Hostname: page-component-cd9895bd7-mkpzs Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-12-23T15:30:16.717Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Effectiveness of school-based interventions in Europe to promote healthy nutrition in children and adolescents: systematic review of published and ‘grey’ literature

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  14 January 2010

Eveline Van Cauwenberghe*
Affiliation:
Department of Movement and Sport Sciences, Ghent University, Watersportlaan 2, 9000Ghent, Belgium
Lea Maes
Affiliation:
Department of Public Health, Ghent University, Watersportlaan 2, 9000Ghent, Belgium
Heleen Spittaels
Affiliation:
Department of Movement and Sport Sciences, Ghent University, Watersportlaan 2, 9000Ghent, Belgium
Frank J. van Lenthe
Affiliation:
Department of Public Health, Erasmus University Medical Centre Rotterdam, PO Box 2040, 3000 CARotterdam, The Netherlands
Johannes Brug
Affiliation:
EMGO Institute for Health and Care Research, VU University Medical Centre, Van der Boechorststraat 7, 1081 BTAmsterdam, The Netherlands
Jean-Michel Oppert
Affiliation:
Department of Nutrition, Université Pierre et Marie Curie, Pitié-Salpêtrière Hospital (AP-HP), Human Nutrition Centre Ile-de-France (CRNH-IdF), Paris, France
Ilse De Bourdeaudhuij
Affiliation:
Department of Movement and Sport Sciences, Ghent University, Watersportlaan 2, 9000Ghent, Belgium
*
*Corresponding author: Eveline Van Cauwenberghe, fax +32 9 264 64 84, email [email protected]
Rights & Permissions [Opens in a new window]

Abstract

The objective of the present review was to summarise the existing European published and ‘grey’ literature on the effectiveness of school-based interventions to promote a healthy diet in children (6–12 years old) and adolescents (13–18 years old). Eight electronic databases, websites and contents of key journals were systematically searched, reference lists were screened, and authors and experts in the field were contacted for studies evaluating school-based interventions promoting a healthy diet and aiming at primary prevention of obesity. The studies were included if they were published between 1 January 1990 and 31 December 2007 and reported effects on dietary behaviour or on anthropometrics. Finally, forty-two studies met the inclusion criteria: twenty-nine in children and thirteen in adolescents. In children, strong evidence of effect was found for multicomponent interventions on fruit and vegetable intakes. Limited evidence of effect was found for educational interventions on behaviour, and for environmental interventions on fruit and vegetable intakes. Interventions that specifically targeted children from lower socio-economic status groups showed limited evidence of effect on behaviour. In adolescents, moderate evidence of effect was found for educational interventions on behaviour and limited evidence of effect for multicomponent programmes on behaviour. In children and adolescents, effects on anthropometrics were often not measured, and therefore evidence was lacking or delivered inconclusive evidence. To conclude, evidence was found for the effectiveness of especially multicomponent interventions promoting a healthy diet in school-aged children in European Union countries on self-reported dietary behaviour. Evidence for effectiveness on anthropometrical obesity-related measures is lacking.

Type
Systematic Review
Copyright
Copyright © The Authors 2009

A healthful diet during childhood and adolescence promotes optimal health, growth and cognitive development of the child and adolescent, and may contribute to the prevention of chronic disease in later life(1Reference Luzzi and James5). Evidence suggests that eating habits adopted early in life track to some extent into adulthood, while the transition from childhood into adolescence is often associated with unhealthful dietary changes(Reference Kelder, Perry and Klepp6Reference Lytle, Seifert and Greenstein8). It is therefore important to establish healthful eating behaviours early in life, and specifically focus on the transition from childhood to adolescence. Dietary recommendations for a healthful diet across Europe recommend consumption of at least five portions of fruit and vegetables a day, reduced intakes of saturated fat and salt, and increased consumption of complex carbohydrates and fibre(3, Reference Brug9). However, dietary consumption surveys show that most European children and adolescents do not meet these guidelines(Reference Currie, Roberts and Morgan10Reference Currie, Gabhainn and Godeau14). Recent figures also show alarming and increasing numbers of obese and overweight children and adolescents in Europe, indicating that energy intakes are higher than energy needs(Reference Branca, Nikogosian and Lobstein11). Discussion about how to tackle the epidemic of obesity is currently high on the health policy agenda and effective health promotion remains a key strategy(Reference Branca, Nikogosian and Lobstein11, 15). Therefore, there is a need to develop and implement effective programmes and policies that will result in children and adolescents adopting healthier diets.

Schools are a crucial social environment for children and adolescents and many attempts have been made to utilise this environment to promote healthful behaviours in youth, including healthful eating habits(Reference Summerbell, Waters and Edmunds16Reference Sharma21). School-based interventions have the potential to reach almost 100 % of children of school age of diverse ethnic and socio-economic groups in the European context. Furthermore, in most European Union countries, primary and secondary schools serve at least one meal every school day. Other influencing factors at school for eating behaviours are food and beverages available at school outside meals (e.g. vending machines and school stores) and nutrition education classes. Schools therefore represent an important setting to promote and provide healthy nutrition and nutrition education(1, Reference Branca, Nikogosian and Lobstein11, 15).

Until now, it is unclear how successful school-based efforts have been in improving the dietary habits of young people in Europe. Previous reviews have mostly dealt with the prevention of obesity(Reference Summerbell, Waters and Edmunds16, Reference Doak, Visscher and Renders17, Reference Flodmark, Marcus and Britton19Reference Jaime and Lock25) or focused only on one specific dietary behaviour(Reference French and Stables18, Reference Knai, Pomerleau and Lock26Reference de Sa and Lock29). In addition, these recently published reviews were often limited in scope; only controlled trials(Reference Summerbell, Waters and Edmunds16Reference Sharma21, 24, Reference Knai, Pomerleau and Lock26, Reference Ammerman, Lindquist and Lohr27, Reference de Sa and Lock29) or long-term studies(Reference Summerbell, Waters and Edmunds16, Reference Flodmark, Marcus and Britton19, Reference Sharma20, 24, Reference Knai, Pomerleau and Lock26) were accepted. Such a strong focus on inclusion of only the most rigorous internal validity designs may disregard promising interventions, for which such designs were not possible or inappropriate. Finally, most of the studies included in these reviews were carried out in the United States(Reference Summerbell, Waters and Edmunds16Reference French and Stables18, Reference Sharma20, Reference Shaya, Flores and Gbarayor22Reference de Sa and Lock29), raising questions about the applicability of these results in European countries given the very different school system and school nutrition situation, as well as differences in eating habits and obesity rates(Reference Branca, Nikogosian and Lobstein11, Reference Currie, Gabhainn and Godeau14). This is, to our knowledge, the first review that tries to systematically review the evidence for effectiveness from studies conducted across Europe on school-based healthful diet promotion among children and adolescents on changes in nutrition behaviours and body composition.

Methods

Literature search

The retrieval of published studies for the present review included a structured search in five electronic databases (PubMed, Web of Science, CINAHL, The Cochrane Library and MDConsult) from January 1990 up to and including December 2007. No language restrictions were applied. The search strategy was initially developed in PubMed and adapted for use in other databases. The search was run by one reviewer (E. V. C.) in October 2007 and was rerun in January and finally in June 2008 to be absolutely sure that all studies up to December 2007 were available through the electronic databases. The search strategy was designed to be inclusive and focused on three key elements: population (e.g. children and adolescents); intervention (e.g. school-based); outcome (e.g. diet and nutrition). In addition, reference lists of all retrieved articles and review articles(Reference Summerbell, Waters and Edmunds16Reference Sharma21, Reference Hardeman, Griffin and Johnston23, 24, Reference Knai, Pomerleau and Lock26, Reference Blanchette and Brug28, Reference Brug and van Lenthe30Reference Norman, Zabinski and Adams33) were screened for potentially eligible articles. Furthermore, a number of websites of research groups that conduct and publish systematic reviews of public-health and health promotion interventions were scanned. These strategies were complemented with a comprehensive search of the ‘grey’ literature, i.e. publications not published in indexed peer-reviewed journals and publications in other languages than English. The following electronic databases were searched: SIGLE; Social Care Online; British National Bibliography for Report Literature. Additionally, the supplements of ‘International Journal of Obesity’ and ‘Acta Paediatrica’ were hand searched. Finally, authors of relevant reports, abstracts and non-English articles, derived from the searches detailed earlier, were contacted and asked for additional information about their study. A complete and detailed summary of the search strategies used, including a full list of the search strings for each database, can be found in Web Extra 1 (http://www.hopeproject.eu/index.php?page = documents&documents_map = %2FWP+9+systematic+review%2F).

Selection of studies

To be eligible for inclusion, studies needed to (1) be conducted in European Union countries; (2) target young children (6–18 years old) in a school setting; (3) aim at the primary prevention of obesity and diseases related to obesity in which the main component or one of the components was the promotion of a healthy diet and (4) report effects on dietary behaviour or on anthropometrics. The studies in the present review were, however, not limited to interventions explicitly aiming to contribute to obesity prevention. Rather, all interventions that targeted dietary behaviours that may be associated with obesity risk were included. Furthermore, papers that deal with the implementation, feasibility, applicability or cost-effectiveness of the interventions were accepted for further review. There were no restrictions on study design, study duration, follow-up period, intervention strategies, control condition and on who delivered the intervention. Studies were considered regardless of their design because for public-health purposes, randomised controlled designs, considered to provide the strongest evidence regarding an intervention, are often unachievable and may even be found inappropriate(Reference McQueen and Jones34Reference Kemm37). In addition, it is stated that different types of evidence (i.e. observational, experimental, extrapolated and experience-based sources) are needed to develop effective strategies for public-health interventions(Reference McQueen and Jones34Reference Kemm37). Yet, in the review process, a distinction was made between evidence from stronger and from weaker study designs.

