate sugars

Plasma glucagon-like peptide-1

A marked increase in plasma GLP-1 was seen after the glucose load, with C max = 9·7 (sem 3·2) pmol/l at 22·9 (sem 2·7) min. The AUC was significantly increased (P = 0·002) compared with water. The equisweet load of fructose only slightly elevated plasma GLP-1 concentrations, with C max = 2·8 (sem 0·5) pmol/l at 20·8 (sem 4·1) min; however, the AUC was not significantly increased compared to water. No increase in plasma GLP-1 concentrations was seen after the administration of sucralose, aspartame or acesulfame K, and no significant difference was observed compared with water (Fig. 1(a) and Table 2).

Fig. 1 (a) Glucagon-like peptide-1 (GLP-1), (b) peptide tyrosine tyrosine (PYY) and (c) ghrelin release in response to an intragastric load of water (–●–) or carbohydrate sugars (glucose (–○–) and fructose (–△–)) or artificial sweeteners (aspartame (…◇…), sucralose (…□…) or acesulfame K (…▽…)). Values are means, with their standard errors represented by error bars.

Table 2 Secretion of glucagon-like peptide-1 (GLP-1), peptide tyrosine tyrosine (PYY) and ghrelin in response to carbohydrate sugars or artificial sweeteners

(Mean values with their standard errors and medians)

AUC, area under the concentration–time curve; C max, maximum peak plasma concentration; T max, time to maximum peak plasma concentration; C min, minimum peak plasma concentration; T min, time to minimum peak plasma concentration; NA, not applicable.

Mean values were significantly different compared with those of water (P < 0·05).

Plasma peptide tyrosine tyrosine

A similar trend was observed for plasma PYY concentrations with elevated concentrations after glucose (C max = 187·4 (sem 32·9) pg/l at 27·9 (sem 3·7) min; P = 0·005) and to a lesser extent after fructose administration (C max = 154·8 (sem 13·0) pg/l at 37·9 (sem 6·6) min; P = 0·004). The AUC was, however, only significantly increased after the glucose load (P = 0·046). No effect was observed with fructose and AS (Fig. 1(b) and Table 2).

Plasma ghrelin

After the glucose and fructose load, fasting plasma ghrelin concentrations were reduced; a significant reduction in the AUC was only observed for glucose (P = 0·046). In contrast, sucralose, aspartame or acesulfame K did not affect plasma ghrelin concentrations (Fig. 1(c) and Table 2).

Blood glucose

After the intragastric infusion of glucose, blood glucose concentrations were significantly increased from 5·0 (sem 0·1) mmol/l at baseline to 8·6 (sem 0·4) mmol/l at 30·0 (sem 4·6) min (P = 0·002) compared to water (Fig. 2(a)). Thereafter, blood glucose concentrations decreased progressively and fell below baseline at 90 min. With fructose administration, blood glucose concentrations were slightly elevated to 5·7 (sem 0·2) mmol/l at 24·2 (sem 4·2) min (P = 0·047) (Fig. 2(a)). The AS did not affect blood glucose concentrations.

Fig. 2 (a) Blood glucose and (b) plasma insulin concentrations in response to an intragastric load of water (–●–) or carbohydrate sugars (glucose (–○–), fructose (–△–)) or artificial sweeteners (aspartame (…◇…), sucralose (…□…) or acesulfame K (…▽…)). Values are means, with their standard errors represented by error bars.

Plasma insulin

Plasma insulin after glucose (C max = 127·4 (sem 19·2) μU/ml; P = 0·002) and fructose (C max = 32·3 (sem 2·0) μU/ml; P = 0·002) administration significantly increased compared with water (Fig. 2(b)). No significant insulin response was observed when sucralose, aspartame or acesulfame K was administered.

Plasma glucagon

Glucagon secretion was slightly suppressed when glucose was administered; the effect was albeit not significant. No effect on plasma glucagon was detected with fructose and AS (data not shown).

