Hostname: page-component-78c5997874-mlc7c Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-11-15T17:12:41.441Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Associations between dietary fatty acid patterns and cognitive function in the Hispanic Community Health Study/Study of Latinos

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  26 August 2021

Nicole Karazurna
Affiliation:
Department of Agriculture Nutrition and Food Systems, University of New Hampshire, Durham, NH, USA
Caitlin Porter
Affiliation:
Department of Agriculture Nutrition and Food Systems, University of New Hampshire, Durham, NH, USA
Semra Aytur
Affiliation:
Department of Health Management and Policy, University of New Hampshire, Durham, NH, USA
Tammy Scott
Affiliation:
Department of Psychiatry, Tufts University, Boston, MA, USA
Josiemer Mattei
Affiliation:
Department of Nutrition, Harvard T.H. Chan School of Public Health, Cambridge, MA, USA
Sabrina Feldeisen
Affiliation:
Department of Biomedical and Nutritional Sciences, University of Massachusetts Lowell, Lowell, MA, USA
Hector Gonzalez
Affiliation:
Department of Neurosciences, University of California, San Diego, La Jolla, CA, USA
Yasmin Mossavar-Rahmani
Affiliation:
Department of Epidemiology and Population Health, Albert Einstein College of Medicine, The Bronx, NY, USA
Daniela Sotres-Alvarez
Affiliation:
Department of Biostatistics, University of North Carolina,  Gillings School of Public Health, Chapel Hill, NC, USA
Linda C. Gallo
Affiliation:
Department of Psychology, San Diego State University, San Diego, CA, USA
Martha L. Daviglus
Affiliation:
Department of Medicine, University of Illinois at Chicago, Chicago, IL, USA
Linda V. Van Horn
Affiliation:
Department of Preventative Medicine, Northwestern University, Evanston, IL, USA
Tali Elfassy
Affiliation:
Department of Medicine, University of Miami, Coral Gables, FL, USA
Marc Gellman
Affiliation:
Department of Psychology, University of Miami, Coral Gables, FL, USA
Ashley Moncrieft
Affiliation:
Department of Center for Applied Research Care, University of South Carolina, Columbia, SC, USA
Katherine L. Tucker
Affiliation:
Department of Biomedical and Nutritional Sciences, University of Massachusetts Lowell, Lowell, MA, USA
Robert C. Kaplan
Affiliation:
Department of Epidemiology and Population Health, Albert Einstein College of Medicine, The Bronx, NY, USA Public Health Sciences Division, Fred Hutchinson Cancer Research Center, Seattle, WA 98109, USA
Sherman J. Bigornia*
Affiliation:
Department of Agriculture Nutrition and Food Systems, University of New Hampshire, Durham, NH, USA
*
*Corresponding author: Sherman J. Bigornia, email [email protected]
Rights & Permissions [Opens in a new window]

Abstract

Our objective was to quantify the cross-sectional associations between dietary fatty acid (DFA) patterns and cognitive function among Hispanic/Latino adults. This study included data from 8942 participants of the Hispanic Community Health Study/Study of Latinos, a population-based cohort study (weighted age 56·2 years and proportion female 55·2 %). The National Cancer Institute method was used to estimate dietary intake from two 24-h recalls. We derived DFA patterns using principal component analysis with twenty-six fatty acid and total plant and animal MUFA input variables. Global cognitive function was calculated as the average z-score of four neurocognitive tests. Survey linear regression models included multiple potential confounders such as age, sex, education, depressive symptoms, physical activity, energy intake and CVD. DFA patterns were characterised by the consumption of long-chain SFA, animal-based MUFA and trans-fatty acids (factor 1); short to medium-chain SFA (factor 2); very-long-chain n-3 PUFA (factor 3); very-long-chain SFA and plant-based MUFA and PUFA (factor 4). Factor 2 was associated with greater scores for global cognitive function (β = 0·037 (sd 0·012)) and the Digit Symbol Substitution (DSS) (β = 0·56 (sd 0·17)), Brief Spanish English Verbal Learning-Sum (B-SEVLT) (β = 0·23 (sd 0·11)) and B-SEVLT-Recall (β = 0·11 (sd 0·05)) tests (P < 0·05 for all). Factors 1 (β = 0·04 (sd 0·01)) and 4 (β = 0·70 (sd 0·18)) were associated with the DSS test (P < 0·05 for all). The consumption of short to medium-chain SFA may be associated with higher cognitive function among US-residing Hispanic/Latino adults. Prospective studies are necessary to confirm these findings.

