Hostname: page-component-cd9895bd7-mkpzs Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-12-23T15:36:51.725Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Does sweetness exposure drive ‘sweet tooth’?

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  26 February 2024

David J. Mela*
Affiliation:
Valkenswaard, The Netherlands
Davide Risso
Affiliation:
Tate & Lyle PLC, London, UK
*
*Corresponding author: David J. Mela, email [email protected]
Rights & Permissions [Opens in a new window]

Abstract

It is widely believed that exposure to sweetened foods and beverages stimulates the liking and desire for sweetness. Here we provide an updated review of the empirical evidence from human research examining whether exposure to sweet foods or beverages influences subsequent general liking for sweetness (‘sweet tooth’), based on the conclusions of existing systematic reviews and more recent research identified from a structured search of literature. Prior reviews have concluded that the evidence for a relationship between sweet taste exposure and measures of sweet taste liking is equivocal, and more recent primary research generally does not support the view that exposure drives increased liking for sweetness, in adults or children. In intervention trials using a range of designs, acute exposure to sweetness usually has the opposite effect (reducing subsequent liking and desire for sweet taste), while sustained exposures have no significant effects or inconsistent effects. Recent longitudinal observational studies in infants and children also report no significant associations between exposures to sweet foods and beverages with measures of sweet taste preferences. Overall, while it is widely assumed that exposure to sweetness stimulates a greater liking and desire for sweetness, this is not borne out by the balance of empirical evidence. While new research may provide a more robust evidence base, there are also a number of methodological, biological and behavioural considerations that may underpin the apparent absence of a positive relationship between sweetness exposure and liking.

Type
Review
Creative Commons
Creative Common License - CCCreative Common License - BY
This is an Open Access article, distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution licence (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/), which permits unrestricted re-use, distribution and reproduction, provided the original article is properly cited.
Copyright
© The Author(s), 2024. Published by Cambridge University Press on behalf of The Nutrition Society

There are consistent national and international public health recommendations to limit intakes of free or added sugars(Reference Walton, Bell and Re1,Reference Rong, Liao and Zhou2) . However, there is less clarity and consistency in how this should be achieved. Part of this reflects debate around the physiological and health effects of low-energy sweeteners (LES), but there are also concerns about sweetness, and whether exposure to sweetness in the diet facilitates or hinders achievement of public health targets. Concern has been expressed about a ‘sweetening of the global diet’(Reference Popkin and Hawkes3), although the evidence for this may differ depending on the nature of the analyses and consideration of foods v. beverages. In the USA, sweetness of the diet appears to have declined over the period 2001–2018, driven by reductions in sweeteners in beverages and tabletop sweeteners, with little change in foods(Reference Kamil, Wilson and Rehm4). A further analysis concluded that, globally, sweetness and added sugar contents decreased in beverages but increased in packaged foods over the period 2008–2019(Reference Russell, Baker and Grimes5).

While there is a general consensus that high intakes of free sugars (and sugar-sweetened beverages in particular) increase the risks of weight gain and metabolic disease(Reference Mela and Woolner6), evidence on the effects of sweetness itself appears to contrast with this. Numerous reviews reflecting a large volume of research indicate that exposure to higher or lower levels of sweetness in the diet is not significantly associated with energy intake or body weight, and greater individual liking for sweetness generally is not clearly associated with obesity, sugar intakes or diet quality(Reference Higgins, Rawal and Baer7Reference Venditti, Musa-Veloso and Lee15). Nevertheless, it is possible that continued exposure to sweet foods and beverages, or LES in particular, may drive a heightened generalised preference for sweetness (and therefore also sugars) in the diet, a so-called ‘sweet tooth’, which would be counterproductive to public health initiatives. Against this, it is also possible that achievement of targets for sugar reduction is facilitated by continued access to sweet foods and beverages low in free sugars.

Variation in exposure to salt in the diet has been shown to influence preferred saltiness levels(Reference Bobowski16). A corresponding belief that exposure to sweetness in the diet maintains or drives a liking or desire for sweetness is widely expressed in the professional literature(Reference Russell, Baker and Grimes5,Reference Baker, Machado and Santos17Reference Ludwig19) and also given as a basis for contemporary guidance on sweetened products from major public health authorities(2024). These views are often framed as a statement of established fact or accepted conjecture, without reference to an underpinning body of empirical research or reviews.

The intent of this overview is to review and provide a consolidated update on the empirical evidence testing the relationship between sweetness exposure and subsequent liking in human adults and children, consider the mechanisms and research challenges that may influence observations of that relationship and make suggestions for future work on the topic.

Methods

While this is a narrative review, a systematic effort was made to identify recent research publications which measured or intervened in exposures to sweetened foods and beverages, and reported an explicit measure of sweetness liking subsequent to exposure. These measures include sensory hedonic testing (e.g. preferred sweetener level), choice of sweet v. non-sweet foods or reported liking or desire for sweet foods. The topic of sweetness exposure and preferences had last been systematically reviewed by Appleton et al. for publications up to 15 August 2017(Reference Appleton, Tuorila and Bertenshaw25). Systematic searches adapting their syntax were carried out on the Embase® and MEDLINE® databases, using the ProQuest Dialog search platform, for the period August 2017 through 13 February 2024. As a further check on the completeness of the formal search, supplemental searches were carried out using Google Scholar to highlight any papers in this period that cited the available systematic reviews and potentially relevant papers cited by publications newly identified from the formal search. The scope included full publications of controlled trials and longitudinal observational cohort studies in all ages and regions, but excluded patient populations, maternal exposures and languages other than English. Cross-sectional studies were excluded because they are particularly prone to confounding, especially reverse causality (e.g. if liking for sweetness increases exposure), and cross-sectional diet-taste relationships have largely been captured in other contemporary reviews(Reference Tan and Tucker12). Titles and abstracts were reviewed and relevant full publications were extracted by one of the authors. In addition, related ongoing and recently completed trials and systematic reviews were identified by a search of the ClinicalTrials.gov trial registry and PROSPERO register of systematic reviews.

Results

Prior systematic reviews

Three systematic reviews have assessed human studies on the relationship between sweetness exposure and subsequent preferences, and all have come to similar conclusions.

The earliest systematic review focused on the influence of taste exposures in utero and up to age 6 months, based on controlled and uncontrolled intervention studies and cohort studies published through November 2014(Reference Nehring, Kostka and von Kries26). The evidence base included a variety of study designs comprising a diverse range of pre- and post-natal food exposures, only a small number of which explicitly assessed sweet taste. The authors concluded that ‘…whether exposure to sweet and salty tastes early in life may increase acceptance for such tastes in later infancy are equivocal’.

Appleton et al. published the most direct and comprehensive systematic review of the topic, which included fourteen controlled trials and seven population cohort studies published up to August 2017(Reference Appleton, Tuorila and Bertenshaw25). The authors concluded that controlled studies indicate a higher sweet taste exposure may reduce sweetness preference in the short term, but with limited effects in the longer term. The evidence from longitudinal cohort studies and the conclusion overall for the relationship between sweet taste exposure and preferences were judged to be ‘equivocal’.