The following exclusion criteria were applied: (1) interventions that were conducted mainly outside the school setting (e.g. community and family); (2) interventions that were not designed for primary prevention (i.e. for the treatment of chronic diseases, aimed at obese children, aimed at treatment or management of eating disorders or aimed at malnourished children); and (3) studies that did not report the effects on dietary behaviour and on anthropometrics.

To identify the relevant studies, one reviewer (E. V. C.) reviewed all titles and abstracts generated from the searches. Articles were rejected on initial screening only if the reviewer could determine from the title and abstract that the article did not meet the inclusion criteria or did meet any of the exclusion criteria. If abstracts were not available or unable to provide sufficient exclusion information, the entire article was retrieved to screen the full text. The evaluation of the full text articles was divided among four reviewers (E. V. C., H. S., I. D. B. and L. M.) to further refine the results using the aforementioned inclusion and exclusion criteria. The decisions were discussed and disagreements between the reviewers were resolved by discussion until consensus was reached.

Quality assessment

A standardised quality assessment tool, the Effective Public Health Practice Project Quality Assessment Tool for Quantitative studies 2003, was used to appraise the methodological rigour of the included studies(Reference Jackson38). The six criteria included for quality assessment were: the extent to which study participants were representative of the target population (i.e. ‘selection bias’); study design (i.e. ‘allocation bias’); control of confounders (i.e. ‘confounders’); whether outcome assessors were blinded (i.e. ‘blinding’); reliability and validity of the data collection tools (i.e. ‘data collection methods’); the withdrawals and dropouts (i.e. ‘withdrawals and dropouts’). Each criterion was rated as strong, moderate or weak, and then summed to obtain an overall score for each study. Studies with at least four criteria rated as strong and with no criteria rated as weak were given an overall rating of ‘strong’. Those studies receiving less than four strong ratings and only one weak rating were given an overall rating of ‘moderate’, and those studies with two or more criteria rated as weak were given an overall study rating of ‘weak’. Any comments on the analyses and on the integrity of the intervention were also collected, but these did not affect the overall rating of quality.

The quality assessment instrument was pilot tested independently by two of the reviewers (E. V. C. and H. S.) on four of the reviewed studies. The reviewers compared their ratings, and where disagreement was noted, discussions ensued until consensus on all ratings was achieved. The quality assessment of the remaining studies was completed by one reviewer (E. V. C.) and discussed with another reviewer (H. S.).

Data extraction

To review the characteristics of the included studies, one reviewer (E. V. C.) extracted detailed information into summary tables. Data extracted included study and intervention characteristics as well as effect indicators. Specific study and intervention characteristics that have been identified previously by health education experts as being crucial for evaluating evidence on public-health interventions were extracted(Reference Rychetnik, Frommer and Hawe36, Reference Jackson39Reference Moher, Jones and Lepage43). The study characteristics included specifics about the study design, participants, context, outcome measures and instruments, and effect and process evaluation variables, and the intervention characteristics included specifics about the intervention components.

Grading of evidence

A rating system of levels of evidence of effect, based on previously used best evidence synthesis, was used to draw conclusions on effectiveness on dietary behaviour and anthropometrics(Reference Engbers, van Poppel and Chin A Paw44Reference van Sluijs, McMinn and Griffin46). Some important adaptations were made to the system because the present review included studies regardless of their design, and a slightly different quality assessment tool was used. The following five levels were distinguished based on the number, design, overall quality and overall effectiveness of studies: (1) strong evidence of effect: (i) at least two (cluster) randomised controlled trials (RCT) of strong quality or (ii) one (cluster) RCT of strong quality and at least two (cluster) RCT of moderate quality. For both situations, consistent results are required; (2) moderate evidence of effect: (i) one (cluster) RCT of moderate quality and at least one (cluster) RCT of weak quality, (ii) one (cluster) RCT of moderate quality and at least one controlled trial of strong quality, (iii) at least three controlled trials of strong quality or (iv) one controlled trial of strong quality and at least three controlled trials of moderate quality. For all situations, consistent results are required; (3) limited evidence of effect: (i) more than one (cluster) RCT of weak quality, (ii) one controlled trial of moderate quality and two controlled trials of weak quality or (iii) two controlled trials of weak quality and at least two before–after, cohort or longitudinal studies. For all situations, consistent results were required; (4) inconclusive evidence of effect: (i) only one study, (ii) multiple before–after, cohort or longitudinal studies or (iii) contradictory results; (5) no evidence of effect: more than one study with consistent results that no significant or relevant results were shown. Results were considered to be consistent if none of the relevant studies pointed in the opposite direction (i.e. a study with a deterioration as overall result) and a maximum 33% of the studies reported mixed results.

The overall result of each study for effectiveness on anthropometrics and dietary behaviour was based on the following system. If at least one finding was significant in the intended direction and no significant findings were found in the opposite direction, it was considered to be an improvement (i.e. ++). For example, if diaries and 24-h recall were used to assess dietary intake and according to the 24-h recall there was a positive significant effect but according to the diaries the intake was unchanged but did not deteriorate significantly, the outcome result for dietary behaviour was identified as an improvement. The same procedure was followed to indicate a deterioration (i.e. −  − ), namely at least one finding was significant in the opposite direction and no significant findings were found in the intended direction. Furthermore, it was stated that the overall result was mixed (i.e. ++/ −  − ) if at least one finding was found in the intended direction and one in the opposite direction. Finally, it was indicated that no effect (i.e. 0) was found if all the findings did not change significantly in any direction.

Data synthesis

Because of the heterogeneity of studies with respect to study design, intervention, participants, measures and outcomes, a meta-analysis was not conducted to estimate a pooled effect size. The present findings, therefore, resulted in a descriptive systematic literature review. In a stratified analysis, we assessed levels of evidence of effect for studies according to outcome measure (i.e. dietary behaviour and anthropometrics), type of intervention (i.e. educational, environmental and multicomponent, i.e. combining education and environmental changes) and target group population (i.e. populations with a low socio-economic background and ethnic minority populations) within each age group (i.e. children and adolescents).

Results

Literature search

The initial database search yielded 8991 publications (Fig. 1). After reviewing the titles or abstracts or both, the total was reduced to 287. Checking the references in these papers and in review articles produced an additional eight papers, and another eleven papers were brought up by manually searching journals and contacting authors. After completely reviewing the 306 articles, 223 publications were excluded because they did not meet one or more of the inclusion criteria. Almost all of the excluded publications were studies conducted outside Europe. Other main reasons for exclusion were that the studies dealt with an irrelevant intervention or that the effects on behaviour and on anthropometrics were not reported. Finally, two interventions were family-based instead of school-based and another two were treatment instead of prevention studies. Thus, fifty-six studies (reported in eighty-three articles) were included; forty-two studies (reported in fifty-three articles) focused only on nutrition, the results of which are presented here. Twenty-nine studies included children and thirteen included adolescents. Of the studies focusing only on nutrition, seven were ‘grey literature’. Two studies in Italian(Reference Agozzino, Esposito and Genovese47, Reference Bonaccorsi, Isola and Tognarelli48), one study in German(Reference Eichhorn, Loss and Nagel49) and one study in French(Reference Borys and Lafay50) were found through PubMed, and the authors were contacted for further information. One study(Reference D'Addesa, Marzi and Sinesio51) was retrieved by hand searching the supplements of the International Journal of Obesity and further information was again delivered by the author. In 2005, the final two studies(Reference Livingstone, McKinley and Robson52, Reference Pearson, Lambert and Barker53) were briefly described in the article of Woolfe et al., obtained through CINAHL, and further information about the two studies was collected by contacting the authors and the Food Standard Agency, London. An extensive table with a full description of the intervention characteristics and results can be found in Web Extra 2 and 3 (http://www.hopeproject.eu/index.php?page = documents&documents_map = %2FWP+9+systematic+review%2F). The studies were organised in the tables according to age group (i.e. children: 6–12 years old and adolescents: 13–18 years old) and the type of intervention (i.e. education-only, environmental or policy-based only or a combination of both: multicomponent). Follow-up periods (i.e. from baseline until follow-up measures) were divided into three categories: short term ( < 3 months), medium term (3–12 months) and long term (>12 months).

Fig. 1 Flow chart of study selection process. PA, physical activity.

Methodological quality

The results of the quality assessment are presented in Web Extra 4 (http://www.hopeproject.eu/index.php?page = documents&documents_map = %2FWP+9+systematic+review%2F). Only five of the forty-two studies were overall rated as strong(Reference James, Thomas and Cavan54Reference Haerens, Deforche and Maes58), eight were rated as moderate(Reference Livingstone, McKinley and Robson52, Reference Bere, Veierod and Klepp59Reference Martens, van Assema and Paulussen65) and twenty-nine were rated as weak. All studies had some methodological weaknesses and none of the included studies fulfilled all the necessary quality criteria. In many studies, selection bias occurred. Only five studies(Reference Bere, Veierod and Bjelland56, Reference Haerens, Deforche and Maes58, Reference Bere, Veierod and Skare60, Reference Bere, Veierod and Bjelland62, Reference Klepp and Wilhelmsen64) received a strong rating on this criterion. More mixed quality assessments were found for the criteria allocation bias (31 % scored weak, 40 % moderate and 29 % strong) and confounders (45 % scored weak, 7 % moderate and 48 % strong). Almost all of the studies used more than one method to measure dietary behaviours. In 76 % of the studies, dietary intake was collected by one of the following instruments: recall of food intake in the past 24 h; food diaries; food frequency checklists. Other methods that were used were self-developed questionnaires (n 9), observations (n 5), weighed measures (n 3) and sales data (n 2). Biomarker assessment, a more objective measurement method, was lacking in all of the studies. Because assessment of nutrition behaviour was self-reported, the blinding criterion, as stated in the dictionary of the tool(Reference Jackson38), was not applicable in all of these cases. All the six studies(Reference Borys and Lafay50, Reference James, Thomas and Cavan54, Reference Angelico, Del Ben and Fabiani66Reference Ask, Hernes and Aarek69) that measured body composition scored weak on blinding. Of all the measurement tools used, 49 % received a weak rating, 19 % a moderate rating and 32 % a strong rating on the criterion data collection methods. About 63 % of the studies reported a dropout between 0 and 40 %, and six studies(Reference Te Velde, Brug and Wind57, Reference Bere, Veierod and Klepp59, Reference Tak, Te Velde and Brug63Reference Martens, van Assema and Paulussen65, Reference Reinaerts, de Nooijer and Candel70) found differences between subjects who dropped out and who participated. Finally, a weak rating on the criterion withdrawals and dropouts was obtained in 43 % of the cases, a moderate rating in 20 % of the cases and a strong rating in 37 % of the cases. Regarding the statistical analyses, for most studies, a power calculation was not reported (60 %), studies failed to apply intention to treat analyses (60 %), and adjustment for clustering of data was lacking when randomisation was carried out at group level (40 %). To conclude, there appeared to be a risk for contamination in some of the included studies (24 %).