Type
Research Article
Copyright
© The Author(s), 2021. Published by Cambridge University Press on behalf of The Nutrition Society

Dementia and cognitive decline are of major public health concern worldwide(1). The global cost of dementia increased by 35 % between 2010 and 2015 to $818 billion annually(Reference Wimo, Guerchet and Ali2). Approximately 50 million individuals are living with dementia, and this estimate is projected to triple by 2050(1,Reference Prince, Bryce and Albanese3) . Based on limited data, it has been estimated that Hispanic/Latino adults in the USA have a 47 % higher prevalence of dementia as compared with non-Hispanic whites(Reference Chen and Zissimopoulos4). The disproportionate burden of cognitive decline experienced by Hispanic/Latino adults is accompanied by lifestyle-related health conditions (i.e. CVD and diabetes)(Reference Moheet, Mangia and Seaquist5Reference Velasco-Mondragon, Jimenez and Palladino-Davis7) that may further contribute to this observation.

Whether dietary factors can inform lifestyle interventions to prevent or treat cognitive decline and dementia is of great interest. Past studies indicate that dietary fat, particularly saturated fat, is a modifiable risk factor for CVD(Reference Sacks, Lichtenstein and Wu8). Given that vascular diseases are the second leading cause of dementia(Reference Iadecola, Duering and Hachinski9), the association of dietary fat with cognitive function merits investigation. Past studies on this topic have been limited in scope because they do not consider the potentially unique effects of specific fatty acids on cardiometabolic pathways that may contribute to accelerated cognitive decline and dementia. These include pathways related to inflammation, insulin resistance and dyslipidaemia(Reference Darweesh, Wolters and Ikram10Reference Zhang, Tian and Liu12). These shortcomings could partially explain why epidemiological investigations of dementia based on total SFA, MUFA, PUFA and trans-fatty acid (TFA) intakes have resulted in inconsistent findings(Reference Barnard, Bunner and Agarwal13Reference Zhang, Chen and Qiu16).

Total SFA and TFA consumption is generally considered to adversely affect health. However, greater intakes or circulating concentrations of short- (< 6 carbons (C)) to medium-chain (6–12 C)(Reference Ericson, Hellstrand and Brunkwall17) and very-long-chain SFA (≥ 20 C)(Reference Lemaitre, Fretts and Sitlani18,Reference Forouhi, Koulman and Sharp19) have been related to lower risk of type 2 diabetes, a condition strongly linked to chronic insulin resistance. Moreover, greater circulating very-long-chain SFA have been associated with lower CVD risk(Reference Malik, Chiuve and Campos20). Although the primary source of TFA in the US diet is processed foods, they naturally occur in ruminant animal meats and milks(Reference Gebauer, Chardigny and Jakobsen21). In one meta-analysis, trans-palmitoleic acid, also known as trans-hexadecenoic acid, consumption and circulating concentration were inversely associated with type 2 diabetes, whereas industrial TFA elevated the risk for CHD and all-cause mortality(Reference de Souza, Mente and Maroleanu22). MUFA and PUFA are recommended as replacements for SFA and TFA to improve cardiovascular health(Reference Sacks, Lichtenstein and Wu8). MUFA can be derived from a variety of plant and animal food sources. Epidemiological evidence suggests that plant-based MUFA consumption benefits cardiovascular risk to a greater extent than animal-based MUFA(Reference Schwingshackl and Hoffmann23,Reference Zong, Li and Sampson24) , although investigations with cognitive outcomes are not available. Very-long-chain n-3 PUFA (e.g. DHA and EPA) consumption improves inflammatory profiles(Reference Li, Huang and Zheng25) and reduces cognitive decline in elderly individuals(Reference Zhang, Hou and Li26). The endogenous elongation of the plant-based n-3 (α-linolenic acid (ALA)) and n-6 PUFA (linoleic acid) to their longer-chain products, EPA/DHA and arachidonic acid (ARA), respectively, is dependent upon the same enzymes(Reference Calder27). Considering that n-3 metabolism is slowed in the presence of greater n-6 PUFA consumption(Reference Brenna, Salem and Sinclair28), measuring the net impact of dietary n-3 and n-6 intakes have on cognitive function is essential to inform dietary fat recommendations for reduction in cognitive decline. Collectively, these data suggest that individual and subsets of fatty acids may differentially affect cardiometabolic pathways; however, the importance of these fatty acids to cognitive function is unknown.