Most recently, Venditti et al. published a scoping review based on a systematic search of evidence on the determinants of sweetness preference in humans(Reference Venditti, Musa-Veloso and Lee15). From six identified sources that studied the possible effects of prior exposure, only one of these(Reference Vennerød, Almli and Berget27) was published after the systematic review of Appleton et al.(Reference Appleton, Tuorila and Bertenshaw25). On the basis of this smaller sample of the literature, Venditti et al.(Reference Venditti, Musa-Veloso and Lee15) concluded that the research findings ‘are inconsistent’ and highlighted a number of challenges to the interpretation of this body of research.

Recent intervention trials with sweet and non-sweet product exposures

Table 1 gives a summary of the newly identified trials reporting on the effects of exposures to sweet v. non-sweet stimuli since August 2017. Seven publications reported eight studies where exposure was followed by an assessment of generalised sweet taste liking (pleasantness, desire), comprising tasting and rating of sweet (and in some cases also non-sweet) products(Reference Thanarajah, DiFeliceantonio and Albus28Reference Okronipa, Arimond and Young34). In three other studies, the measure was the relative intake (choice) of sweet foods in mixed buffet snacks or meals(Reference Rogers, Ferriday and Irani32,Reference Appleton35,Reference Fantino, Fantino and Matray36) . None of the interventions involving sweet taste exposure resulted in increases in measures of sweet taste liking and, in line with a large volume of previous research, acute exposures to sweetness generally decreased desire for and liking of the same and other sweet stimuli.

Table 1. Intervention trials published since August 2017 testing the effects of exposure to sweetened v. unsweetened test products on subsequent measures of sweet taste liking

LES = Low-energy sweetener; SSB = Sugar-sweetened beverage.

Four additional publications reported on the effects of exposures to sweet v. non-sweet products, followed by assessments only related to sweetness liking or choice within that same product format, either beverages(Reference Kendig, Chow and Martire37Reference Judah, Mullan and Yee39) or an infant feeding supplement in oatmeal(Reference Johnson, Shapiro and Moding40). These generated a mixed pattern of results, mainly no significant effect of sweetness exposure or possible (inconsistent) differences between results for exposure to LES relative to sugar.

Recent observational/longitudinal cohort studies

Only three recent longitudinal cohort studies were identified that reported associations of sweet taste exposure with liking, and these are summarised in Table 2. Two longitudinal studies reporting on sweetness exposures in infancy found no associations with subsequent measures of sweet taste liking either later in infancy(Reference Müller, Chabanet and Zeinstra41) or in pre-adolescent childhood(Reference Yuan, Nicklaus and Forhan42). A further study found no longitudinal relationships between intakes of sugar and liking for milkshakes that varied in sugar and fat contents(Reference Papantoni, Shearrer and Sadler43).

Table 2. Longitudinal cohort studies published since August 2017 testing the association of exposure to sweet foods with liking for sweetness

Ongoing (registered) trials and reviews

A substantial number of additional, potentially relevant intervention trials with as-yet-unpublished results on measures of sweet taste liking were identified through public registries or protocol publications. These trials have a range of different designs and objectives, allocating subjects to diet periods varying in sweetness or free sugar levels, or specifically testing the effects of the use of LES (e.g. v. water). The status of these trials was reported (per 13 February 2024) to be ‘completed’ (ClinicalTrials.gov identifiers NCT05010408, NCT04609657 and NCT04226911(Reference Kjølbæk, Manios and Blaak44)), or with estimated completion dates in 2023 (ClinicalTrials.gov identifier NCT04633681(Reference Gibbons, O’Hara and O’connor45)), 2024 (ClinicalTrials.gov identifiers NCT02591134(Reference Masic, Harrold and Christiansen46,Reference Harrold, Hill and Radu47) , NCT04816955(Reference Boxall, Arden-Close and James48), NCT05672017, NCT04497974(Reference Čad, Tang and de Jong49), NCT05684757 and NCT05932329) or 2025 (ClinicalTrials.gov identifiers NCT04567108 and NCT04079855). Most of the studies have exposure durations of several weeks or months and include only adult participants. In addition to these intervention trials, one relevant systematic review was identified in the PROSPERO register of systematic reviews. This lists a number of research questions on the associations of sugar and sweetener exposures with sweet taste preference during childhood(Reference Smith, Dagali and Llewellyn50). The registered protocol includes both intervention trials and cohort studies, and its status is given as ‘ongoing’.

Discussion

A moderately large and varied body of research has tested for a relationship between sweetness exposure and subsequent liking for sweetness in human adults and children. Taken together with the conclusions from earlier systematic reviews, more recent research is consistent with the view that evidence for this relationship leans towards disconfirmation or is at best equivocal. Professional positions and public health guidance should therefore be cautious in expressing a presumption that sweetness exposure drives a liking for sweetness or otherwise provide underpinning for that view.

None of the recent trials involving acute or sustained sweet exposures followed by assessments of generalised liking found that these exposures increased the liking or choice of sweet stimuli or foods (Table 1). Arguably, test batteries with a range of products best assess the effects of an intervention on generalised ‘sweet tooth’. In line with a large volume of previous research, acute exposures to sweetness generally led to a transient decrease in desire for and liking of the same and other sweet stimuli. Trials where exposures were accompanied by tests for the preferred level of sweetness only within that same (exposed) product format generated less consistent results. While that design may be relevant for that single product type (mainly beverages), it does not allow conclusions to be drawn about a more generalised effect on liking for sweetness in the diet. A further limitation is that this design may be susceptible to a demand artefact, whereby the experience with or knowledge of the intervention itself may prompt subjects to shift their reported liking toward the direction of the product sweetness level they recently experienced as part of the intervention.

Results from acute studies of sweetness exposure are in line with the well-established principle of sensory-specific satiety, whereby exposure to a sensory attribute such as sweetness produces a transient reduction in the relative liking or intake of foods and beverages sharing that attribute(Reference Rolls51). With repeated exposure to sweetness, some decrease in liking due to monotony or boredom might also be hypothesised; however, no relative change in liking was reported in studies where the intervention required subjects to consume sweetened stimuli daily over several weeks (Table 1). Research on the expression and interpretation of food-related monotony has yielded inconsistent results and may be influenced by factors related to the subjects, methods of assessment and nature of test foods(Reference Hetherington, Pirie and Nabb52Reference Stubenitsky, Aaron and Catt56). That literature suggests that acute sensory-specific satiety effects may be sustained or somewhat attenuated over time, but it is not clear whether the ‘monotony’ of repeated exposures mainly manifests as a decrease in actual liking for the target foods or a decrease in desire to consume them. The studies on sweetness identified here generally used commonly consumed food formats, and monotony effects may be more likely to be seen with an increased frequency of foods that are usually infrequently consumed(Reference Hetherington, Pirie and Nabb52). Given that liking for sweetness is innate and unconditioned, and may be reinforced by frequent association with the concomitant intake of energy and macronutrients, the general liking for sweetness may be little affected by variation in exposure to any individual sweet food(s).