Appetite profile

The appetite profile revealed that the (energy-containing) carbohydrate sugar loads induced the longest-lasting increase in fullness ratings above baseline, which was prolonged for fructose 108·4 (sem 6·1) min and for glucose 90·5 (sem 11·2) min. In contrast, water (74·0 (sem 13·4) min) and the AS acesulfame K (65·6 (sem 15·8) min), aspartame (83·1 (sem 15·9) min) and sucralose (65·3 (sem 14·1) min) induced shorter augmented fullness above initial ratings. However, due to the large data variability, these differences did not reach statistical significance (Fig. 3(c) and Table 3). Satiety and hunger ratings showed similar trends, however, with generally smaller differences. Overall, the AS increased satiety and fullness and reduced hunger ratings to an amount that was intermediate between the carbohydrate sugars and the water control.

Fig. 3 Effect of the intragastric infusion of water (–●–) or carbohydrate sugars (glucose (–○–) and fructose (–△–)) or artificial sweeteners (aspartame (…◇…), sucralose (…□…) or acesulfame K (…▽…)) on (a) hunger, (b) satiety and (c) fullness ratings shown as mean changes from the initial rating. Values are means, with their standard errors represented by error bars. VAS, visual analogue scale.

Table 3 Appetite profile expressed as return to baseline values (in min) after intragastric loads of water, carbohydrate sugars or artificial sweeteners

(Mean values with their standard errors and medians)

No significant differences were detected compared with water.

Side effects

No side effects were reported when glucose, fructose or the AS was administered.

Administration of 2-deoxy-d-glucose

Plasma glucagon-like peptide-1, peptide tyrosine tyrosine and ghrelin

Administration of 2DG did not stimulate the secretion of GLP-1 and PYY; also circulating ghrelin was not affected (data not shown).

Plasma glucose, glucagon and insulin

2DG administration markedly increased blood glucose from baseline concentrations of (5·5 (sem 0·2) mmol/l) to peak concentrations of 11·2 (sem 1·3) mmol/l at 120 min. Minor effects were observed for plasma insulin with a small increase at 120 min. Plasma glucagon appeared to slightly increase at 60 min compared with water and glucose loads (Fig. 4(a)–(c)). Due to the small number of subjects, no formal statistical comparison was performed.

Fig. 4 Effect of the intragastric infusion of 2-deoxy-d-glucose (–□–) on (a) blood glucose, (b) plasma insulin, (c) plasma glucagon and (d) hunger ratings, (e) fullness ratings and (f) satiety ratings in comparison to intragastric water (–●–) and glucose (–○–) loads. Values are means, with their standard errors represented by error bars. VAS, visual analogue scale.

Appetite profile

After administration of 2DG, hunger ratings were clearly increased compared to water, in contrast, glucose visibly reduced hunger ratings from 30 to 120 min. Feelings of fullness and satiety showed similar trends, with glucose triggering increased fullness and satiety, whereas 2DG clearly reduced fullness and satiety ratings compared to water (Fig. 4(d)–(f)). Due to the small number of subjects, no formal statistical comparison was performed.

Side effects

No nausea or abdominal discomfort was reported when 2DG was administered; also blood pressure and heart rate were not significantly affected. However, between 30 and 120 min, all subjects reported distinct feelings of warmth and mild dizziness; two subjects sweated visibly.