Given the potentially unique effects of fatty acid subtypes and individual species on risk factors for cognitive decline, a more nuanced examination of dietary fat intake in relation to cognitive function is needed irrespective of the dietary fat class. To improve our understanding of the role of dietary fat intake and cognitive function, the overall aim of this study was to measure the associations of dietary fatty acid (DFA) patterns, informed by individual fatty acid species, with cognitive function in a large cohort of US Hispanic/Latino adults. We hypothesised that DFA patterns high in short to medium and very-long-chain SFA, plant-based MUFA and very-long-chain n-3 PUFA would be associated with greater global cognitive function, and, conversely, that DFA patterns characterised by industrially produced TFA and long-chain SFA would be associated with lower global cognitive function.

Methods

Participants

We examined data from the Hispanic Community Health Study/Study of Latinos (HCHS/SOL) (n 16 415), a population-based US cohort that aims to identify risk factors associated with various health conditions and prevalence among Hispanic/Latinos 18–74 years of age(Reference LaVange, Kalsbeek and Sorlie29). Participants from diverse Hispanic/Latino backgrounds (Mexican, Cuban, Puerto Rican, Dominican and Central/South American) were recruited from four field sites (Bronx, NY, Chicago, IL, Miami, FL, and San Diego, CA)(Reference Sorlie, Avilés-Santa and Wassertheil-Smoller30). Target areas surrounding the site locations were determined via census blocks sampled from strata defined by the cross-classification of (i) high and low Hispanic/Latino concentration and (ii) high and low educational attainment from the 2000 US decennial census data. Baseline examinations (2008–2011) were conducted in the participant’s preferred language (80 % in Spanish).

All participants provided informed consent which was witnessed and formally recorded. This study was conducted according to the guidelines laid down in the Declaration of Helsinki, and all HCHS/SOL study procedures were approved by the Institutional Review Boards at each study site.

Assessment of dietary intake

Estimated intake from foods was measured using two bilingual-interviewer administered 24-h dietary recalls according to the multiple-pass method established by the Nutrition Coordinating Center, University of Minnesota(31). An initial recall was administered in person during the baseline examination, and the second about 5–30 d later by telephone(Reference Siega-Riz, Sotres-Alvarez and Ayala32). Nutrients were analysed using the Nutrition Data System (NDS-R) for Research version 11 software(31). As previously described(Reference Siega-Riz, Sotres-Alvarez and Ayala32,Reference Mattei, Sotres-Alvarez and Daviglus33) , nutrient intake was estimated by the National Cancer Institute method(Reference Tooze, Kipnis and Buckman34), with data from individuals with one or two valid 24-h recalls. The National Cancer Institute method includes a weighting factor to account for the sampling probabilities. A one-part model was fit with the following covariates: age, sex, field site, Hispanic/Latino background (Central American, Cuban, Dominican, Mexican, Puerto Rican, South American and more than one/other), first or second 24-h recall, recall day of the week and self-reported intake being greater, same or less than usual.