The evidence here has not directly tested whether the response to sustained additions or reductions in sweetness in the diet would have similar (opposing) effects. Interventions where sweetness exposure is reduced would seem most relevant to public health interests in reducing intakes of free sugars. In contrast, tests of increased sweetness exposure and longitudinal observational studies are focused more on identifying possible influences on the establishment or maintenance of sweet taste liking. Of the sustained intervention trials in Table 1, three clearly tested the effects of reduced sweetness exposure in adults, mainly where water replaced SSB(Reference Kendig, Chow and Martire37Reference Judah, Mullan and Yee39). In other cases, there was insufficient information to determine whether the interventions represented an increased or decreased exposure to sweetness for the participants(Reference Thanarajah, DiFeliceantonio and Albus28,Reference Carroll, Chen and Templeman31,Reference Fantino, Fantino and Matray36) , or the interventions were a balance of increased and decreased sweetness exposures for different individuals(Reference Appleton, Rajska and Warwick29). All the studies in children used designs where test foods provided a supplemental source of sweet (relative to non-sweet or no) exposures(Reference Okronipa, Arimond and Arnold33,Reference Okronipa, Arimond and Young34,Reference Johnson, Shapiro and Moding40) .

There were very few new longitudinal cohort studies (Table 2). Neither of the two fairly large studies found significant associations of sweet taste exposures in infancy with later measures of liking. A further study reported no longitudinal associations between sugar intakes and liking for sweet stimuli in an adolescent population. One additional study was excluded, although it had a longitudinal element. Vennerød et al. collected data on a cohort of children at mean ages of ∼4 and ∼5 years(Reference Vennerød, Almli and Berget27). Path modelling was applied to explain children’s preferences for sweetness levels in drinks and chocolate, based on latent factors including concurrent measures of their parents’ attitudes to children’s foods and eating, and the child’s reported intake of foods including sweet foods and snacks. The authors report a small but statistically significant, positive association between sweet food exposure and sweetness preferences; however, it appears only the age 5 (thus cross-sectional) data were used in the analysis model, which also assumed a priori a direction of causal interpretation (from exposures to preferences).

The conclusions of earlier systematic reviews as well as more recent research therefore suggest that sweetness exposure makes only a limited contribution to observed variance in sweet taste liking or preferences. However, many of the intervention trials were small, and the various intervention and observational studies were diverse in design, intent and interpretation, with few explicitly designed to address this specific research question. Nevertheless, the general conclusions from the current research corpus contrast with widely held beliefs about sweetness exposure as a driver of sweet taste liking. It is therefore relevant to consider possible reasons why there is poor empirical support for this ‘expected’ relationship.

Limitations in methodologies

It is possible that the ‘expected’ relationship (sweet taste exposure → sweet taste liking) exists, but is obscured by limitations in research design and methods related to the exposures or outcome measures. For example, the variation in sweetness exposures in intervention trials may have been too narrow in size and scope, often being limited to just a single food or beverage format. However, similar results (absence of effect) have come from studies involving variation in whole-diet sweetness exposures(Reference Appleton, Tuorila and Bertenshaw25). In observational studies, reported intake data may not be a valid reflection of habitual diet intakes and must further be translated into a measure of orosensory perceptual exposure to sweet taste. Use of intakes of added sugars or sweet foods and beverages as a proxy indicator of sweet taste liking is highly problematic. Food choices, especially in children, may reflect household food offerings or availability/affordability, and specific ‘sweet’ or sweetener-containing products may vary greatly in their actual sweetness. The availability of food sensory profile databases(Reference Lease, Hendrie and Poelman57,Reference Martin, Visalli and Lange58) is a major improvement over earlier research, which made assumptions of sweetness based on the presence of sugars and sweeteners in the diet. Nevertheless, there is no standardised approach for measuring the sweetness of the total diet, making it difficult to combine and compare data across different studies(Reference Trumbo, Appleton and De Graaf11,Reference Iatridi, Hayes and Yeomans59) . Mean sweetness ratings from trained panels in an isolated setting cannot fully capture the momentary time-intensity profile of sweetness experienced by an individual eating the same food or beverage under real-life conditions.

There are also differences in the methods used to measure individual sweet taste ‘liking’. For the purpose of this review and others(Reference Appleton, Tuorila and Bertenshaw25), these methods included sensory hedonic testing, as well as the reported liking, desire or choice and intake of sweet (relative to non-sweet) foods. This diversity of outcome measures reflects the nature of the primary evidence base, and it appears here that the results from different outcome measures generated similar (or at least not clearly dissimilar) conclusions. Nevertheless, variation in how ‘liking’ is operationalised leaves questions as to what measures are most sensitive, appropriate or meaningful (e.g. relevant to behaviour) and impedes direct comparisons and quantitative meta-analyses of primary research. Conceptual distinctions can be made amongst ‘liking’, ‘desire’ (wanting or incentive motivation) and ‘preference’ (choice), and there are different ways to operationalise these in research(Reference Mela60,Reference Oustric, Thivel and Dalton61) . Although the practical relevance of these distinctions may only be apparent under certain conditions, further attention to alternative methods and greater standardisation across studies may advance the field. Sensory hedonic (taste) testing under controlled testing conditions is often seen as a ‘gold standard’, but has important limitations as a measure of generalised sweetness liking, particularly where only a single test medium is used (e.g. sugar in water). Individual differences in the perception of sweetness can vary with sweetener type and concentrations, the context and methods used to present stimuli and collect ratings, and food matrix in which the taste stimuli are delivered(Reference Trumbo, Appleton and De Graaf11). Recent evidence also suggests specific differences in methodologies for determining preferred sweetness levels may also influence apparent relationships with dietary intakes(Reference Peng, Ginieis and Abeywickrema62).

Underlying much of this research is a presumed (causal) relationship between sweet taste perception and diet, particularly sweet taste liking and free sugar intakes. While this seems intuitive, it has proven difficult to demonstrate robust, consistent relationships in practice. A recent, comprehensive systematic review concluded that measures of preferred sweetness concentrations had inconsistent associations with diet(Reference Tan and Tucker12). Measures of taste preferences based on preferences for real foods (choice tasks) may be more ecologically relevant and perhaps predictive of habitual diet, but are also open to more sources of variability and confounding(Reference Cheon, Reister and Hunter14).

In addition to the general methodological issues, all the evidence here has come from populations in the USA, western Europe and Australia, with the exception of 2 related studies in Ghanaian children(Reference Okronipa, Arimond and Arnold33,Reference Okronipa, Arimond and Young34) . It is possible that other populations, with perhaps much lower or higher habitual consumption of sweetened foods and beverages, may respond differently to changes in sweetness exposures.