Discussion

GLP-1 and PYY have important metabolic functions; both have been shown to reduce food intake in humans(Reference Cummings and Overduin17). GLP-1 also directly stimulates pancreatic β-cells; the so-called incretin effect accounts for approximately 50–70 % of the total insulin secretion after a meal(Reference Vilsbøll, Krarup and Madsbad18). In addition, obese subjects have attenuated plasma concentrations of both peptides, suggesting major changes in regulatory functions(Reference le Roux, Batterham and Aylwin19, Reference Verdich, Toubro and Buemann20). For GLP-1, long-acting analogues are already clinically available (exenatide and liraglutide) for the treatment of type 2 diabetes and, interestingly, subcutaneous injections of exenatide to patients with type 2 diabetes are associated with a gradual and linear weight loss with no signs of impaired efficacy with time. In view of this great therapeutic success, the focus is now shifted towards intestinal endocrine cells that naturally produce GLP-1 and PYY. The question is whether it is possible to increase the secretion of these peptides from endogenous stores using specific stimuli. However, mechanisms by which carbohydrates, fats and proteins in the gut lumen stimulate the release of GI peptides from enteroendocrine cells are only insufficiently understood. Regarding the detection of carbohydrates in the gut, sweet taste receptors (T1R2+T1R3), α-gustducin, and several other proteins comprising a full signalling machinery similar to that found in the mouth have been identified in enteroendocrine cells of the rodent(Reference Höfer, Püschel and Drenckhahn21) and human small intestine and colon(Reference Jang, Kokrashvili and Theodorakis2, Reference Rozengurt, Wu and Chen22). Several studies have shown co-localisation of taste receptors with GLP-1, GIP, PYY and cholecystokinin(Reference Jang, Kokrashvili and Theodorakis2, Reference Rozengurt and Sternini3, Reference Rozengurt, Wu and Chen22). In addition, studies in mice using knockout models for α-gustducin (α-gust − / − ) or T1R3 (T1R3 − / − ) have revealed impaired glucose-stimulated incretin secretion(Reference Jang, Kokrashvili and Theodorakis2). In vitro, both carbohydrate sugars and the AS sucralose were capable of stimulating GLP-1 and GIP release from enteroendocrine cell lines (GLUTag, NCI-H716); more importantly, lactisole, a sweet receptor antagonist, blocked the secretion of GLP-1(Reference Jang, Kokrashvili and Theodorakis2, Reference Margolskee, Dyer and Kokrashvili4). Based on these studies, it has been suggested that the gut directly senses glucose or other sweet compounds by taste-signalling elements expressed in L cells and that this leads to GLP-1 release from these same cells(Reference Kokrashvili, Mosinger and Margolskee11).

Here, we have investigated in healthy human subjects whether sweetness-matched carbohydrate sugars and AS have equivalent effects on the release of GI satiety peptides, glucose homeostasis and appetite feelings. The data reveal that mainly glucose, and to a small extent fructose, stimulated GLP-1 and PYY release and reduced ghrelin secretion; both also affected appetite with (albeit not significantly) increased satiety and fullness compared with water. The equisweet AS loads did not affect GI peptide secretion with minimal effects on appetite compared with water. These findings correspond to human studies also reporting a lack of effect of AS on incretin release, plasma glucose, C-peptide and gastric emptying(Reference Gregersen, Jeppesen and Holst23Reference Mezitis, Maggio and Koch25); however, the data are in contrast to the above-mentioned in vitro studies. This may be due to the simple fact that cell culture-based in vitro experiments do only model human physiology, whereas in vivo, the regulatory interface of the GI tract is much more complex and modulated by multiple homeostatic and non-homeostatic factors. In addition, studies in mice and rats have indicated that the majority of incretin-expressing cells in the duodenum and jejunum do not co-localise with α-gustducin(Reference Sutherland, Young and Cooper26, Reference Fujita, Wideman and Speck27), suggesting that the taste receptor pathway is not the only one involved in signal transduction and subsequent GI peptide secretion. Other, non-taste receptor-mediated mechanisms might be involved in the induction of GI peptide release. Several studies have suggested that the secretion of incretins is also dependent on actively absorbed carbohydrate sugars: in rats, glucose-stimulated GIP secretion was markedly suppressed not only by gymnemic acid (a potent blocker of sweet taste) but also by phlorizin, which inhibits the active transport of glucose by the Na-dependent glucose co-transporter 1(Reference Fushiki, Kojima and Imoto28). In addition, in vitro studies using the enteroendocrine GLUTag cell line have shown that GLP-1 release was impaired when glucose absorption was blocked with phlorizin(Reference Gribble, Williams and Simpson29). Further studies by Ritzel et al. (Reference Ritzel, Fromme and Ottleben30) have shown that when the non-sweet, non-metabolisable sugar-analogues, 3-O-methylglucose (absorbed via passive and active glucose transport systems) and 2DG (absorbed only via passive glucose transport systems) were perfused into the rat ileum, 3-O-methylglucose induced the secretion of GLP-1, whereas 2DG did not, suggesting that the release of GLP-1 is independent of intracellular metabolism but dependent on active cellular uptake.