Total animal- and plant-based MUFA consumption was derived using methods similar to others(Reference Zong, Li and Sampson35). Plant-based MUFA included those from fruit, vegetables and grains products while animal-based MUFA included meats, animal fats (e.g. lard) and dairy products. For mixed foods/dishes with both animal- and plant-based components (e.g. desserts, soups), food labels from manufactures or recipes were used to identify plant and animal ingredients. Total animal- and plant-based MUFA was calculated by summing the gram intake for each food/dish reported.

Dietary fatty acid patterns

Consistent with methods used previously(Reference Bigornia, Lichtenstein and Harris36,Reference Noel, Newby and Ordovas37) , DFA patterns were identified using principal component analysis with varimax rotation (proc factor in SAS). Principal component analysis is a statistical method widely employed in nutritional epidemiology to identify latent underlying diet and nutrient patterns from a set of foods and nutrients(Reference Lever, Krzywinski and Altman38,Reference Noel, Newby and Ordovas39) . Input variables were expressed as a proportion of total energy and included twenty-six individual fatty acids, as well as total animal and plant based MUFA (Table 1). To decide on the number of DFA patterns (factors) to retain, we initially selected three to eight factor solutions. The choice to use a four-factor solution was based on the scree plot, eigenvalues and interpretability of identified factors. For each of these four DFA patterns, a factor score was calculated by summing the fatty acid inputs weighted by loadings of each fatty acid input.

Table 1. Factor loadings of dietary fatty acid patterns

(Mean values and standard deviations)

* Factor loadings greater than 0·35.

Dietary fatty acid patterns were derived using principal component analysis; Variation explained by DFA patterns 1, 2, 3 and 4 was 8·1, 4·8, 4·1 and 2·3 %, respectively.

Values expressed as grams or mg per 2000 kcal.

Neurocognitive testing

At baseline, neurocognitive testing was conducted with participants ≥45 years, as previously described(Reference Gonzalez, Tarraf and Gouskova40). The battery of tests included the Spanish English Verbal Learning Test (B-SEVLT) to evaluate episodic memory, the Word Fluency Test (WF) to evaluate executive function and the Digit Symbol Substitution test (DSS) to evaluate psychomotor speed. Tests were conducted by trained examiners and in the language preferred by the participant. The B-SEVLT asked participants to reiterate a list of fifteen words that were read to them over three trials. Two scores were derived, including: (1) the sum total of correctly learned words (B-SEVLT-Sum) and (2) the number of correctly recalled words after an interference trial (B-SVELT-Recall). For the WF, participants were asked to list as many words as they could that began with the letter A or F in 60 s. The DSS test required participants to decipher a code by translating numbers to symbols from a key on the test form within a 90 s window. Our primary outcome was global cognitive function, which was calculated as the average score of each of the four cognitive test scores, following a z-score transformation(Reference Strizich, Kaplan and González41). The secondary outcomes were individual scores on the B-SEVLT-Sum, B-SEVLT-Recall, WF and DSS.

Covariates

Socio-demographic covariates reported at baseline included sex (male/female), age (years), education (< high school, high school or equivalent, > high school), household income (< $30 000 or ≥ $30 000), field centre (Miami, Bronx, San Diego, Chicago), Hispanic/Latino background (Central American, Cuban, Dominican, Mexican, Puerto Rican, South American or more than one/other) and smoking status (current smoker v. not). Clinical covariates included: type 2 diabetes (yes/no), CVD (yes/no), hypertension (yes/no), depressive symptoms score and physical activity level (MET-min/d), ascertained using the WHO Global Physical Activity Questionnaire(Reference Hoos, Espinoza and Marshall42). Participants were categorised as having type 2 diabetes if they (1) self-reported, (2) were using anti-hyperglycaemic medication, (3) had fasting blood glucose > 126 mg/dl or ≥ 200 mg/dl on an oral glucose tolerance test or (4) HbA1c ≥ 6·5 %. CVD was determined based on self-reported history of myocardial infarction, transient ischaemic attack or stroke. Hypertension was defined by (1) self-report, (2) anti-hypertension medication use or (3) systolic blood pressure > 140 mmHg or diastolic blood pressure >90 mmHg. Depressive symptoms were assessed using the 10-item Center for Epidemiologic Studies Depression Scale(Reference Andresen, Malmgren and Carter43).