Despite the possible methodological limitations, the general research findings are presumed to be valid, suggesting that exposure to sweetness has little impact on subsequent generalised liking for sweetness. There are a number of mechanisms that could explain why that may be true and why presumed analogies to salt exposure may not be appropriate.

Developmental changes and interindividual differences

There is a wide range of within-person factors that have been shown to have an influence on sweet taste liking(Reference Venditti, Musa-Veloso and Lee15). Taste cells and receptors start to develop before birth, and an innate unlearned general preference for sweet-tasting stimuli is observed not only in infancy but even in utero (Reference Mennella, Bobowski and Reed63,Reference Ustun, Reissland and Covey64) . It may simply be difficult to see the effects of exposure against a strong background of universal liking for sweetness and established individual phenotypic variation within that. Preferences for sweet-tasting foods are observed in children across all cultures, declining from mid-adolescence for reasons that are not clear(Reference Mennella, Bobowski and Reed63) and increasing again in older adults (> 60 years), possibly linked to reduced taste sensitivity and discrimination(Reference Venditti, Musa-Veloso and Lee15).

Within the innate general liking for sweetness, there are also sweet ‘likers’ and ‘dislikers’, and others showing intermediate responses, as defined by their acceptance of increasing concentrations of sucrose or other sweet tastants(Reference Armitage, Iatridi and Yeomans8,Reference Peng, Ginieis and Abeywickrema62,Reference Bachmanov, Bosak and Floriano65) . Sweet ‘likers’ show rises in liking with increasing sucrose concentrations until an eventual plateau, while sweet ‘dislikers’ show an increasing aversion to higher concentrations. Between these extremes are patterns of liking that are relatively stable or follow an inverted U-shape across rising concentrations. These differences in sweet-liking phenotypes have been observed across different cultures(Reference Garneau, Nuessle and Mendelsberg66), and a key question here is whether this variation reflects differential exposures. The genetic basis for sweet liker phenotypes and sweet liking in general appear to be complex, but factors including polymorphisms in the TAS1R3 sweet receptor gene may explain up to 50 % of the variation of individuals’ liking for sweetness(Reference Venditti, Musa-Veloso and Lee15,Reference Bachmanov, Bosak and Floriano65,Reference Yang, Kraft and Shen67Reference Keskitalo, Tuorila and Spector70) . Numerous studies have also investigated the cross-sectional relationships between sweet taste liking phenotypes and dietary intake of sugars or sweet foods, but the results have been inconsistent, and differences speculated to be attributable to habitual diets, variation in the methods used to define these phenotypes or genetic background(Reference Iatridi, Hayes and Yeomans59,Reference Garneau, Nuessle and Mendelsberg66Reference Diószegi, Llanaj and Ádány68,Reference Keskitalo, Tuorila and Spector70) . Unfortunately, the paucity of longitudinal data limits conclusions on the possible direction of causality of associations with dietary sweetness exposures(Reference Armitage, Iatridi and Yeomans8).

Sweet v. Salt taste: differences in perception and biology

Preferences for salty taste can be altered after sustained changes in sodium intakes, although corresponding changes or dietary associations with the perceived intensity of salty stimuli are not consistently observed(Reference Bertino, Beauchamp and Engelman71Reference Tan, Sotirelis and Bojeh74). Some studies have reported that reductions in sweet taste exposure led to enhanced ratings of sweetness intensity, despite having no significant effects on liking(Reference Ebbeling, Feldman and Steltz38,Reference Wise, Nattress and Flammer75) . There is not a clear explanation for the lack of corresponding effects of exposure interventions on liking and perceived intensity in some studies, either for saltiness or sweetness(Reference Blais, Pangborn and Borhani73,Reference Wise, Nattress and Flammer75) . Although parallels may be assumed between the effects of sweet and salty taste exposures, there are important differences in their nature and mechanisms. This may be related to the suggested evolutionary relevance of sweetness and saltiness; that is as signals for sources of sugars and sodium, respectively(Reference Breslin76). While sodium is a nutrient required for maintenance of electrolyte balance and other functions, sugars are not essential in the diet. Also, in contrast to sugars, salty stimuli do not contribute to energy intake and are only required in low amounts, and higher concentrations become aversive by recruiting sour and bitter pathways(Reference Oka, Butnaru and von Buchholtz77).

From a molecular perspective, sweet tastants, through their multiple binding sites situated either at the extra-cellular and/or transmembrane regions, are captured in taste cells by G protein-coupled receptors, mainly taste receptor type 1 (TAS1R), members 2 (T1R2) and 3 (T1R3). This generates a complex intracellular signalling pathway that finally leads to depolarisation and nerve signals interpreted centrally as ‘sweet’ messages(Reference Roper and Chaudhari78). In contrast, salty tastants are conveyed through ion channels, with entry of sodium (Na+) triggering depolarisation and a ‘salty’ message(Reference Oka, Butnaru and von Buchholtz77,Reference Roper and Chaudhari78) . Thus, sweet stimuli interact with dedicated receptors to generate second messengers while the salty stimulus itself is transported into the cell, involving different neurons and brain activation patterns(Reference Spetter, Smeets and de Graaf79). Lastly, while salt intake is strictly regulated through a highly selective and saturable sodium transport mechanism, the broad range of sweet-tasting stimuli is less strictly regulated and mainly limited by availability and post-ingestive satiety(Reference Spetter, Smeets and de Graaf79,Reference Bigiani, Ghiaroni and Fieni80) . These differences in the biology of sweet and salt taste might underlie differences in the observed effects of variations in tastant exposure on measures of liking in human studies.

Low-energy sweeteners v. Sugars

There are trends toward continuing increases in global sales of LES and their relative contribution toward sweetness in the diet(Reference Russell, Baker and Grimes5). It is therefore relevant to consider whether the relationship between sweetness exposures and liking is influenced by the source of sweetness. Based largely on a subset of animal studies it has been suggested that exposure to LES specifically (v. sugars) may specifically influence the development of sweet taste perception and liking(Reference Yunker, Patel and Page81Reference Chometton, Tsan and Hayes83). A statement that early consumption of LES may affect later preferences for sugars is also expressed in recent child-feeding guidance from the WHO(24). The leading hypotheses posit that LES consumption may alter the development of sweet taste receptor expression and glucose sensing, or ‘uncouple’ sweet taste from the energy value of sweet foods in nature(Reference Yunker, Patel and Page81,Reference Chometton, Tsan and Hayes83,Reference Sylvetsky, Conway and Malhotra84) . However, many of the underpinning elements of these hypotheses have limited or equivocal empirical support, and their replicability and interpretation have been challenged(Reference Boakes, Kendig and Martire85Reference Glendinning, Hart and Lee87).