In this regard, we performed an experiment to test whether an intragastric load of 2DG affects GLP-1, PYY or ghrelin secretion. The data show that 2DG does not affect peptide secretion, confirming previous animal and in vitro data for humans and extending the findings to PYY and ghrelin secretion. 2DG is known to reduce intracellular glucose utilisation by competitively inhibiting hexokinase activity and glucose membrane carrier systems. The resulting intracellular glucoprivation induces counter-regulatory mechanisms leading to hyperglycaemia(Reference Rezek and Kroeger31, Reference Welle, Thompson and Campbell32). The almost linear rise in blood glucose concentrations in the present study reflects these metabolic effects; although blood glucose concentrations are high, cellular utilisation of glucose at cerebral and peripheral glucosensitive sites did not occur. The observed side effects (heat flushes and sweating) further suggest sympathetic activation. In parallel, we observed a marked increase in hunger feelings. 2DG has been demonstrated to augment food intake and thirst in mammalian species including rats, pigs, monkeys and humans due to the functional hypoglycaemic state(Reference Welle, Thompson and Campbell32). Our data, therefore, support the basic concept of decreasing blood glucose as a metabolic correlate of hunger, namely ‘the glucostatic theory’(Reference Mayer33). As mentioned above, the increase in hunger was, however, not reflected by increased plasma ghrelin concentrations or alterations in plasma GLP-1 or PYY.

Fructose infusions (25 g) did not affect GLP-1, PYY and ghrelin plasma concentrations to the same extent as the equisweet loads of glucose (50 g); however, fructose induced plasma levels much closer to those observed after glucose infusions than the AS. It is arguable that the different molar load may have affected peptide secretion differentially; however, previous human data document that GLP-1 secretion in response to equal doses (75 g) of either fructose or glucose is less for fructose(Reference Kong, Chapman and Goble34). The relationship between the molecular structure of carbohydrate sugars and their ability to stimulate the release of GLP-1 or GIP has been investigated in detail(Reference Shima, Suda and Nishimoto35, Reference Sirinek, Levine and O'Dorisio36). Sirinek et al. (Reference Sirinek, Levine and O'Dorisio36) demonstrated that only glucose and, to a lesser extent, galactose (C-4 epimer), but not fructose (C-2 keto sugar), mannose (C-2 epimer) or sorbitol (reduced alcohol of glucose), can stimulate the release of GIP. Based on these observations, it has been proposed that a special sugar sensor with specific steric requirements is necessary to directly stimulate incretin release(Reference Sirinek, Levine and O'Dorisio36).

The finding that AS have less effect on satiety, fullness and hunger ratings compared with carbohydrate sugars confirms the lack of post-ingestive mechanisms and supports recent findings in rats, suggesting that the consumption of AS might lead to increased body weight and obesity(Reference Swithers and Davidson37). However, correlations between appetite ratings and amount of food consumed are quite small, and we did not directly measure food intake. The observed satiating effects of fructose in comparison with the effects of glucose, together with the rather small effects of fructose on peptide levels, suggest that additional mechanisms must operate to terminate a meal and/or to determine inter-meal satiety. Initial studies by Blundell et al. (Reference Foltin, Fischman and Moran38Reference Rogers and Blundell40) and a few other studies have reported energy compensation in humans when energy-containing sugars were replaced by AS. A large number of subsequent studies could, however, not show an effect of AS on appetite and food intake(Reference Canty and Chan41Reference Drewnowski, Massien and Louis-Sylvestre43).

Conclusions

Sweet taste detection via T1R2+T1R3 is suggested to be one potential mechanism by which glucose is sensed in gut enteroendocrine cells to trigger peptide secretion. Here, we demonstrate that equisweet solutions of either glucose, fructose or AS have different effects on gut peptide secretion: only glucose potently stimulates the secretion of GLP-1 and PYY and decreases ghrelin; in contrast, fructose is much less effective and AS have no effect. We infer from these observations that sweetness per se is not sufficient to stimulate the secretion of these peptides in humans. Additional chemosensory mechanisms directed towards the structural integrity of the glucose molecule (as one of the major fuels for the body) must exist including active transport systems. Finally, potential energy-sensing mechanisms or energy thresholds might exist for the secretion of GLP-1 and PYY, although it is unlikely that the release is directly related to the energetic load in a dose–response manner.