General characteristics of the studies

Study characteristics

Forty-two studies were included in the present review, twenty-nine in children and thirteen in adolescents. The study characteristics of each included study can be found separately for children and adolescents in Tables 1 and 2. Most of the studies in children and adolescents were carried out in the United Kingdom. Study population size varied from 40 to 6076 in the studies in children and from 54 to 4020 participants in the studies in adolescents. Three studies in children(Reference Pearson, Lambert and Barker53, Reference Boaz, Ziebland and Wyke71, Reference O'Brien, Roe and Reeves72) and two in adolescents(Reference Hassapidou, Fotiadou and Maglara68, Reference Ask, Hernes and Aarek69) were pilot studies. One-third of the studies in children(Reference Agozzino, Esposito and Genovese47, Reference D'Addesa, Marzi and Sinesio51, Reference Livingstone, McKinley and Robson52, Reference Mangunkusumo, Brug and de Koning55, Reference Bere, Veierod and Bjelland56, Reference Bere, Veierod and Klepp59, Reference Bere, Veierod and Bjelland62, Reference Boaz, Ziebland and Wyke71, Reference Eriksen, Haraldsdottir and Pederson73Reference Wind, Bjelland and Perez-Rodrigo76) and just over half of the studies in adolescents(Reference Eichhorn, Loss and Nagel49, Reference Haerens, Deforche and Maes58, Reference Klepp and Wilhelmsen64, Reference Martens, van Assema and Paulussen77Reference Parker and Fox80) reported on process data such as appreciation, implementation, barriers, feasibility, subscription rates or opinions about the programme. Of those studies, only three studies(Reference Agozzino, Esposito and Genovese47, Reference Bere, Veierod and Bjelland56, Reference Wind, Bjelland and Perez-Rodrigo76) and one study(Reference Haerens, Deforche and Maes58), respectively, considered variation in integrity in analysing the effects of the programme. Short-term effects were reported in nine studies in children(Reference Livingstone, McKinley and Robson52, Reference Mangunkusumo, Brug and de Koning55, Reference Shemilt, Harvey and Shepstone61, Reference Boaz, Ziebland and Wyke71Reference Eriksen, Haraldsdottir and Pederson73, Reference Friel, Kelleher and Campbell81Reference Ransley, Greenwood and Cade83) and five studies in adolescents(Reference Haerens, Deforche and Maes58, Reference Martens, van Assema and Paulussen65, Reference Tsorbatzoudis78, Reference Loughridge and Barratt79, Reference Gratton, Povey and Clark-Carter84), medium-term effects in fifteen(Reference Agozzino, Esposito and Genovese47, Reference Bonaccorsi, Isola and Tognarelli48, Reference D'Addesa, Marzi and Sinesio51, Reference James, Thomas and Cavan54, Reference Bere, Veierod and Bjelland56, Reference Te Velde, Brug and Wind57, Reference Bere, Veierod and Klepp59, Reference Bere, Veierod and Skare60, Reference Bere, Veierod and Bjelland62, Reference Tak, Te Velde and Brug63, Reference Panunzio, Antoniciello and Pisano67, Reference Reinaerts, de Nooijer and Candel70, Reference Ransley, Greenwood and Cade83, Reference Anderson, Porteous and Foster85, Reference Horne, Tapper and Lowe86) and six studies(Reference Eichhorn, Loss and Nagel49, Reference Klepp and Wilhelmsen64, Reference Hassapidou, Fotiadou and Maglara68, Reference Ask, Hernes and Aarek69, Reference Tsorbatzoudis78, Reference Prell, Berg and Jonsson87), respectively, and long-term effects in eight studies(Reference Borys and Lafay50, Reference Pearson, Lambert and Barker53, Reference Bere, Veierod and Bjelland56, Reference Te Velde, Brug and Wind57, Reference Bere, Veierod and Skare60, Reference Bere, Veierod and Bjelland62, Reference Angelico, Del Ben and Fabiani66, Reference James, Thomas and Kerr88) and one study(Reference Passmore and Harris89), respectively. Although all of the projects conducted an outcome assessment at the end of the intervention period, only seven projects in children(Reference Pearson, Lambert and Barker53, Reference Mangunkusumo, Brug and de Koning55, Reference Bere, Veierod and Bjelland56, Reference Bere, Veierod and Skare60, Reference Tak, Te Velde and Brug63, Reference Horne, Tapper and Lowe86, Reference James, Thomas and Kerr88) and only four in adolescents(Reference Haerens, Deforche and Maes58, Reference Klepp and Wilhelmsen64, Reference Tsorbatzoudis78, Reference Loughridge and Barratt79) conducted a follow-up measurement some time after the intervention period had ended. Some studies aimed at specific target groups within the population at large. Eight studies in children(Reference Agozzino, Esposito and Genovese47, Reference Shemilt, Harvey and Shepstone61, Reference O'Brien, Roe and Reeves72, Reference Friel, Kelleher and Campbell81, Reference Anderson, Porteous and Foster85, Reference Horne, Tapper and Lowe86, Reference Fogarty, Antoniak and Venn90, Reference Wells and Nelson91) and four studies in adolescents(Reference Haerens, Deforche and Maes58, Reference Martens, van Assema and Paulussen65, Reference Loughridge and Barratt79, Reference Parker and Fox80) targeted children and adolescents from low socio-economic backgrounds, and two studies in children addressed ethnic minority groups(Reference Tak, Te Velde and Brug63, Reference Reinaerts, de Nooijer and Candel70). All of the studies in children and adolescents targeted both boys and girls.

Table 1 Study and intervention characteristics of included studies promoting healthy nutrition in children

E, experts; T, teachers; RT, research team; FV, fruit and vegetables; NA, not applicable; SS, school staff; F, fruit;+, present in the study; ≠ , several dietary behaviours; ?, not reported in the study.

Table 2 Study and intervention characteristics of included studies promoting healthy nutrition in adolescents

No., number; FV, fruit and vegetables; RT, research team; SS, school staff; T, teachers; E, experts; NA, not applicable; +, present in the study; ≠ , several dietary behaviours; ?, not reported in the study.