The types of compounds that may elicit a sweet taste (not only sugars and LES but also specific fibres, proteins and amino acids) are diverse in their energy value, chemistry, sweetness profiles, bioactivity and metabolism. While they activate the same taste receptors, they do not necessarily share the same binding sites or affinities(Reference Schweiger, Grüneis and Treml88). Recent systematic reviews of brain imaging studies data indicate uncertainty as to possible differences in neural responses to LES and caloric sweeteners; however, there are many limitations to that evidence base, and questions around the robustness and interpretation of results(Reference Roberts, Giesbrecht and Fallon89,Reference Yeung and Wong90) . It is not yet clear how these types of observations might be associated with subsequent physiological or behavioural responses. There is also mixed evidence regarding the stimulation of cephalic phase responses by specific LES relative to caloric sweeteners(Reference Lasschuijt, Mars and de Graaf91Reference Pullicin, Glendinning and Lim93), though LES in general appear to have limited effects on post-prandial gut hormone or other physiological responses(Reference Zhang, Noronha and Khan94). The post-ingestive responses to sugars reflect the nature of the carbohydrate, not the perceived sweetness(Reference Grüneis, Schweiger and Galassi95). Lastly, while it has been suggested that LES corrupt the natural relationship between sweetness and energy content, sweetness is poorly predictive of the energy content of human foods and diets, even when LES are excluded(Reference Rogers96,Reference van Langeveld, Gibbons and Koelliker97) .

The trials described in Table 1 tested foods or beverages containing sugars(Reference Thanarajah, DiFeliceantonio and Albus28,Reference Chaaban and Andersen30,Reference Carroll, Chen and Templeman31,Reference Okronipa, Arimond and Arnold33,Reference Okronipa, Arimond and Young34,Reference Johnson, Shapiro and Moding40) , LES(Reference Appleton, Rajska and Warwick29,Reference Rogers, Ferriday and Irani32,Reference Fantino, Fantino and Matray36) or (separately) both LES and sugars(Reference Rogers, Ferriday and Irani32,Reference Appleton35,Reference Kendig, Chow and Martire37Reference Judah, Mullan and Yee39) as exposures and compared these to unsweetened controls. The general pattern of results was similar for LES and sugar exposures, and the examples where these deviated (all using beverages) generated inconsistent results. Kendig et al. found reductions in liking for higher sugar concentrations following sustained exposure to water or SSB, but not LES(Reference Kendig, Chow and Martire37). In contrast, Ebbeling et al. reported significant decreases in preferred sugar concentrations following sustained exposure to LES and water, but not SSB(Reference Ebbeling, Feldman and Steltz38). Appleton reported significantly lower percent energy from sweet foods following a single exposure to SSB but not LES relative to water (but no comparison of LES to SSB)(Reference Appleton35). The literature search also identified two additional recent trials that specifically compared the effects of LES v. sugar exposures, but had no non-sweet comparators. Dalenberg et al. found no differences in repeated exposure to LES v. sugar on the preference for or perceived intensity of sucrose solutions(Reference Dalenberg, Patel and Denis98). Casperson et al. reported that following a single acute exposure to LES or sugar with a meal, LES comparatively increased the relative reward value of sweet v. salty/savoury snacks, but with no differences between LES and sugar in their effects on ratings of desire to eat sweet, salty, savoury or fatty foods, nor amounts of sugar or of sweet v. salty/savoury foods consumed(Reference Casperson, Johnson and Roemmich99). Taken together, most of the evidence here from human trials on sweetness exposure and liking indicates largely similar results for LES and sugars. However, there are exceptions to this, and there remains active debate around this question, as well as whether effects might differ for specific LES. Given widespread public health guidance focused on reducing sugars and an increasing use of LES in some product sectors, this remains a relevant topic for further research.

Limitations of this review

This was not a formal systematic review of literature, though an effort was made using multiple approaches to identify any potentially relevant publications, and no publication testing the research question as framed was intentionally excluded. However, it is possible some relevant sources have been overlooked, in part because taste or liking measures are usually not a primary outcome in studies on sugars or sweeteners. There was also no attempt to grade the evidence, for example using a quality assessment tool or weighting of the included studies. Over the next few years, results are expected from a number of studies where testing for effects of exposures on sweet taste liking is an a priori objective, and those results may solidify or shift the current balance of evidence.

Research recommendations

  • Given the volume of newer research and ongoing studies, it would be timely to undertake new high-quality, formal systematic reviews of this research question (with meta-analysis if possible) in the next 2–4 years. Where possible, these should also consider the source of sweetness (sugars or specific LES).

  • Research using sweetness exposure as an independent variable should be based on objective data on perceived sweetness and consider the sources of sweetness, rather than using intakes of sugars or sweeteners as a proxy for the sweetness of foods or diets(Reference Trumbo, Appleton and De Graaf11,Reference Appleton, Tuorila and Bertenshaw25) . Recent advances in the availability of large data sets profiling the sensory attributes of common foods are already facilitating analyses of taste-diet-health relationships(Reference Müller, Chabanet and Zeinstra41).

  • Measures to assess generalised sweet taste liking should include a range of sweet and non-sweet stimuli. The comparability of research and its use in quantitative meta-analyses would be facilitated by greater standardisation and validation of hedonic (liking, preference, choice) methodologies.

  • The priority for observational research should be on longitudinal cohort data where available. Data analysis approaches should be pre-planned and registered, with careful consideration given to the selection of comparisons, covariates and models, and sources of potential confounding. Although not reviewed here, there is a gap in retrospective or prospective analyses assessing infant and child taste preferences in relation to prior maternal exposures to LES. Where possible these analyses would benefit from consideration of specific LES (due to differences in the potential for foetal or infant exposures).

Conclusions

Despite the widespread presumption of a ‘sweet exposure → sweet tooth’ causal pathway, the balance of evidence from recent primary research publications, added to evidence captured in earlier systematic reviews, does not provide empirical support that direction of relationship. Or, at best, the evidence for such a relationship is equivocal. The present conclusions are similar for research in adults and children, for LES and sugars, and from both intervention and cohort studies. Moreover, at least in the short term, sweetness exposure consistently suppresses the desire for sweetness. There are a number of limitations to the available research base, which may in part be addressed by future studies; however, there are also a number of plausible bio-behavioural explanations why sweet taste exposure may explain little of the observed variance in sweet taste liking in humans.

Acknowledgements

The authors are grateful for helpful comments from colleagues on earlier drafts of the manuscript.

Author D. J. M. received no specific grant or other remuneration from any funding agency, commercial or not-for-profit sectors for his part in drafting of this review. Author D. R. had financial support as an employee of Tate & Lyle PLC, which also supported any publication fees.