We also considered dietary factors that may confound associations between fat intakes and cognitive function, including added sugar(Reference Crichton, Elias and Torres44), fruits and vegetables(Reference Mottaghi, Amirabdollahian and Haghighatdoost45), and alcohol(Reference Anstey, Mack and Cherbuin46). Animal products, including red meat, are sources of saturated fat, but are also rich in certain B vitamins, including B6, B12 and niacin. Intake of these nutrients has been associated with better cognitive functioning(Reference Qin, Xun and Jacobs47Reference An, Feng and Zhang52). Therefore, models were also adjusted for these micronutrients. Dietary factors were expressed relative to total energy intake, either as a percentage of total energy or as servings or g/8368 kJ (2000 kcal).

Statistical analyses

Of the 16 415 participants, 9714 individuals were eligible to participate in neurocognitive testing (≥ 45 years). From these, 9170 had complete neurocognitive data available, 9553 had one or two valid 24-h dietary recalls and 9141 had complete covariate information. The final analytic sample size was 8942. Analyses were conducted using SAS version 9.4 (SAS Institute). Significance was set at P < 0·05 for all analyses. Survey linear regression was utilised for descriptive purposes and primary and secondary analyses, as this process accounts for the non-random sampling methods conducted in HCHS/SOL, and the oversampling of individuals older than 45 years(Reference LaVange, Kalsbeek and Sorlie29). Differences in baseline characteristics by quintile of DFA pattern scores were adjusted for age and sex, as appropriate.

Spearman correlations were used to identify food categories most closely associated with each DFA pattern. Correlations were adjusted for age, sex and total energy intake. Food categories were informed by the Nutrition Coordinating Center predefined food groupings available in the NDS-R software package. The 135 food groups were subsequently collapsed to thirty-two broader food categories. The consumption of each food category was estimated using the mean value from two valid 24-h recalls. Correlations were only conducted for individuals with two valid 24-h recalls (n 8435).

In primary analyses, survey linear regression was used to examine the linear association between each continuous DFA pattern and the global cognitive function continuous score. As a complementary analysis, we also examined DFA pattern scores expressed in quintiles. Tukey’s post hoc tests were used to conduct pairwise DFA pattern comparisons across quintiles. Model covariates included age, sex, total energy intake, physical activity, education, income, Hispanic/Latino background, field site, Center for Epidemiologic Studies Depression Scale score, smoking status, diabetes, CVD and hypertension, as well as intakes of added sugar, alcohol, vitamin B6, vitamin B12, niacin, fruit and vegetables. In secondary analyses, we examined relationships between continuous DFA patterns and the continuous score of each neurocognitive test. For all models, total energy was adjusted for using the nutrient density approach.

Several additional analyses were considered. We examined the associations between fatty acid subgroups and global cognitive function using survey linear regression. Fatty acids were grouped as follows: short- and medium-chain SFA (butyric, caproic, caprylic, capric and lauric acids), long-chain SFA (myristic, palmitic, margaric and stearic acids), very-long-chain SFA (arachidic and behenic acids), plant-based MUFA, animal-based MUFA, n-6 PUFA (linoleic, parinaric and arachidonic), very-long-chain n-3 PUFA (EPA, DPA and DHA) and linolenic acid (LA). Due to limitations of the dietary analysis software version, LA could not be distinguished between α-linolenic and γ-linolenic isomers, which are n-3 and n-6 class fatty acids, respectively. The nutrient density approach was used to model the associations with the proportions of energy from fatty acid subgroups as independent variables modelled simultaneously along with total energy and the proportions of energy from protein and alcohol. Regression coefficients represent the predicted change in global cognitive function (z-score) when the fatty acid subgroup increases by 1 % of energy at the expense of an equal amount of energy from carbohydrates. Models were adjusted for age, sex, total energy intake, physical activity, education, Hispanic/Latino background, field site, Center for Epidemiologic Studies Depression Scale score, smoking status, diabetes, CVD, hypertension and dietary B6, B12, niacin, fruits and vegetables.