Intervention characteristics

The characteristics of the interventions of each included study are presented separately for children and adolescents in Tables 1 and 2. About half of the interventions among children(Reference Agozzino, Esposito and Genovese47, Reference Bonaccorsi, Isola and Tognarelli48, Reference Borys and Lafay50Reference Mangunkusumo, Brug and de Koning55, Reference Angelico, Del Ben and Fabiani66, Reference Panunzio, Antoniciello and Pisano67, Reference Boaz, Ziebland and Wyke71, Reference O'Brien, Roe and Reeves72, Reference Friel, Kelleher and Campbell81, Reference Turnin, Tauber and Couvaras92) and among adolescents(Reference Haerens, Deforche and Maes58, Reference Klepp and Wilhelmsen64, Reference Martens, van Assema and Paulussen65, Reference Hassapidou, Fotiadou and Maglara68, Reference Tsorbatzoudis78, Reference Gratton, Povey and Clark-Carter84) were educational, consisting mainly of classroom-based activities (e.g. an adapted curriculum and distribution of educational materials). Six programmes in children(Reference Bere, Veierod and Klepp59Reference Shemilt, Harvey and Shepstone61, Reference Eriksen, Haraldsdottir and Pederson73, Reference Fogarty, Antoniak and Venn90, Reference Wells and Nelson91) and two programmes in adolescents(Reference Eichhorn, Loss and Nagel49, Reference Passmore and Harris89) used environmental modifications to stimulate a more healthful diet, namely increased availability and accessibility of healthy foods, subscription or distribution programmes, school lunch modifications and incentives. Another nine studies in children(Reference Bere, Veierod and Bjelland56, Reference Te Velde, Brug and Wind57, Reference Bere, Veierod and Bjelland62, Reference Tak, Te Velde and Brug63, Reference Reinaerts, de Nooijer and Candel70, Reference Lowe, Horne and Tapper82, Reference Ransley, Greenwood and Cade83, Reference Anderson, Porteous and Foster85, Reference Horne, Tapper and Lowe86) and another five studies in adolescents(Reference Ask, Hernes and Aarek69, Reference Loughridge and Barratt79, Reference Parker and Fox80, Reference Prell, Berg and Jonsson87, Reference Young93) combined these two components. The duration of the interventions varied greatly between projects from a minimum of 2 weeks(Reference Boaz, Ziebland and Wyke71) to a maximum of 5 years(Reference Borys and Lafay50, Reference Angelico, Del Ben and Fabiani66) in children and from a minimum of 1 week(Reference Gratton, Povey and Clark-Carter84) to a maximum of 2 years(Reference Parker and Fox80) in adolescents. Ten studies in children(Reference Agozzino, Esposito and Genovese47, Reference Bonaccorsi, Isola and Tognarelli48, Reference Livingstone, McKinley and Robson52, Reference Mangunkusumo, Brug and de Koning55Reference Te Velde, Brug and Wind57, Reference Bere, Veierod and Bjelland62, Reference Reinaerts, de Nooijer and Candel70, Reference Friel, Kelleher and Campbell81, Reference Anderson, Porteous and Foster85) and eight studies in adolescents(Reference Eichhorn, Loss and Nagel49, Reference Haerens, Deforche and Maes58, Reference Klepp and Wilhelmsen64, Reference Martens, van Assema and Paulussen65, Reference Tsorbatzoudis78, Reference Gratton, Povey and Clark-Carter84, Reference Prell, Berg and Jonsson87, Reference Passmore and Harris89) evaluated an intervention, which was explicitly informed by one or more behavioural theories, a theoretical framework or an explicit theory-based planning model. The majority of the projects using a theory in children reported the use of the social cognitive theory(Reference Livingstone, McKinley and Robson52, Reference Bere, Veierod and Bjelland56, Reference Bere, Veierod and Bjelland62) or the intervention mapping protocol(Reference Reinaerts, de Nooijer and Candel70, Reference Perez-Rodrigo, Wind and Hildonen94). In adolescents, the theory of planned behaviour was most frequently used(Reference Haerens, Deforche and Maes58, Reference Tsorbatzoudis78, Reference Gratton, Povey and Clark-Carter84, Reference Prell, Berg and Jonsson87, Reference Passmore and Harris89). The consumption of fruit and vegetables was promoted in more than half of the interventions in children(Reference D'Addesa, Marzi and Sinesio51, Reference Pearson, Lambert and Barker53, Reference Mangunkusumo, Brug and de Koning55Reference Te Velde, Brug and Wind57, Reference Bere, Veierod and Klepp59, Reference Bere, Veierod and Skare60, Reference Bere, Veierod and Bjelland62, Reference Tak, Te Velde and Brug63, Reference Reinaerts, de Nooijer and Candel70, Reference Boaz, Ziebland and Wyke71, Reference Eriksen, Haraldsdottir and Pederson73, Reference Lowe, Horne and Tapper82, Reference Ransley, Greenwood and Cade83, Reference Anderson, Porteous and Foster85, Reference Horne, Tapper and Lowe86, Reference Fogarty, Antoniak and Venn90, Reference Wells and Nelson91), while in adolescents several dietary behaviours were addressed in more than half of the interventions(Reference Eichhorn, Loss and Nagel49, Reference Klepp and Wilhelmsen64, Reference Martens, van Assema and Paulussen65, Reference Ask, Hernes and Aarek69, Reference Tsorbatzoudis78, Reference Parker and Fox80, Reference Passmore and Harris89, Reference Young93). All but three projects in children(Reference Shemilt, Harvey and Shepstone61, Reference Fogarty, Antoniak and Venn90, Reference Wells and Nelson91) and one study in adolescents(Reference Hassapidou, Fotiadou and Maglara68) reported teachers or research staff as the primary intervention providers, while school staff and experts were involved occasionally in the delivery of the intervention. The intervention components were mainly delivered in the school setting, since this was an explicit inclusion criterion, but some projects in children involved additional family-based components(Reference Agozzino, Esposito and Genovese47, Reference Bonaccorsi, Isola and Tognarelli48, Reference Borys and Lafay50, Reference D'Addesa, Marzi and Sinesio51, Reference Pearson, Lambert and Barker53, Reference Mangunkusumo, Brug and de Koning55Reference Te Velde, Brug and Wind57, Reference Bere, Veierod and Bjelland62, Reference Angelico, Del Ben and Fabiani66, Reference Reinaerts, de Nooijer and Candel70Reference O'Brien, Roe and Reeves72, Reference Friel, Kelleher and Campbell81Reference Ransley, Greenwood and Cade83, Reference Anderson, Porteous and Foster85, Reference Horne, Tapper and Lowe86) or additional community-based components(Reference Borys and Lafay50, Reference Te Velde, Brug and Wind57, Reference Reinaerts, de Nooijer and Candel70). Few projects in adolescents had additional family-based components(Reference Klepp and Wilhelmsen64, Reference Martens, van Assema and Paulussen65, Reference Hassapidou, Fotiadou and Maglara68, Reference Ask, Hernes and Aarek69, Reference Young93). Finally, some projects in children and adolescents reported the use of health screening plus feedback(Reference Livingstone, McKinley and Robson52, Reference Mangunkusumo, Brug and de Koning55, Reference Martens, van Assema and Paulussen65, Reference Angelico, Del Ben and Fabiani66) and the use of peers(Reference Eichhorn, Loss and Nagel49, Reference Klepp and Wilhelmsen64, Reference Lowe, Horne and Tapper82, Reference Horne, Tapper and Lowe86, Reference Passmore and Harris89).

Evidence of effect

Tables 3 and 4 present the components to calculate the levels of evidence of effect on anthropometrics and dietary behaviour in studies in children and adolescents. The study design, according to the Study Design Algorithm used by the Community Guide(Reference Zaza, Wright-De Agüero and Briss95), the overall quality rating of each study and the overall effectiveness of each study are presented in these tables. It was found that 76 % of the studies in children resulted in an improvement in dietary behaviour and 25 % in body composition. In adolescents, this was 77 and 0 %, respectively. Ten studies in children and six studies in adolescents also measured the effects on dietary determinants. These results are not included in the present paper, but they can be found in Web Extra 3 (http://www.hopeproject.eu/index.php?page = documents&documents_map = %2FWP+9+systematic+review%2F).

Table 3 Study design, overall study quality and overall intervention effectiveness on anthropometrics and dietary behaviour of included studies promoting healthy nutrition in children

/, Not measured; ++/ − − , mixed results; 0, no effect; RCT, randomised controlled trial; ++, overall improvement; − − , overall deterioration; SES, socio-economic status; IG, intervention group.

Table 4 Study design, overall study quality and overall intervention effectiveness on anthropometrics and dietary behaviour of included studies promoting healthy nutrition in adolescents

RCT, randomised controlled trial; /, not measured; ++, overall improvement; 0, no effect; ++/ − − , mixed results.

Finally, Table 5 summarises the stratified levels of evidence for the effectiveness of interventions to promote healthy nutrition in children and adolescents.

Table 5 Summary of levels of evidence for the effectiveness of interventions promoting healthy nutrition in children and adolescents, stratified by intervention type and target group

SES, socio-economic status.

Educational interventions in children

Fourteen studies evaluated the effect of education-only interventions in children on dietary behaviour, including two strong(Reference James, Thomas and Cavan54, Reference Mangunkusumo, Brug and de Koning55) and one weak(Reference Panunzio, Antoniciello and Pisano67) cluster RCT, one moderate(Reference Livingstone, McKinley and Robson52) and three weak(Reference D'Addesa, Marzi and Sinesio51, Reference Boaz, Ziebland and Wyke71, Reference Friel, Kelleher and Campbell81) controlled trials, and five weak before–after studies(Reference Agozzino, Esposito and Genovese47, Reference Bonaccorsi, Isola and Tognarelli48, Reference Pearson, Lambert and Barker53, Reference Angelico, Del Ben and Fabiani66, Reference O'Brien, Roe and Reeves72) and two weak prospective cohorts(Reference Borys and Lafay50, Reference Turnin, Tauber and Couvaras92). In six studies, effectiveness was not found(Reference Bonaccorsi, Isola and Tognarelli48, Reference D'Addesa, Marzi and Sinesio51, Reference Mangunkusumo, Brug and de Koning55, Reference Angelico, Del Ben and Fabiani66, Reference Panunzio, Antoniciello and Pisano67, Reference Boaz, Ziebland and Wyke71) and in two studies mixed results were found(Reference Agozzino, Esposito and Genovese47, Reference O'Brien, Roe and Reeves72). Positive effects were found in the short term(Reference Livingstone, McKinley and Robson52, Reference Friel, Kelleher and Campbell81), the medium term(Reference James, Thomas and Cavan54), the long term(Reference Borys and Lafay50, Reference Pearson, Lambert and Barker53) and in a prospective cohort(Reference Turnin, Tauber and Couvaras92), and in one study an improvement was mainly found in children from advantaged areas(Reference Friel, Kelleher and Campbell81). This equates to limited evidence that educational interventions in children can alter dietary behaviour positively.

Only four educational studies in children reported the effect on anthropometrics, including one strong(Reference James, Thomas and Cavan54) and one weak(Reference Panunzio, Antoniciello and Pisano67) cluster RCT, one weak before–after study(Reference Angelico, Del Ben and Fabiani66) and one weak prospective cohort(Reference Borys and Lafay50). A well-executed study discouraging the consumption of carbonated drinks reported a positive effect on the prevalence of obesity after 1 year(Reference James, Thomas and Cavan54), but this effect was not sustained after 2 years(Reference James, Thomas and Kerr88). However, no significant changes in BMI, centile z-scores and waist z-scores were found at both follow-ups. Two studies found significant negative effects in subgroups at medium term(Reference Panunzio, Antoniciello and Pisano67) and long term(Reference Borys and Lafay50), and one study found no effect on BMI(Reference Angelico, Del Ben and Fabiani66). This provides inconclusive evidence that educational interventions can contribute to changes in body composition.

Environmental interventions in children

Five studies on fruit and vegetable subscription or distribution programmes and one breakfast distribution programme(Reference Shemilt, Harvey and Shepstone61) assessed the effect on fruit and vegetable intakes and on breakfast habits, including one moderate cluster RCT(Reference Shemilt, Harvey and Shepstone61), two moderate(Reference Bere, Veierod and Klepp59, Reference Bere, Veierod and Skare60) and one weak(Reference Eriksen, Haraldsdottir and Pederson73) controlled trials and two weak prospective cohorts(Reference Fogarty, Antoniak and Venn90, Reference Wells and Nelson91). Effectiveness was found in the six studies, but only in one study a sustained effect at the long term was detected(Reference Bere, Veierod and Skare60). This suggests that there is limited evidence that environmental interventions can improve fruit and vegetable intakes and there is inconclusive evidence that environmental interventions can improve breakfast habits.

None of the interventions with environmental modifications in children measured the effect on body composition.