Author D. J. M. is a former employee and current shareholder in Unilever and has carried out consultancy work for Unilever, Cargill Inc., Danone, CBC Israel and Tate & Lyle PLC, providing scientific expertise in the areas of sugars and sweeteners. He is an (unpaid) member of the Scientific Industrial Advisory Board for the European Commission project SWEET, which studies the benefits and risks of sweeteners and sweetness enhancers. He was a co-author of a systematic review (Appleton et al., 2018) cited in this review. Author D. R. is employed at Tate & Lyle PLC.

Both authors shared in formulating the research question, interpreting the evidence and drafting the manuscript. Author D. J. M. was primarily responsible for carrying out systematic searches of the literature and research registries. Both authors take responsibility for the final content.

Footnotes

Retired.

References

Walton, J, Bell, H, Re, R et al. (2021) Current perspectives on global sugars consumption: definitions, recommendations, population intakes, challenges and future direction. Nutr Res Rev 36, 122.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rong, S, Liao, Y, Zhou, J et al. (2021) Comparison of dietary guidelines among 96 countries worldwide. Trends Food Sci Technol 109, 219229.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Popkin, BM & Hawkes, C (2016) Sweetening of the global diet, particularly beverages: patterns, trends, and policy responses. Lancet Diab Endocrinol 4, 174186.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kamil, A, Wilson, AR & Rehm, CD (2021) Estimated sweetness in US diet among children and adults declined from 2001 to 2018: a serial cross-sectional surveillance study using NHANES 2001–2018. Front Nutr 8, 777857.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Russell, C, Baker, P, Grimes, C et al. (2023) Global trends in added sugars and non-nutritive sweetener use in the packaged food supply: drivers and implications for public health. Public Health Nutr 26, 952964.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Mela, DJ & Woolner, EM (2018) Perspective: total, added, or free? What kind of sugars should we be talking about? Adv Nutr 9, 6369.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Higgins, KA, Rawal, R, Baer, DJ et al. (2022) Scoping review and evidence map of the relation between exposure to dietary sweetness and body weight-related outcomes in adults. Adv Nutr 13, 23412356.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Armitage, RM, Iatridi, V & Yeomans, MR (2021) Understanding sweet-liking phenotypes and their implications for obesity: narrative review and future directions. Physiol Behav 235, 113398.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cox, DN, Hendrie, GA & Carty, D (2016) Sensitivity, hedonics and preferences for basic tastes and fat amongst adults and children of differing weight status: a comprehensive review. Food Qual Pref 48, 359367.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kamil, A & Wilson, AR (2021) Sweet taste perceptions and preferences may not be associated with food intakes or obesity. Nutr Today 56, 6269.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Trumbo, PR, Appleton, KM, De Graaf, K et al. (2021) Perspective: measuring sweetness in foods, beverages, and diets: toward understanding the role of sweetness in health. Adv Nutr 12, 343354.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Tan, S-Y & Tucker, RM (2019) Sweet taste as a predictor of dietary intake: a systematic review. Nutrients 11, 94.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Liem, DG & Russell, CG (2019) The influence of taste liking on the consumption of nutrient rich and nutrient poor foods. Front Nutr 6, 174.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cheon, E, Reister, EJ, Hunter, SR et al. (2021) Finding the sweet spot: measurement, modification, and application of sweet hedonics in humans. Adv Nutr 12, 23582371.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Venditti, C, Musa-Veloso, K, Lee, HY et al. (2020) Determinants of sweetness preference: a scoping review of human studies. Nutrients 12, 718.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bobowski, N (2015) Shifting human salty taste preference: potential opportunities and challenges in reducing dietary salt intake of Americans. Chemosens Percept 8, 112116.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Baker, P, Machado, P, Santos, T et al. (2020) Ultra-processed foods and the nutrition transition: global, regional and national trends, food systems transformations and political economy drivers. Obes Rev 21, e13126.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lutter, CK, Grummer-Strawn, L & Rogers, L (2021) Complementary feeding of infants and young children 6 to 23 months of age. Nutr Rev 79, 825846.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ludwig, DS (2009) Artificially sweetened beverages: cause for concern. JAMA 302, 24772478.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
World Health Organization Regional Office for the Eastern Mediterranean (2016) Policy Statement and Recommended Actions for Lowering Sugar Intake and Reducing Prevalence of Type 2 Diabetes and Obesity in the Eastern Mediterranean Region. WHO-EM/NUT/273/E. https://applications.emro.who.int/dsaf/EMROPUB_2016_en_18687.pdf (accessed 30 November 2023).Google Scholar
Pan American Health Organization (2016) Pan American Health Organization Nutrient Profile Model. Washington, DC; ISBN 978–92–75–11873–3; https://iris.paho.org/bitstream/handle/10665.2/18621/9789275118733_eng.pdf PAHO (accessed 13 February 2024).Google Scholar
Health Canada (2023) Sugar Substitutes and Healthy Eating. https://food-guide.canada.ca/en/tips-for-healthy-eating/sugar-substitutes-and-healthy-eating/ (accessed 30 November 2023).Google Scholar
World Health Organization (2023) WHO Guideline for Complementary Feeding of Infants and Young Children 6–23 Months of Age. Geneva: World Health Organization.Google Scholar
Appleton, KM, Tuorila, H, Bertenshaw, E et al. (2018) Sweet taste exposure and the subsequent acceptance and preference for sweet taste in the diet: systematic review of the published literature. Am J Clin Nutr 107, 405419.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Nehring, I, Kostka, T, von Kries, R et al. (2015) Impacts of in utero and early infant taste experiences on later taste acceptance: a systematic review. J Nutr 145, 12711279.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Vennerød, FFF, Almli, VL, Berget, I et al. (2017) Do parents form their children’s sweet preference? The role of parents and taste sensitivity on preferences for sweetness in pre-schoolers. Food Qual Pref 62, 172182.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Thanarajah, SE, DiFeliceantonio, AG, Albus, K et al. (2023) Habitual daily intake of a sweet and fatty snack modulates reward processing in humans. Cell Metab 35, 571584.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Appleton, KM, Rajska, J, Warwick, SM et al. (2022) No effects of sweet taste exposure at breakfast for 3 weeks on pleasantness, desire for, sweetness or intake of other sweet foods: a randomised controlled trial. Br J Nutr 127, 14281438.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Chaaban, N & Andersen, BV (2021) Sensory specific desires. The role of sensory taste exposure in desire for food with a similar or different taste profile. Foods 10, 3005.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Carroll, HA, Chen, YC, Templeman, IS et al. (2020) Effect of plain v. sugar-sweetened breakfast on energy balance and metabolic health: a randomized crossover trial. Obesity 28, 740748.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Rogers, PJ, Ferriday, D, Irani, B et al. (2020) Sweet satiation: acute effects of consumption of sweet drinks on appetite for and intake of sweet and non-sweet foods. Appetite 149, 104631.