CVD predicts the onset of dementia(Reference Stefanidis, Askew and Greaves53,Reference Kuzma, Lourida and Moore54) . Due to concerns that individuals with CVD may have altered dietary fat intake as part of disease management, we also conducted sensitivity analyses, where models were repeated excluding individuals with prevalent CVD (n 549). Further, as greater educational attainment may lower the risk of dementia(Reference Valenzuela and Sachdev55), we also conducted sensitivity analyses excluding individuals attaining less than a high school education (n 3738).

Results

Dietary fatty acid patterns

Four DFA patterns were identified using principal component analysis (Table 1). Factor 1 was characterised by long-chain (13–20 C) SFA and MUFA, as well as animal-based MUFA and TFA. Short- (<6 C) and medium-chain SFA (6–12 C) loaded onto factor 2. The animal-based n-3 PUFA (EPA, DPA and DHA) and the n-6 PUFA ARA loaded onto factor 3. Factor 4 was distinguished by long-chain plant-based MUFA and PUFA (linoleic and LA). Factors 1 through 4 explained 8·1, 4·8, 4·1 and 2·3 % of the variability in fatty acid intake, respectively.

Factor 1 was most strongly and positively correlated with the consumption of red, processed, and organ meats, and eggs, and negatively correlated with pasta and rice dishes, fruit, vegetables, and cold and cooked cereals (online Supplementary Table S1). Factor 2 was positively correlated with milk, cheese and related products, as well as animal fats (e.g. butter) and other grain-based recipes (e.g. tacos), and negatively with red, processed, and organ meats and meat, poultry, and fish recipes. Factor 3 was most strongly associated with seafood, poultry and egg consumption. Factor 4 was positively correlated with the consumption of plant-based fats and oils specifically margarine and shortening, oil, salad dressing, and nuts and nut butters and negatively correlated with breads and related products.

Descriptive characteristics by dietary fatty acid patterns

Age was positively associated with factors 2 and 4, but inversely with factor 1 (Table 2). Participants with high DFA intake most reflective of factor 1 tended to be women, whereas men had DFA patterns more consistent with factors 2 and 3. Total energy intake was inversely associated with factors 1–3 (Table 2). Hispanic/Latino background and field site location, and intakes of fruit and vegetables, added sugar, alcohol and B vitamins (B6, B12 and niacin) varied significantly across DFA patterns.

Table 2. Age and sex adjusted characteristics by dietary fatty acid pattern score (quintile) (n 8942)

(Mean values and standard deviations)

* P F-test < 0·05 and

** P F-test < 0·0001 indicate significant differences across dietary fatty acid categories (factor score quintile).

Values are age and sex-adjusted means with their standard error and proportions estimated using survey procedures. Survey linear and logistic regression were used to compare means and proportions between factor score categories (quintiles).

Dietary intake variables are expressed per 2000 kcal/d, unless otherwise indicated.

Dietary fatty acid patterns and cognitive function

The distribution of the cognitive scores is reported in online Supplementary Table S2. Factor 2 was linearly and positively associated with the global cognitive function score (P = 0·002) and there was a trend towards a positive association with factor 4 (P = 0·053) (Table 3). However, significant associations were not observed for factors 1 or 3. In complementary analyses, global cognitive function scores were significantly higher among individuals in factor 2 quintile 5, compared with quintiles 1 and 2 (Table 3, P F-test = 0·008). There was a suggestion of differences in global cognitive function score between factor 3 quintiles 1 and 2 (P F-test = 0·04; P Q1 v. Q2 = 0·08). Conversely, the global cognitive function score did not vary significantly by factor 1 or factor 4 score quintile. In secondary analyses, we evaluated associations between the DFA patterns and individual cognitive function tests (Table 4). Factor 2 was associated with greater scores on the DSS, B-SEVLT-Sum and B-SEVLT-Recall (P = 0·03–0·007) and approached significance with WF (P = 0·06). Both factors 1 and 4 were positively and significantly associated with the DSS test, but not with other tests. Null associations were observed with factor 3.