Multicomponent interventions in children

Nine multicomponent studies assessed the effect on fruit and/or vegetable intake, including two strong(Reference Bere, Veierod and Bjelland56, Reference Perez-Rodrigo, Wind and Hildonen94), one moderate(Reference Bere, Veierod and Bjelland62) and one weak(Reference Anderson, Porteous and Foster85) cluster RCT, one moderate(Reference Tak, Te Velde and Brug63) and three weak(Reference Reinaerts, de Nooijer and Candel70, Reference Ransley, Greenwood and Cade83, Reference Horne, Tapper and Lowe86) controlled trials and one weak before–after study(Reference Lowe, Horne and Tapper82). All consisted primarily of a fruit and/or vegetables subscription or distribution programme combined with a nutrition education curriculum and all found an improvement in dietary behaviour. Eight studies reported effects in subgroups only(Reference Bere, Veierod and Bjelland56, Reference Bere, Veierod and Bjelland62, Reference Tak, Te Velde and Brug63, Reference Reinaerts, de Nooijer and Candel70, Reference Lowe, Horne and Tapper82, Reference Anderson, Porteous and Foster85, Reference Horne, Tapper and Lowe86, Reference Perez-Rodrigo, Wind and Hildonen94) and five studies reported a long-term effect(Reference Bere, Veierod and Bjelland56, Reference Te Velde, Brug and Wind57, Reference Bere, Veierod and Bjelland62, Reference Ransley, Greenwood and Cade83, Reference Horne, Tapper and Lowe86). This provides strong evidence that multicomponent interventions can have a positive effect on fruit and vegetable intakes.

None of the multicomponent studies in children measured the effect on anthropometrics.

Educational interventions in adolescents

All the education-only programmes in adolescents measured the effect on dietary intake, including one strong(Reference Haerens, Deforche and Maes58), one moderate(Reference Martens, van Assema and Paulussen65) and one weak(Reference Hassapidou, Fotiadou and Maglara68) cluster RCT, one weak RCT(Reference Gratton, Povey and Clark-Carter84) and one moderate(Reference Klepp and Wilhelmsen64) and one weak(Reference Tsorbatzoudis78) controlled trial. Positive effects were almost always reported with one study reporting mixed results(Reference Martens, van Assema and Paulussen65). Furthermore, four studies reported effects in subgroups only(Reference Haerens, Deforche and Maes58, Reference Klepp and Wilhelmsen64, Reference Martens, van Assema and Paulussen65, Reference Hassapidou, Fotiadou and Maglara68) and in one study the effect was sustained after 1 year(Reference Klepp and Wilhelmsen64). To conclude, there is moderate evidence that educational interventions in adolescents can improve dietary behaviour.

Inconclusive evidence of effect was found for education-only interventions in adolescents on body composition, with only one weak cluster randomised trial(Reference Hassapidou, Fotiadou and Maglara68) measuring the effect on height and weight and reporting no significant change(Reference Hassapidou, Fotiadou and Maglara68).

Environmental interventions in adolescents

Two environmental programmes assessed the effect on behaviour, including one weak controlled trial(Reference Passmore and Harris89) and one weak before–after study(Reference Eichhorn, Loss and Nagel49). Both trials used a peer-based setting approach. A positive long-term effect on food intake at school was found in one study(Reference Passmore and Harris89), equating to inconclusive evidence of effect.

None of the interventions with environmental modifications in adolescents measured the effect on anthropometrics.

Multicomponent interventions in adolescents

The five multicomponent programmes measured the effect on dietary intake, including a weak cluster RCT(Reference Ask, Hernes and Aarek69), two weak controlled trials(Reference Loughridge and Barratt79, Reference Prell, Berg and Jonsson87) and two weak prospective cohort studies(Reference Parker and Fox80, Reference Young93). All programmes consisted primarily of a distribution programme or changes in the school lunch combined with a nutrition education curriculum. Effectiveness was found in four studies and one study reported mixed results(Reference Parker and Fox80), suggesting that there is limited evidence of effect. Furthermore, in three studies the effect was only measured on food intake at school(Reference Loughridge and Barratt79, Reference Parker and Fox80, Reference Prell, Berg and Jonsson87) and in one study a sustained effect at the long term was not found(Reference Parker and Fox80).

Inconclusive evidence was found for the effect of multicomponent interventions on body composition; the weak cluster randomised trial in which anthropometrics were measured did not find effects(Reference Ask, Hernes and Aarek69).

Children from low socio-economic backgrounds

All eight studies targeting children from low socio-economic backgrounds assessed the effect on dietary behaviour, including one moderate(Reference Shemilt, Harvey and Shepstone61) and one weak(Reference Anderson, Porteous and Foster85) cluster RCT, two weak controlled trials(Reference Friel, Kelleher and Campbell81, Reference Horne, Tapper and Lowe86), two weak before–after studies(Reference Agozzino, Esposito and Genovese47, Reference O'Brien, Roe and Reeves72) and two weak prospective cohort studies(Reference Fogarty, Antoniak and Venn90, Reference Wells and Nelson91). Two studies reported mixed results(Reference Agozzino, Esposito and Genovese47, Reference O'Brien, Roe and Reeves72), while the other studies reported improvements in dietary behaviour. This provides limited evidence that interventions targeted at children with a low socio-economic status are effective in changing dietary behaviour. None of the interventions specifically aimed at low socio-economic groups measured effects on body composition.

Children from ethnic minority populations

Only two studies evaluated the effect of an intervention in children from ethnic minority groups, including one moderate(Reference Tak, Te Velde and Brug63) and one weak controlled trial(Reference Reinaerts, de Nooijer and Candel70). A significant positive effect on dietary intake was found in both studies, suggesting inconclusive evidence of effect. Both studies did not measure anthropometrics.

Adolescents from low socio-economic backgrounds

Four studies in adolescents targeted adolescents with a low socio-economic status, including one strong(Reference Haerens, Deforche and Maes58) and one moderate(Reference Martens, van Assema and Paulussen65) cluster RCT, one weak controlled trial(Reference Loughridge and Barratt79) and one weak prospective cohort study(Reference Parker and Fox80). All four studies measured the effect on dietary behaviour. Two studies found an improvement in behaviour in this target group(Reference Haerens, Deforche and Maes58, Reference Loughridge and Barratt79), while two studies found mixed results(Reference Martens, van Assema and Paulussen65, Reference Parker and Fox80). Furthermore, in two studies the effect was only found in a subgroup(Reference Haerens, Deforche and Maes58, Reference Martens, van Assema and Paulussen65). This suggests that there is inconclusive evidence that interventions in adolescents with low socio-economic backgrounds can change dietary behaviour positively. None of these studies measured the effect on body composition.

Adolescents from ethnic minority populations

None of the studies measured the effect in adolescents from ethnic minority populations.

Discussion

The purpose of the present review was to compile the evidence regarding the effectiveness of school-based programmes promoting a healthy diet on dietary intake and anthropometrics in children and adolescents in Europe. As the number of studies conducted among children was more than double and more studies in children were of higher quality, this resulted in more evidence of effect for interventions among children than among adolescents.

In children, there is strong evidence that multicomponent interventions that combine improved availability of fruit and vegetables with a nutrition education curriculum delivered by the teacher and at least some parent involvement can alter intake of fruit and vegetables. Furthermore, limited evidence of effect was found for nutrition education-only programmes delivered by teachers using practical activities such as taste testing, cooking classes, etc. Limited evidence of effect was also found for the effectiveness of programmes that only focused on environmental change. These were restricted to fruit and vegetables distribution programmes, either for free(Reference Bere, Veierod and Klepp59Reference Shemilt, Harvey and Shepstone61, Reference Fogarty, Antoniak and Venn90, Reference Wells and Nelson91) or as a subscription programme(Reference Bere, Veierod and Klepp59, Reference Bere, Veierod and Skare60, Reference Eriksen, Haraldsdottir and Pederson73). For both schemes, evidence for effectiveness was found. Furthermore, inconclusive evidence of effect on dietary behaviour was found for environmental initiatives that targeted breakfast habits, for studies directed at children from low socio-economic backgrounds or ethnic minority groups. The present review also shows inconclusive evidence of effect of educational interventions on body composition, while this effect was never measured in the environmental and multicomponent interventions in children. For children, we can conclude that a range of fruit and vegetable promotion activities can be recommended for implementation and have a great chance of success, as all the environmental and multicomponent interventions including a fruit and vegetable provision scheme resulted in improvements in this behaviour. The European Commission has indeed issued implementation of such schemes across Europe (http://ec.europa.eu/health-eu/my_lifestyle/nutrition/index_en.htm). Whether fruit or vegetable promotion will meaningfully contribute to obesity prevention is rather doubtful, and more studies are needed that target a range of nutrition behaviours that contribute importantly to energy balance.

The same conclusion was found in a recent review investigating the effectiveness of worldwide interventions to promote fruit and/or vegetable consumption in children in schools, including fifteen studies from Europe, fourteen studies from the USA and one study from New Zealand(Reference de Sa and Lock29). Of the thirty studies included, 70 % increased fruit and vegetable intakes, with none decreasing intake. Nevertheless, of the seven school fruit and vegetables schemes targeting obesity reduction, only one study managed to produce positive impact on BMI and fruit and vegetable intakes.

In adolescents, moderate evidence was found for the effect of education-only interventions on dietary intake. Educational trials in adolescents usually provided a nutrition education curriculum that was delivered by the teachers. Besides, limited evidence of effect is provided for multicomponent programmes on dietary behaviour. The environmental part of these multicomponent trials consisted of adapting or increasing the availability of healthy food. This was combined with educating the adolescents about healthy nutrition by the teacher. In general, inconclusive evidence of effect of all these interventions was found on anthropometrics. As in children, it is also apparent in the trials for adolescents that there is an urgent need for dietary interventions in adolescents that measure the effect on overweight or obesity.