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Okronipa, H, Arimond, M, Arnold, CD et al. (2019) Exposure to a slightly sweet lipid-based nutrient supplement during early life does not increase the level of sweet taste most preferred among 4-to 6-year-old Ghanaian children: follow-up of a randomized controlled trial. Am J Clin Nutr 109, 12241232.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Okronipa, H, Arimond, M, Young, RR et al. (2019) Exposure to a slightly sweet lipid-based nutrient supplement during early life does not increase the preference for or consumption of sweet foods and beverages by 4–6-year-old Ghanaian preschool children: follow-up of a randomized controlled trial. J Nutr 149, 532541.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Appleton, KM (2021) Repeated exposure to and subsequent consumption of sweet taste: reanalysis of test meal intake data following the repeated consumption of sweet v. non-sweet beverages. Physiol Behav 229, 113221.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Fantino, M, Fantino, A, Matray, M et al. (2018) Beverages containing low energy sweeteners do not differ from water in their effects on appetite, energy intake and food choices in healthy, non-obese French adults. Appetite 125, 557565.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kendig, MD, Chow, JY, Martire, SI et al. (2023) Switching from sugar-to artificially-sweetened beverages: a 12-week trial. Nutrients 15, 2191.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ebbeling, CB, Feldman, HA, Steltz, SK et al. (2020) Effects of sugar-Sweetened, artificially Sweetened, and Unsweetened beverages on cardiometabolic risk factors, body composition, and sweet taste preference: a randomized controlled trial. J Am Heart Assoc 9, e015668.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Judah, G, Mullan, B, Yee, M et al. (2020) A habit-based randomised controlled trial to reduce sugar-sweetened beverage consumption: the impact of the substituted beverage on behaviour and habit strength. Int J Behav Med 27, 623635.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Johnson, SL, Shapiro, AL, Moding, KJ et al. (2021) Infant and toddler consumption of sweetened and unsweetened lipid nutrient supplements after 2-week home repeated exposures. J Nutr 151, 28252834.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Müller, C, Chabanet, C, Zeinstra, GG et al. (2023) The sweet tooth of infancy: is sweetness exposure related to sweetness liking in infants up to 12 months of age? Br J Nutr 129, 14621472.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Yuan, WL, Nicklaus, S, Forhan, A et al. (2021) Associations between infant dietary intakes and liking for sweetness and fattiness sensations in 8-to-12-year-old children. Nutrients 13, 2659.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Papantoni, A, Shearrer, GE, Sadler, JR et al. (2021) Longitudinal associations between taste sensitivity, taste liking, dietary intake and BMI in adolescents. Front Psychol 12, 597704.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kjølbæk, L, Manios, Y, Blaak, EE et al. (2022) Protocol for a multicentre, parallel, randomised, controlled trial on the effect of sweeteners and sweetness enhancers on health, obesity and safety in overweight adults and children: the sweet project. BMJ Open 12, e061075.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gibbons, C, O’Hara, B, O’connor, D et al. (2022) Acute and repeated impact of sweeteners and sweetness enhancers in solid and semi-solid foods on appetite: protocol for a multicentre, cross-over, RCT in people with overweight/obesity–the SWEET Project. BMJ Open 12, e063903.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Masic, U, Harrold, J, Christiansen, P et al. (2017) EffectS of non-nutritive sWeetened beverages on appetITe during aCtive weigHt loss (SWITCH): protocol for a randomized, controlled trial assessing the effects of non-nutritive sweetened beverages compared to water during a 12-week weight loss period and a follow up weight maintenance period. Contemp Clin Trials 53, 8088.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Harrold, JA, Hill, S, Radu, C et al. (2023) Effects of non-nutritive sweetened beverages v. water after a 12-week weight-loss program: a randomized controlled trial. Obesity 31, 19962008.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Boxall, LR, Arden-Close, E, James, J et al. (2022) Protocol: the effects of nutrient- v. food- v. food-substitution-based dietary recommendations for reducing free sugar intakes, on free sugar intakes, dietary profiles and sweet taste outcomes: a randomised controlled trial. Nutr Health (Epublication ahead of print version 12 July 2022).CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Čad, EM, Tang, CS, de Jong, HB et al. (2023) Study protocol of the sweet tooth study, randomized controlled trial with partial food provision on the effect of low, regular and high dietary sweetness exposure on sweetness preferences in Dutch adults. BMC Public Health 23, 119.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Smith, A, Dagali, M, Llewellyn, C et al. (2020) Free Sugar and Non-Nutritive Sweetener Intake During Childhood – Effects and Associations with Adiposity, Appetite and Preference for Sweet Taste: A Systematic Review. PROSPERO 2020 CRD42020185780 https://www.crd.york.ac.uk/prospero/display_record.php?ID=CRD42020185780 (accessed 13 February 2024).Google Scholar
Rolls, BJ (1986) Sensory-specific satiety. Nutr Rev 44, 93101.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hetherington, MM, Pirie, LM & Nabb, S (2002) Stimulus satiation: effects of repeated exposure to foods on pleasantness and intake. Appetite 38, 1928.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hetherington, MM, Bell, A & Rolls, BJ (2000) Effects of repeat consumption on pleasantness, preference and intake. Br Food J 102, 507521.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Tey, SL, Brown, RC, Gray, AR et al. (2012) Long-term consumption of high energy-dense snack foods on sensory-specific satiety and intake. Am J Clin Nutr 95, 10381047.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Zandstra, E, Weegels, M, Van Spronsen, A et al. (2004) Scoring or boring? Predicting boredom through repeated in-home consumption. Food Qual Pref 15, 549557.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Stubenitsky, K, Aaron, J, Catt, S et al. (1999) Effect of information and extended use on the acceptance of reduced-fat products. Food Qual Pref 10, 367376.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lease, H, Hendrie, GA, Poelman, AA et al. (2016) A sensory-diet database: a tool to characterise the sensory qualities of diets. Food Qual Pref 49, 2032.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Martin, C, Visalli, M, Lange, C et al. (2014) Creation of a food taste database using an in-home ‘taste’ profile method. Food Qual Pref 36, 7080.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Iatridi, V, Hayes, JE & Yeomans, MR (2019) Quantifying sweet taste liker phenotypes: time for some consistency in the classification criteria. Nutrients 11, 129.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Mela, DJ (2006) Eating for pleasure or just wanting to eat? Reconsidering sensory hedonic responses as a driver of obesity. Appetite 47, 1017.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Oustric, P, Thivel, D, Dalton, M et al. (2020) Measuring food preference and reward: application and cross-cultural adaptation of the Leeds Food Preference Questionnaire in human experimental research. Food Qual Pref 80, 103824.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Peng, M, Ginieis, R, Abeywickrema, S et al. (2023) Rejection thresholds for sweetness reduction in a model drink predict dietary sugar intake. Food Qual Pref 110, 104965.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Mennella, JA, Bobowski, NK & Reed, DR (2016) The development of sweet taste: from biology to hedonics. Rev Endocr Metab Disord 17, 171178.