Our experimental approach was based on equisweet solutions, so we cannot exclude that different molar loads and osmolarities would have affected peptide secretion differentially. Also, we cannot rule out that under certain conditions, AS may enhance GLP-1 release when mixed with glucose, as it has been suggested that AS might indirectly contribute to GIP and GLP-1 release by modulating the expression of Na-dependent glucose co-transporter 1. These considerations are questions for future research; they are based, at least in part, on the present findings showing that the stimulation of gut sweet taste receptors per se is not sufficient to produce relevant regulatory peptide responses.

Acknowledgements

We thank the team of the Clinical Research Center Mrs Claudia Bläsi, Sylvia Ketterer and Gerdien Gamboni for their expert technical assistance. The study was supported by a grant of the Swiss National Science Foundation (grant no. 320000-118330). The authors have no conflict of interest. R. E. S. and C. B. designed the research; F. F. and A. T. conducted the research; R. E. S. and J. D. performed statistical analysis; R. E. S. and C. B. wrote the manuscript. C. B. had primary responsibility for the final content. All authors read and approved the final manuscript.

References

1Theodorakis, M, Carlson, O, Michopoulos, S, et al. (2006) Human duodenal enteroendocrine cells: source of both incretin peptides, GLP-1 and GIP. Am J Physiol Endocrinol Metab 290, E550E559.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
2Jang, H, Kokrashvili, Z, Theodorakis, M, et al. (2007) Gut-expressed gustducin and taste receptors regulate secretion of glucagon-like peptide-1. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A 104, 1506915074.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
3Rozengurt, E & Sternini, C (2007) Taste receptor signaling in the mammalian gut. Curr Opin Pharmacol 7, 557562.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
4Margolskee, R, Dyer, J, Kokrashvili, Z, et al. (2007) T1R3 and gustducin in gut sense sugars to regulate expression of Na+-glucose cotransporter 1. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A 104, 1507515080.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
5Dyer, J, Salmon, K, Zibrik, L, et al. (2005) Expression of sweet taste receptors of the T1R family in the intestinal tract and enteroendocrine cells. Biochem Soc Trans 33, 302305.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
6Creutzfeldt, W (1980) Insulinotropic factors of the gut – the broadening incretin concept. Gastroenterology 78, 16311632.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
7Woods, S (2004) Gastrointestinal satiety signals I. An overview of gastrointestinal signals that influence food intake. Am J Physiol Gastrointest Liver Physiol 286, G7G13.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
8Degen, L, Oesch, S, Casanova, M, et al. (2005) Effect of peptide YY3-36 on food intake in humans. Gastroenterology 129, 14301436.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
9Gutzwiller, JP, Goke, B, Drewe, J, et al. (1999) Glucagon-like peptide-1: a potent regulator of food intake in humans. Gut 44, 8186.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
10Schmitz, O (2008) The GLP-1 concept in the treatment of type 2 diabetes – still standing at the gate of dawn? J Clin Endocrinol Metab 93, 375377.Google Scholar
11Kokrashvili, Z, Mosinger, B & Margolskee, R (2009) T1r3 and alpha-gustducin in gut regulate secretion of glucagon-like peptide-1. Ann N Y Acad Sci 1170, 9194.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
12Rogers, P, Carlyle, J, Hill, A, et al. (1988) Uncoupling sweet taste and calories: comparison of the effects of glucose and three intense sweeteners on hunger and food intake. Physiol Behav 43, 547552.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
13Bellisle, F & Drewnowski, A (2007) Intense sweeteners, energy intake and the control of body weight. Eur J Clin Nutr 61, 691700.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
14Flint, A, Raben, A, Blundell, JE, et al. (2000) Reproducibility, power and validity of visual analogue scales in assessment of appetite sensations in single test meal studies. Int J Obes Relat Metab Disord 24, 3848.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
15Raben, A, Tagliabue, A & Astrup, A (1995) The reproducibility of subjective appetite scores. Br J Nutr 73, 517530.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
16Steinert, RE, Poller, B, Castelli, MC, et al. (2009) Orally administered glucagon-like peptide-1 affects glucose homeostasis following an oral glucose tolerance test in healthy male subjects. Clin Pharmacol Ther 86, 644650.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
17Cummings, D & Overduin, J (2007) Gastrointestinal regulation of food intake. J Clin Invest 117, 1323.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
18Vilsbøll, T, Krarup, T, Madsbad, S, et al. (2001) No reactive hypoglycaemia in Type 2 diabetic patients after subcutaneous administration of GLP-1 and intravenous glucose. Diabet Med 18, 144149.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
19le Roux, CW, Batterham, RL, Aylwin, SJ, et al. (2006) Attenuated peptide YY release in obese subjects is associated with reduced satiety. Endocrinology 147, 38.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