If we compare the present results with previous reviews including studies from outside Europe, similar conclusions were found. The most recent international review, reviewing the effectiveness of interventions improving the school food environment, has found effectiveness for school food policies on food intake, but little evaluation of the impact on BMI was found(Reference Jaime and Lock25). The present review included eleven studies from the USA and seven from Europe.

It is believed that parents play a direct role in children's and adolescent's eating patterns and consequently it is advocated that interventions, aimed at improving children's and adolescent's nutrition, need to address the family(Reference Rasmussen, Krolner and Klepp96Reference Patrick and Nicklas98). Furthermore, previous reviews concluded that the involvement of parents is an important determinant for success(Reference French and Stables18, Reference Sharma21, Reference Knai, Pomerleau and Lock26, Reference Blanchette and Brug28). Many school-based interventions included in the present review included some parental involvement, 62 % of the studies in children and 38 % of the studies in adolescents. Although this was usually limited to newsletters, folders, homework assignments, or at best the organisation of some family nights at school, just over half of these interventions were successful in improving dietary behaviour making it difficult to come to a similar strong conclusion as in other international reviews(Reference French and Stables18, Reference Sharma21, Reference Knai, Pomerleau and Lock26, Reference Blanchette and Brug28).

The evidence of effect compiled in these European Union studies suggests a number of recommendations for future research in Europe. First, the present review showed that in the few studies that included anthropometric measures, the evidence on body composition was lacking or inconclusive. Only a limited number of studies measured anthropometrics and the studies that did were mostly ‘low-dose’ studies over short periods. Furthermore, most of the included studies were not explicitly aiming to contribute to obesity prevention and in the end obesity prevention is only partly a nutritional issue. Nevertheless, there is an urgent need for studies that include measures of body composition to fill the gap in the European literature. Second, more consensus is needed about ‘best measures’ for diet. This would help to increase comparability between studies and would facilitate assessment of effectiveness. Studies would also be stronger when self-reports, which could reflect a desire to report the ‘right answer’, would be verified with more objective monitoring(Reference van't Veer, Kardinaal and Bausch-Goldbohm99, Reference Livingstone, Robson and Wallace100). Consequently, this illustrates the need for objective and validated outcome measures for dietary behaviour. Third, some studies only observed the effect on food intake at school(Reference Angelico, Del Ben and Fabiani66, Reference O'Brien, Roe and Reeves72, Reference Loughridge and Barratt79, Reference Parker and Fox80, Reference Prell, Berg and Jonsson87, Reference Passmore and Harris89). This raises the question whether children might compensate during the rest of the day. It is therefore important to assess the effect of the intervention on total dietary behaviour. Fourth, more research is needed in specific groups. In general, more studies are needed in adolescents; specifically, older adolescents (16–18 years old) were never addressed in the included studies. Furthermore, socio-economic status and ethnicity are identified as determinants of healthy eating(Reference Brug101), but fewer interventions targeted children and adolescents from low socio-economic backgrounds(Reference Haerens, Deforche and Maes58, Reference Martens, van Assema and Paulussen65, Reference Loughridge and Barratt79, Reference Parker and Fox80) and from ethnic minority populations. Consequently, future research should address these populations. Fifth, it appeared that follow-up periods were relatively short, only 24 % of the studies in children and 9 % in adolescents reported long-term effects. Additionally, most of the interventions focused on short-term changes right after the intervention, 86 and 72 %, respectively, while it is essential to have measures of at least 6 months after the intervention to be able to study the retention of behaviour change and to detect if weight status is modified(Reference Jackson39). Studies with greater length are needed to make it possible to draw conclusions about the effectiveness on behaviour and obesity and about the sustainability of an intervention. Sixth, cost-effectiveness data were only provided in one study(Reference Shemilt75), while it is recommended that the efficiency of the intervention is also measured next to its effectiveness. Economic analyses and economic evidence must become a central part of prevention research. These are needed for appropriate policy decision making and for assessing long-term benefits(Reference Rychetnik, Frommer and Hawe36, Reference Kellam and Langevin102). Finally, a lot of the methodological shortcomings reported in the quality analysis were due to lack of information. So we advocate that researchers follow existing guidelines for reporting trials in the future(Reference Des Jarlais, Lyles and Crepaz40, Reference Moher, Schulz and Altman42). Furthermore, the main reasons that studies received an overall low-quality rating were mainly due to a weak rating on the criteria selection bias and data collection methods. Consequently, European studies should try to do a better job on these quality criteria. In the same line, there is substantial evidence from the literature to suggest that the explicit use of theory will significantly improve the chances of effectiveness(Reference Jackson39), but not all of the studies reported the use of a theoretical background. Especially, the educational and multicomponent studies relied on a theoretical background, while environmental interventions never reported a theoretical background. In the present review, it is not possible to conclude that a theoretical background is a key factor to success because both, studies that reported the use of a theory and studies that did not, showed effectiveness.

One of the major advantages of the present review is that we also collected grey literature and lower quality studies. In this way, the European evidence was broadened up, but grey literature and lower quality studies are useful information that is unfortunately excluded in many systematic reviews that instead primarily focus on studies with rigid designs. Another advantage is that the present review looks specifically at the evidence available in Europe. European studies were the minority of included studies in other systematic reviews, which makes them hard to generalise to Europe. However, most of the studies included in the present review were carried out in countries from Western and Northern parts of Europe. This raises questions about the generalisability of these results to other countries in Europe, especially because contextual variables were often lacking in the included studies. To assess the usefulness of these strategies in the other parts of Europe, we would recommend replicating evaluations of the successful interventions identified in the present review adapted to a specific country and its culture.

Conclusion

Implications for policy makers

Various policy documents have called for the development of effective strategies to improve dietary habits in children and adolescents to help halt or reverse the increase in obesity and to improve other aspects of health.

In children, it appears that a subscription or distribution programme for fruit and vegetables combined with an educational component is likely to be effective to stimulate the consumption of fruit and vegetables. Educational programmes solely can also be effective in stimulating a healthy diet among children. Furthermore, evidence was found for fruit and vegetable subscription or distribution programmes on intake. Finally, studies in children from low socio-economic backgrounds seemed to be effective to improve dietary intake.

In adolescents, an educational programme is likely to be effective to promote healthy nutrition. Additionally, evidence was also found for programmes that adapted school lunches or increased the availability of healthy food and combined this with a nutritional curriculum on food intake.

To conclude, evidence of effect was found for European school-based initiatives that promote a healthy diet in school-aged children on behaviour. European evidence of effect was not established for school-based dietary interventions on obesity prevention. Furthermore, as discussed before, recommendations should not only be based on effectiveness, a lot of other factors such as sustainability, integrity, context and cost-effectiveness should also be considered to be able to deliver appropriate evidence-based recommendations to the policy makers.

Implications for research

Future research should focus on filling the gaps identified in the present review. To improve the quality of the evidence of effectiveness on these kind of interventions, it is important that high-quality studies are executed, namely studies with the most rigorous design as possible, an appropriate sample size, a follow-up beyond post intervention, the use of more objective measures of dietary behaviour, measurements of body composition and the assessment of implementation issues and cost-effectiveness. Next to high-quality studies, high-quality interventions are required. These are interventions with greater length and intensity as low-dose interventions over short periods are unlikely to induce improvements in behaviour and anthropometrics, sufficient integrity, and adequate involvement of the parents and schools. Furthermore, studies should be reported according to certain standards (e.g. CONSORT and the TREND statement), so that existing studies can be better compared and all the necessary information for the reader is delivered(Reference Des Jarlais, Lyles and Crepaz40, Reference Moher, Schulz and Altman42).

Acknowledgements

The present work was supported by a European Commission funded project called Health promotion through Obesity Prevention in Europe (HOPE, www.hopeproject.eu). The full search strategy and the complete characteristics, results and quality assessment table are available to view at (http://www.hopeproject.eu/index.php?page = documents&documents_map = %2FWP+9+systematic+review%2F). The contribution of the funder and the authors is as follows: The HOPE project identified the research question, study design and decided to submit the article for publication. E. V. C. led the review and designed the search strategy. She carried out the literature searches and screened the initial results, extracted data, analysed the findings and drafted the tables. H. S., I. D. B. and L. M. assisted with screening the full-text articles, and E. V. C. and H. S. assessed the methodological quality of the studies. E. V. C. drafted the manuscript. All authors contributed to synthesising the results and critical revision of the manuscript, and all approved the final version. The authors have no conflicts of interest to declare. Ethical approval was not required for the study.