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ustun, B, Reissland, N, Covey, J et al. (2022) Flavor sensing in utero and emerging discriminative behaviors in the human fetus. Psychol Sci 33, 16511663.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bachmanov, AA, Bosak, NP, Floriano, WB et al. (2011) Genetics of sweet taste preferences. Flavour Fragance J 26, 286294.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Garneau, NL, Nuessle, TM, Mendelsberg, BJ et al. (2018) Sweet liker status in children and adults: consequences for beverage intake in adults. Food Qual Pref 65, 175180.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Yang, Q, Kraft, M, Shen, Y et al. (2019) Sweet liking status and PROP taster status impact emotional response to sweetened beverage. Food Qual Pref 75, 133144.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Diószegi, J, Llanaj, E & Ádány, R (2019) Genetic background of taste perception, taste preferences, and its nutritional implications: a systematic review. Front Genet 10, 1272.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hwang, L-D, Lin, C, Gharahkhani, P et al. (2019) New insight into human sweet taste: a genome-wide association study of the perception and intake of sweet substances. Am J Clin Nutr 109, 17241737.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Keskitalo, K, Tuorila, H, Spector, TD et al. (2007) Same genetic components underlie different measures of sweet taste preference. Am J Clin Nutr 86, 16631669.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bertino, M, Beauchamp, G & Engelman, K (1982) Long-term reduction in dietary sodium alters the taste of salt. Am J Clin Nutr 36, 11341144.Google ScholarPubMed
Bertino, M, Beauchamp, GK & Engelman, K (1986) Increasing dietary salt alters salt taste preference. Physiol Behav 38, 203213.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Blais, CA, Pangborn, RM, Borhani, NO et al. (1986) Effect of dietary sodium restriction on taste responses to sodium chloride: a longitudinal study. Am J Clin Nutr 44, 232243.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Tan, S-Y, Sotirelis, E, Bojeh, R et al. (2021) Is dietary intake associated with salt taste function and perception in adults? A systematic review. Food Qual Pref 92, 104174.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Wise, PM, Nattress, L, Flammer, LJ et al. (2016) Reduced dietary intake of simple sugars alters perceived sweet taste intensity but not perceived pleasantness. Am J Clin Nutr 103, 5060.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Breslin, PA (2013) An evolutionary perspective on food and human taste. Curr Biol 23, R409R418.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Oka, Y, Butnaru, M, von Buchholtz, L et al. (2013) High salt recruits aversive taste pathways. Nat 494, 472475.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Roper, SD & Chaudhari, N (2017) Taste buds: cells, signals and synapses. Nat Rev Neurosci 18, 485497.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Spetter, MS, Smeets, PA, de Graaf, C et al. (2010) Representation of sweet and salty taste intensity in the brain. Chem Senses 35, 831840.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bigiani, A, Ghiaroni, V & Fieni, F (2003) Channels as taste receptors in vertebrates. Prog Biophys Mol Biol 83, 193225.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Yunker, AG, Patel, R & Page, KA (2020) Effects of non-nutritive sweeteners on sweet taste processing and neuroendocrine regulation of eating behavior. Curr Nutr Rep 9, 278289.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Moriconi, E, Feraco, A, Marzolla, V et al. (2020) Neuroendocrine and metabolic effects of low-calorie and non-calorie sweeteners. Front Endocrinol (Lausanne) 11, 444.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Chometton, S, Tsan, L, Hayes, AM et al. (2023) Early-life influences of low-calorie sweetener consumption on sugar taste. Physiol Behav 264, 114133.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Sylvetsky, AC, Conway, EM, Malhotra, S et al. (2017) Development of sweet taste perception: implications for artificial sweetener use. Endocr Dev 32, 8799.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Boakes, RA, Kendig, MD, Martire, SI et al. (2016) Sweetening yoghurt with glucose, but not with saccharin, promotes weight gain and increased fat pad mass in rats. Appetite 105, 114128.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Glendinning, JI (2018) Oral post-oral actions of low-calorie sweeteners: a tale of contradictions and controversies. Obesity 26, S9S17.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Glendinning, JI, Hart, SA, Lee, H et al. (2019) Low calorie sweeteners cause only limited metabolic effects in mice. Am J Physiol Regul Integr Comp Physiol 318, R70R80.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Schweiger, K, Grüneis, V, Treml, J et al. (2020) Sweet taste antagonist lactisole administered in combination with sucrose, but not glucose, increases energy intake and decreases peripheral serotonin in male subjects. Nutrients 12, 3133.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Roberts, CA, Giesbrecht, T, Fallon, N et al. (2020) A systematic review and activation likelihood estimation meta-analysis of fMRI studies on sweet taste in humans. J Nutr 150, 16191630.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Yeung, AWK & Wong, NSM (2020) How does our brain process sugars and non-nutritive sweeteners differently: a systematic review on functional magnetic resonance imaging studies. Nutrients 12, 3010.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lasschuijt, MP, Mars, M, de Graaf, C et al. (2020) Endocrine cephalic phase responses to food cues: a systematic review. Adv Nutr 11, 13641383.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Dhillon, J, Lee, JY & Mattes, RD (2017) The cephalic phase insulin response to nutritive and low-calorie sweeteners in solid and beverage form. Physiol Behav 181, 100109.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Pullicin, AJ, Glendinning, JI & Lim, J (2021) Cephalic phase insulin release: a review of its mechanistic basis and variability in humans. Physiol Behav 239, 113514.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Zhang, R, Noronha, JC, Khan, TA et al. (2023) The effect of non-nutritive sweetened beverages on postprandial glycemic and endocrine responses: a systematic review and network meta-analysis. Nutrients 15, 1050.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Grüneis, V, Schweiger, K, Galassi, C et al. (2021) Sweetness perception is not involved in the regulation of blood glucose after oral application of sucrose and glucose solutions in healthy male subjects. Mol Nutr Food Res 65, 2000472.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Rogers, PJ (2018) The role of low-calorie sweeteners in the prevention and management of overweight and obesity: evidence v. conjecture. Proc Nutr Soc 77, 230238.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
van Langeveld, AW, Gibbons, S, Koelliker, Y et al. (2017) The relationship between taste and nutrient content in commercially available foods from the United States. Food Qual Pref 57, 17.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Dalenberg, JR, Patel, BP, Denis, R et al. (2020) Short-term consumption of sucralose with, but not without, carbohydrate impairs neural and metabolic sensitivity to sugar in humans. Cell Metab 31, 493502.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Casperson, SL, Johnson, L & Roemmich, JN (2017) The relative reinforcing value of sweet v. savory snack foods after consumption of sugar-or non-nutritive sweetened beverages. Appetite 112, 143149.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Figure 0

Table 1. Intervention trials published since August 2017 testing the effects of exposure to sweetened v. unsweetened test products on subsequent measures of sweet taste liking

Figure 1

Table 2. Longitudinal cohort studies published since August 2017 testing the association of exposure to sweet foods with liking for sweetness