20Verdich, C, Toubro, S, Buemann, B, et al. (2001) The role of postprandial releases of insulin and incretin hormones in meal-induced satiety – effect of obesity and weight reduction. Int J Obes Relat Metab Disord 25, 12061214.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
21Höfer, D, Püschel, B & Drenckhahn, D (1996) Taste receptor-like cells in the rat gut identified by expression of alpha-gustducin. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A 93, 66316634.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
22Rozengurt, N, Wu, S, Chen, M, et al. (2006) Colocalization of the alpha-subunit of gustducin with PYY and GLP-1 in L cells of human colon. Am J Physiol Gastrointest Liver Physiol 291, G792G802.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
23Gregersen, S, Jeppesen, P, Holst, J, et al. (2004) Antihyperglycemic effects of stevioside in type 2 diabetic subjects. Metabolism 53, 7376.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
24Ma, J, Bellon, M, Wishart, J, et al. (2009) Effect of the artificial sweetener, sucralose, on gastric emptying and incretin hormone release in healthy subjects. Am J Physiol Gastrointest Liver Physiol 296, G735G739.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
25Mezitis, N, Maggio, C, Koch, P, et al. (1996) Glycemic effect of a single high oral dose of the novel sweetener sucralose in patients with diabetes. Diabetes Care 19, 10041005.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
26Sutherland, K, Young, R, Cooper, N, et al. (2007) Phenotypic characterization of taste cells of the mouse small intestine. Am J Physiol Gastrointest Liver Physiol 292, G1420G1428.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
27Fujita, Y, Wideman, R, Speck, M, et al. (2009) Incretin release from gut is acutely enhanced by sugar but not by sweeteners in vivo. Am J Physiol Endocrinol Metab 296, E473E479.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
28Fushiki, T, Kojima, A, Imoto, T, et al. (1992) An extract of Gymnema sylvestre leaves and purified gymnemic acid inhibits glucose-stimulated gastric inhibitory peptide secretion in rats. J Nutr 122, 23672373.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
29Gribble, F, Williams, L, Simpson, A, et al. (2003) A novel glucose-sensing mechanism contributing to glucagon-like peptide-1 secretion from the GLUTag cell line. Diabetes 52, 11471154.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
30Ritzel, U, Fromme, A, Ottleben, M, et al. (1997) Release of glucagon-like peptide-1 (GLP-1) by carbohydrates in the perfused rat ileum. Acta Diabetol 34, 1821.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
31Rezek, M & Kroeger, E (1976) Glucose antimetabolites and hunger (theoretical article). J Nutr 106, 143157.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
32Welle, S, Thompson, D, Campbell, R, et al. (1980) Increased hunger and thirst during glucoprivation in humans. Physiol Behav 25, 397403.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
33Mayer, J (1953) Glucostatic mechanism of regulation of food intake. N Engl J Med 249, 1316.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
34Kong, M, Chapman, I, Goble, E, et al. (1999) Effects of oral fructose and glucose on plasma GLP-1 and appetite in normal subjects. Peptides 20, 545551.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
35Shima, K, Suda, T, Nishimoto, K, et al. (1990) Relationship between molecular structures of sugars and their ability to stimulate the release of glucagon-like peptide-1 from canine ileal loops. Acta Endocrinol (Copenh) 123, 464470.Google ScholarPubMed
36Sirinek, K, Levine, B, O'Dorisio, T, et al. (1983) Gastric inhibitory polypeptide (GIP) release by actively transported, structurally similar carbohydrates. Proc Soc Exp Biol Med 173, 379385.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
37Swithers, S & Davidson, T (2008) A role for sweet taste: calorie predictive relations in energy regulation by rats. Behav Neurosci 122, 161173.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
38Foltin, R, Fischman, M, Moran, T, et al. (1990) Caloric compensation for lunches varying in fat and carbohydrate content by humans in a residential laboratory. Am J Clin Nutr 52, 969980.Google Scholar
39Blundell, J & Hill, A (1986) Paradoxical effects of an intense sweetener (aspartame) on appetite. Lancet 1, 10921093.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
40Rogers, P & Blundell, J (1989) Separating the actions of sweetness and calories: effects of saccharin and carbohydrates on hunger and food intake in human subjects. Physiol Behav 45, 10931099.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
41Canty, D & Chan, M (1991) Effects of consumption of caloric vs noncaloric sweet drinks on indices of hunger and food consumption in normal adults. Am J Clin Nutr 53, 11591164.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
42Rolls, B, Kim, S & Fedoroff, I (1990) Effects of drinks sweetened with sucrose or aspartame on hunger, thirst and food intake in men. Physiol Behav 48, 1926.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
43Drewnowski, A, Massien, C, Louis-Sylvestre, J, et al. (1994) Comparing the effects of aspartame and sucrose on motivational ratings, taste preferences, and energy intakes in humans. Am J Clin Nutr 59, 338345.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Figure 0