References

1Centers for Disease Control and Prevention (1997) Guidelines for school health programs to promote lifelong healthy eating. J Sch Health 67, 926.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
2Lytle, LA & Kubik, MY (2003) Nutritional issues for adolescents. Best Pract Res Clin Endocrinol Metab 17, 177189.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
3World Health Organization (2003) Diet, Nutrition and The Prevention of Chronic Diseases: Report of a Joint WHO/FAO Expert Consultation. WHO Technical Report Series no. 916. Geneva: WHO.Google Scholar
4Willett, WC (1994) Diet and health: what should we eat? Science 264, 532537.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
5Luzzi, AF & James, WPT (2001) European diet and public health: the continuing challenge. Public Health Nutr 4, 275292.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
6Kelder, SH, Perry, CL, Klepp, KI, et al. (1994) Longitudinal tracking of adolescent smoking, physical activity, and food choice behaviors. Am J Public Health 84, 11211126.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
7Lien, N, Lytle, LA & Klepp, KI (2001) Stability in consumption of fruit, vegetables, and sugary foods in a cohort from age 14 to age 21. Prev Med 33, 217226.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
8Lytle, P, Seifert, MPH, Greenstein, MS, et al. (2000) How do children's eating patterns and food choices change over time? Results from a cohort study. Am J Health Promot 14, 222228.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
9Brug, J (2006) Healthful nutrition promotion in Europe: goals, target populations, and strategies. Patient Educ Couns 63, 255257.Google ScholarPubMed
10Currie, C, Roberts, C, Morgan, A, et al. (2004) Young People's Health in Context: International Report from The HBSC 2001/02 Survey. WHO Policy Series: Health Policy for Children and Adolescents Issue 4. Copenhagen: WHO Regional Office for Europe.Google Scholar
11Branca, F, Nikogosian, H & Lobstein, T (2007) The Challenge of Obesity in the WHO European Region and the Strategies for Response. Copenhagen: World Health Organization.Google Scholar
12Lambert, J, Agostoni, C, Elmadfa, I, et al. (2004) Dietary intake and nutritional status of children and adolescents in Europe. Br J Nutr 92, Suppl. 2, S147S211.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
13Cruz, JAA (2000) Dietary habits and nutritional status in adolescents over Europe - Southern Europe. Eur J Clin Nutr 54, S29S35.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
14Currie, C, Gabhainn, SN, Godeau, E, et al. (2008) Inequalities in Young People's Health: International Report From The HBSC 2005/06 Survey. WHO Policy Series: Health Policy for Children and Adolescents Issue 5. Copenhagen: WHO Regional Office for Europe.Google Scholar
15Commission of the European Communities (2005) Green paper: ‘Promoting Healthy Diets and Physical Activity: A European Dimension for The Prevention of Overweight, Obesity and Chronic Diseases’. Brussels: Commission of the European Communities.Google Scholar
16Summerbell, CD, Waters, E, Edmunds, LD, et al. (2005) Interventions for preventing obesity in children. The Cochrane Database of Systematic Reviews CD001871.Google ScholarPubMed
17Doak, CM, Visscher, TL, Renders, CM, et al. (2006) The prevention of overweight and obesity in children and adolescents: a review of interventions and programmes. Obes Rev 7, 111136.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
18French, SA & Stables, G (2003) Environmental interventions to promote vegetable and fruit consumption among youth in school settings. Prev Med 37, 593610.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
19Flodmark, CE, Marcus, C & Britton, M (2006) Interventions to prevent obesity in children and adolescents: a systematic literature review. Int J Obes 30, 579589.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
20Sharma, M (2006) School-based interventions for childhood and adolescent obesity. Obes Rev 7, 261269.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
21Sharma, M (2006) International school-based interventions for preventing obesity in children. Obes Rev 8, 155167.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
22Shaya, FT, Flores, D, Gbarayor, CM, et al. (2008) School-based obesity interventions: a literature review. J Sch Health 78, 189196.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
23Hardeman, W, Griffin, S, Johnston, M, et al. (2000) Interventions to prevent weight gain: a systematic review of psychological models and behaviour change methods. Int J Obes Relat Metab Disord 24, 131143.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
24Institute for Health and Clinical Excellence National Collaborating Centre for Primary Care (2006) Obesity: The Prevention, Identification, Assessment and Management of Overweight and Obesity in Adults and Children. Obesity: Full Guideline no. CG43. London: NICE.Google Scholar
25Jaime, PC & Lock, K (2009) Do school based food and nutrition policies improve diet and reduce obesity? Prev Med 48, 4553.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
26Knai, C, Pomerleau, J, Lock, K, et al. (2006) Getting children to eat more fruit and vegetables: a systematic review. Prev Med 42, 8595.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
27Ammerman, AS, Lindquist, CH, Lohr, KN, et al. (2002) The efficacy of behavioral interventions to modify dietary fat and fruit and vegetable intake: a review of the evidence. Prev Med 35, 2541.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
28Blanchette, L & Brug, J (2005) Determinants of fruit and vegetable consumption among 6–12-year-old children and effective interventions to increase consumption. J Hum Nutr Diet 18, 431443.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
29de Sa, J & Lock, K (2008) Will European agricultural policy for school fruit and vegetables improve public health? A review of school fruit and vegetable programmes. Eur J Public Health 18, 558568.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
30Brug, J & van Lenthe, F (2005) Environmental Determinants and Interventions for Physical Activity, Nutrition and Smoking: A Review. Rotterdam: Erasmus MC.Google Scholar
31Flynn, MA, McNeil, DA, Maloff, B, et al. (2006) Reducing obesity and related chronic disease risk in children and youth: a synthesis of evidence with ‘best practice’ recommendations. Obes Rev 7, Suppl. 1, 766.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
32Katz, DL, O'Connell, M, Yeh, MC, et al. (2005) Public health strategies for preventing and controlling overweight and obesity in school and worksite settings: a report on recommendations of the Task Force on Community Preventive Services. MMWR Recomm Rep 54, 112.Google ScholarPubMed
33Norman, GJ, Zabinski, MF, Adams, MA, et al. (2007) A review of eHealth interventions for physical activity and dietary behavior change. Am J Prev Med 33, 336345.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
34McQueen, DV & Jones, CM (2007) Global Perspectives on Health Promotion Effectiveness. New York: Springer Science.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
35Swinburn, B, Gill, T & Kumanyika, S (2005) Obesity prevention: a proposed framework for translating evidence into action. Obes Rev 6, 2333.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
36Rychetnik, L, Frommer, M, Hawe, P, et al. (2002) Criteria for evaluating evidence on public health interventions. J Epidemiol Community Health 56, 119127.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
37Kemm, J (2006) The limitations of ‘evidence-based’ public health. J Eval Clin Pract 12, 319324.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
38Jackson, N (2003) Handbook Systematic Reviews of Health Promotion and Public Health Interventions. The Cochrane Collaboration, pp. 5767. Victoria, Australia. http://www.ph.cochrane.org/en/authors.html.Google Scholar
39Jackson, N (2005) Criteria for the systematic review of health promotion and public health interventions. Health Promot Int 20, 367374.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
40Des Jarlais, DC, Lyles, C, Crepaz, N, et al. (2004) Improving the reporting quality of nonrandomized evaluations of behavioral and public health interventions: the TREND statement. Am J Public Health 94, 361366.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
41Altman, DG, Schulz, KF, Moher, D, et al. (2001) The revised CONSORT statement for reporting randomized trials: explanation and elaboration. Ann Intern Med 134, 663694.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
42Moher, D, Schulz, K & Altman, D (2001) The CONSORT statement: revised recommendations for improving the quality of reports of parallel group randomized trials. BMC Med Res Methodol 1, 2.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
43Moher, D, Jones, A, Lepage, L, et al. (2001) Use of the CONSORT statement and quality of reports of randomized trials: a comparative before-and-after evaluation. JAMA 285, 19921995.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
44Engbers, LH, van Poppel, MN, Chin A Paw, MJ, et al. (2005) Worksite health promotion programs with environmental changes: a systematic review. Am J Prev Med 29, 6170.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
45Proper, KI, Staal, BJ, Hildebrandt, VH, et al. (2002) Effectiveness of physical activity programs at worksites with respect to work-related outcomes. Scand J Work Environ Health 28, 7584.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
46van Sluijs, EM, McMinn, AM & Griffin, SJ (2007) Effectiveness of interventions to promote physical activity in children and adolescents: systematic review of controlled trials. BMJ 335, 703.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
47Agozzino, E, Esposito, D, Genovese, S, et al. (2007) Evaluation of the effectiveness of a nutrition education intervention performed by primary school teachers. Ann Ig 19, 315324.Google ScholarPubMed
48Bonaccorsi, G, Isola, A, Tognarelli, M, et al. (2002) Changes in eating habits among a group of children after completion of an educational intervention program in elementary school. Ann Ig 14, 243251.Google ScholarPubMed
49Eichhorn, C, Loss, J & Nagel, E (2007) Process and outcome evaluation of the school-based project ‘Students’ enterprises for healthy nutrition'. Int J Public Health 52, 242254.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
50Borys, JM & Lafay, L (2000) Nutritional information for children to modify the food habits of the whole family. Rev Med Suisse Romande 120, 207209.Google ScholarPubMed
51D'Addesa, D, Marzi, V, Sinesio, F, et al. (2006) Nutrition intervention to promote higher fruit, vegetable and legume consumption among schoolchildren. Int J Obes 30, S5S6.Google Scholar
52Livingstone, M, McKinley, M, Robson, P, et al. (2002) Development and Evaluation of an Interactive Multi-media CD-ROM for the Promotion of Nutrition Education in Secondary Schoolchildren no. N09012. London: Food Standards Agency.Google Scholar
53Pearson, T, Lambert, N & Barker, M (2002) Increasing Fruit and Vegetable Consumption in Children: The Development and Evaluation of a School-based Intervention Using Art/play Therapy no. N09008. London: Food Standards Agency.Google Scholar
54James, J, Thomas, P, Cavan, D, et al. (2004) Preventing childhood obesity by reducing consumption of carbonated drinks: cluster randomised controlled trial. BMJ 328, 1237.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
55Mangunkusumo, RT, Brug, J, de Koning, HJ, et al. (2007) School-based internet-tailored fruit and vegetable education combined with brief counselling increases children's awareness of intake levels. Public Health Nutr 10, 273279.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
56Bere, E, Veierod, MB, Bjelland, M, et al. (2006) Outcome and process evaluation of a Norwegian school-randomized fruit and vegetable intervention: fruits and vegetables make the marks (FVMM). Health Educ Res 21, 258267.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
57Te Velde, SJ, Brug, J, Wind, M, et al. (2008) Effects of a comprehensive fruit- and vegetable-promoting school-based intervention in three European countries: the Pro Children Study. Br J Nutr 99, 893903.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