Table 1 Nutritional composition/sweetening power of the test solutions

Figure 1

Fig. 1 (a) Glucagon-like peptide-1 (GLP-1), (b) peptide tyrosine tyrosine (PYY) and (c) ghrelin release in response to an intragastric load of water (–●–) or carbohydrate sugars (glucose (–○–) and fructose (–△–)) or artificial sweeteners (aspartame (…◇…), sucralose (…□…) or acesulfame K (…▽…)). Values are means, with their standard errors represented by error bars.

Figure 2

Table 2 Secretion of glucagon-like peptide-1 (GLP-1), peptide tyrosine tyrosine (PYY) and ghrelin in response to carbohydrate sugars or artificial sweeteners(Mean values with their standard errors and medians)

Figure 3

Fig. 2 (a) Blood glucose and (b) plasma insulin concentrations in response to an intragastric load of water (–●–) or carbohydrate sugars (glucose (–○–), fructose (–△–)) or artificial sweeteners (aspartame (…◇…), sucralose (…□…) or acesulfame K (…▽…)). Values are means, with their standard errors represented by error bars.

Figure 4

Fig. 3 Effect of the intragastric infusion of water (–●–) or carbohydrate sugars (glucose (–○–) and fructose (–△–)) or artificial sweeteners (aspartame (…◇…), sucralose (…□…) or acesulfame K (…▽…)) on (a) hunger, (b) satiety and (c) fullness ratings shown as mean changes from the initial rating. Values are means, with their standard errors represented by error bars. VAS, visual analogue scale.

Figure 5

Table 3 Appetite profile expressed as return to baseline values (in min) after intragastric loads of water, carbohydrate sugars or artificial sweeteners(Mean values with their standard errors and medians)

Figure 6

Fig. 4 Effect of the intragastric infusion of 2-deoxy-d-glucose (–□–) on (a) blood glucose, (b) plasma insulin, (c) plasma glucagon and (d) hunger ratings, (e) fullness ratings and (f) satiety ratings in comparison to intragastric water (–●–) and glucose (–○–) loads. Values are means, with their standard errors represented by error bars. VAS, visual analogue scale.