58Haerens, L, Deforche, B, Maes, L, et al. (2007) A computer-tailored dietary fat intake intervention for adolescents: results of a randomized controlled trial. Ann Behav Med 34, 253262.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
59Bere, E, Veierod, MB & Klepp, KI (2005) The Norwegian School Fruit Programme: evaluating paid vs. no-cost subscriptions. Prev Med 41, 463470.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
60Bere, E, Veierod, MB, Skare, O, et al. (2007) Free School Fruit-sustained effect three years later. Int J Behav Nutr Phys Act 4, 5.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
61Shemilt, I, Harvey, I, Shepstone, L, et al. (2004) A national evaluation of school breakfast clubs: evidence from a cluster randomized controlled trial and an observational analysis. Child Care Health Dev 30, 413427.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
62Bere, E, Veierod, MB, Bjelland, M, et al. (2006) Free School Fruit-sustained effect 1 year later. Health Educ Res 21, 268275.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
63Tak, NI, Te Velde, SJ & Brug, J (2007) Ethnic differences in 1-year follow-up effect of the Dutch Schoolgruiten Project – promoting fruit and vegetable consumption among primary-school children. Public Health Nutr 10, 14971507.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
64Klepp, KI & Wilhelmsen, BU (1993) Nutrition education in junior high schools: incorporating behavior change strategies into home economics courses. Health Educ Res 8, 547554.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
65Martens, MK, van Assema, P, Paulussen, TG, et al. (2008) Krachtvoer: effect evaluation of a Dutch healthful diet promotion curriculum for lower vocational schools. Public Health Nutr 11, 271278.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
66Angelico, F, Del Ben, M, Fabiani, L, et al. (1991) Management of childhood obesity through a school-based programme of general health and nutrition education. Public Health 105, 393398.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
67Panunzio, MF, Antoniciello, A, Pisano, A, et al. (2007) Nutrition education intervention by teachers may promote fruit and vegetable consumption in Italian students. Nutr Res 27, 524528.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
68Hassapidou, MN, Fotiadou, E & Maglara, E (1997) A nutrition intervention programme for lower secondary schools in Greece. Health Educ J 56, 134144.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
69Ask, AS, Hernes, S, Aarek, I, et al. (2006) Changes in dietary pattern in 15 year old adolescents following a 4 month dietary intervention with school breakfast – a pilot study. Nutr J 5, 33.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
70Reinaerts, E, de Nooijer, J, Candel, M, et al. (2007) Increasing children's fruit and vegetable consumption: distribution or a multicomponent programme? Public Health Nutr 10, 939947.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
71Boaz, A, Ziebland, S, Wyke, S, et al. (1998) A ‘five-a-day’ fruit and vegetable pack for primary school children. Part II: controlled evaluation in two Scottish schools. Health Educ J 57, 105116.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
72O'Brien, N, Roe, C & Reeves, S (2002) A quantitative nutritional evaluation of a healthy eating intervention in primary school children in a socioeconomically disadvantaged area – a pilot study. Health Educ J 61, 320328.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
73Eriksen, K, Haraldsdottir, J, Pederson, R, et al. (2003) Effect of a fruit and vegetable subscription in Danish schools. Public Health Nutr 6, 5763.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
74Reinaerts, EB, de Nooijer, J & de Vries, NK (2007) Fruit and vegetable distribution program versus a multicomponent program to increase fruit and vegetable consumption: which should be recommended for implementation? J Sch Health 77, 679686.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
75Shemilt, I (2004) A national evaluation of school breakfast clubs: where does economics fit in? Child Care Health Dev 30, 429437.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
76Wind, M, Bjelland, M, Perez-Rodrigo, C, et al. (2008) Appreciation and implementation of a school-based intervention are associated with changes in fruit and vegetable intake in 10- to 13-year old schoolchildren – the Pro Children study. Health Educ Res 23, 9971007.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
77Martens, M, van Assema, P, Paulussen, T, et al. (2006) Krachtvoer: process evaluation of a Dutch programme for lower vocational schools to promote healthful diet. Health Educ Res 21, 695704.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
78Tsorbatzoudis, H (2005) Evaluation of a planned behavior theory-based intervention programme to promote healthy eating. Percept Mot Skills 101, 587604.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
79Loughridge, JL & Barratt, J (2005) Does the provision of cooled filtered water in secondary school cafeterias increase water drinking and decrease the purchase of soft drinks? J Hum Nutr Diet 18, 281286.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
80Parker, L & Fox, A (2001) The Peterborough Schools Nutrition Project: a multiple intervention programme to improve school-based eating in secondary schools. Public Health Nutr 4, 12211228.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
81Friel, S, Kelleher, C, Campbell, P, et al. (1999) Evaluation of the nutrition education at primary school (NEAPS) programme. Public Health Nutr 2, 549555.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
82Lowe, CF, Horne, PJ, Tapper, K, et al. (2004) Effects of a peer modelling and rewards-based intervention to increase fruit and vegetable consumption in children. Eur J Clin Nutr 58, 510522.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
83Ransley, JK, Greenwood, DC, Cade, JE, et al. (2007) Does the school fruit and vegetable scheme improve children's diet? A non-randomised controlled trial. J Epidemiol Commun Health 61, 699703.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
84Gratton, L, Povey, R & Clark-Carter, D (2007) Promoting children's fruit and vegetable consumption: interventions using the theory of planned behaviour as a framework. Br J Health Psychol 12, 639650.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
85Anderson, AS, Porteous, LE, Foster, E, et al. (2005) The impact of a school-based nutrition education intervention on dietary intake and cognitive and attitudinal variables relating to fruits and vegetables. Public Health Nutr 8, 650656.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
86Horne, PJ, Tapper, K, Lowe, CF, et al. (2004) Increasing children's fruit and vegetable consumption: a peer-modelling and rewards-based intervention. Eur J Clin Nutr 58, 16491660.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
87Prell, HC, Berg, MC, Jonsson, LM, et al. (2005) A school-based intervention to promote dietary change. J Adolesc Health 36, 529.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
88James, J, Thomas, P & Kerr, D (2007) Preventing childhood obesity: two year follow-up results from the Christchurch obesity prevention programme in schools (CHOPPS). BMJ 335, 762.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
89Passmore, S & Harris, G (2005) School nutrition action groups and their effect upon secondary school-aged pupils' food choices. Nutr Bull 30, 364369.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
90Fogarty, AW, Antoniak, M, Venn, AJ, et al. (2007) Does participation in a population-based dietary intervention scheme have a lasting impact on fruit intake in young children? Int J Epidemiol 36, 10801085.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
91Wells, L & Nelson, M (2005) The national school fruit scheme produces short-term but not longer-term increases in fruit consumption in primary school children. Br J Nutr 93, 537542.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
92Turnin, MC, Tauber, MT, Couvaras, O, et al. (2001) Evaluation of microcomputer nutritional teaching games in 1,876 children at school. Diabetes Metab 27, 459464.Google Scholar
93Young, I (1993) Healthy eating policies in schools: an evaluation of effects on pupils' knowledge, attitudes and behaviour. Health Educ J 52, 39.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
94Perez-Rodrigo, C, Wind, M, Hildonen, C, et al. (2005) The pro children intervention: applying the intervention mapping protocol to develop a school-based fruit and vegetable promotion programme. Ann Nutr Metab 49, 267277.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
95Zaza, S, Wright-De Agüero, L, Briss, P, et al. (2000) Data collection instrument and procedure for systematic reviews in the guide to community preventive services. Task force on community preventive services. Am J Prev Med 18, 4474.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
96Rasmussen, M, Krolner, R, Klepp, KI, et al. (2006) Determinants of fruit and vegetable consumption among children and adolescents: a review of the literature. Part I: quantitative studies. Int J Behav Nutr Phys Act 3, 22.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
97Cullen, KW, Baranowski, T, Owens, E, et al. (2003) Availability, accessibility, and preferences for fruit, 100 % fruit juice, and vegetables influence children's dietary behavior. Health Educ Behav 30, 615626.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
98Patrick, H & Nicklas, TA (2005) A review of family and social determinants of children's eating patterns and diet quality. J Am Coll Nutr 24, 8392.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
99van't Veer, P, Kardinaal, AF, Bausch-Goldbohm, RA, et al. (1993) Biomarkers for validation. Eur J Clin Nutr 47, Suppl. 2, S58S63.Google ScholarPubMed
100Livingstone, MB, Robson, PJ & Wallace, JM (2004) Issues in dietary intake assessment of children and adolescents. Br J Nutr 92, Suppl. 2, S213S222.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
101Brug, J (2008) Determinants of healthy eating: motivation, abilities and environmental opportunities. Fam Pract 25, Suppl. 1, i50i55.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
102Kellam, SG & Langevin, DJ (2003) A framework for understanding ‘evidence’ in prevention research and programs. Prev Sci 4, 137153.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
103Heude, B, Lafay, L, Borys, JM, et al. (2003) Time trend in height, weight, and obesity prevalence in school children from Northern France, 1992–2000. Diabetes Metab 29, 235240.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
104Lafay, L, Vray, M, Boute, D, et al. (1998) Food and nutritional data for a population from northern France: the Fleurbaix Laventie Ville Sante (FLVS) Study. Rev Epidemiol Sante Publique 46, 263275.Google ScholarPubMed
105Woolfe, J & Stockley, L (2005) Nutrition health promotion in schools in the UK: learning from food standards agency funded schools research. Health Educ J 64, 229246.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
106Wind, M (2006) The development, implementation and evaluation of a school-based intervention to promote fruit and vegetable intake among 10–13 year-old European schoolchildren. PhD Thesis, Erasmus Universiteit Rotterdam.Google Scholar
107Martens, MK (2005) Krachtvoer. The development, implementation, and evaluation of an educational programme for 12–14-year-old students to promote healthy eating. PhD Thesis, Universiteit Maastricht.Google Scholar
Figure 0

Fig. 1 Flow chart of study selection process. PA, physical activity.

Figure 1

Table 1 Study and intervention characteristics of included studies promoting healthy nutrition in children

Figure 2

Table 2 Study and intervention characteristics of included studies promoting healthy nutrition in adolescents

Figure 3

Table 3 Study design, overall study quality and overall intervention effectiveness on anthropometrics and dietary behaviour of included studies promoting healthy nutrition in children

Figure 4

Table 4 Study design, overall study quality and overall intervention effectiveness on anthropometrics and dietary behaviour of included studies promoting healthy nutrition in adolescents

Figure 5

Table 5 Summary of levels of evidence for the effectiveness of interventions promoting healthy nutrition in children and adolescents, stratified by intervention type and target group