Table 3. Adjusted mean of global cognitive function score by dietary fatty acid pattern quintile (n 8942)

(Mean values with their standard errors)

* Values represent the adjusted means with their standard error neurocognitive test z-score by factor score quintile. Differences in cognitive function score between factor score quintiles were examined using survey linear regression with factor scores treated categorically and neurocognitive test scores continuously. Tukey’s post-hoc tests were used for pairwise comparisons. Values with different letters are significantly different (P < 0·05).

Models are adjusted for age (years), sex (M/F), energy intake (kcal/d), physical activity (MET-min/d), education (< high school, high school or equivalent, > high school), Hispanic/Latino background (Central American, Cuban, Dominican, Mexican, Puerto Rican, South American and multiple/other/missing), field site (Bronx, Chicago, Miami, San Diego), CES-D score, type 2 diabetes (y/n), CVD (y/n), current smoker (y/n), hypertension (y/n), household income (< $30 000 or not) and dietary intakes of fruits (servings/d/kcal), vegetables (servings/d/kcal), alcohol (servings/d/kcal), added sugar (% of energy/d), B12 (mg/d/kcal), B6 (mg/d/kcal) and niacin (mg/d/kcal).

Dietary fatty acid patterns were characterised by: long-chain (13– 20 C) SFA and MUFA, as well as animal-based MUFA and TFA (factor 1); short- (< 6 C) and medium-chain SFA (6–12 C) (factor 2); animal-based n-3 PUFA (EPA, DPA and DHA), but also the n-6 PUFA arachidonic acid (factor 3); and long-chain plant-based MUFA, as well as PUFA found in plant food sources (linoleic and linolenic acid) (factor 4).

§ Values are adjusted β ± se and can be interpreted as the sd change in neurocognitive test score per one sd change in factor score. Analyses were conducted using survey linear regression treating each dietary fatty acid pattern score as a continuous variable.

|| In post-hoc analyses, we did not observe significant differences in global cognitive function score between factor 3 dietary fatty acid pattern quintiles; however, there was a suggestion of difference comparing quintile 1 v. 2 (P = 0·08).

Table 4. Associations between dietary fatty acid patterns and individual neurocognitive tests (n 8942)

(β-coefficients and standard error of the mean)

* Dietary fatty acid patterns were characterised by: long-chain (1320 C) SFA and MUFA, as well as animal-based MUFA and TFA (factor 1); short- (<6 C) and medium-chain SFA (6–12 C) (factor 2); animal-based n-3 PUFA (EPA, DPA and DHA), but also the n-6 PUFA arachidonic acid (factor 3); and long-chain plant-based MUFA, as well as PUFA found in plant food sources (linoleic and linolenic acid) (factor 4).

Survey linear regression was used to examine associations with factor and neurocognitive test scores treated as continuous variables. Models are adjusted for age (years), sex (M/F), energy intake (kcal/d), physical activity (MET-min/d), education (< high school, high school or equivalent, > high school), Hispanic/Latino background (Central American, Cuban, Dominican, Mexican, Puerto Rican, South American and multiple/other/missing), field site (Bronx, Chicago, Miami, San Diego), CES-D score, type 2 diabetes (y/n), CVD (y/n), current smoker (y/n), hypertension (y/n), household income (< $30 000 or not) and dietary intakes of fruits (servings/d/kcal), vegetables (servings/d/kcal), alcohol (servings/d/kcal), added sugar (% of energy/d), B12 (mg/d/kcal), B6 (mg/d/kcal) and niacin (mg/d/kcal).

Additional analyses

The exclusion of individuals with CVD did not change the observed associations between the DFA patterns and global cognitive function (online Supplementary Table S3). In general, the result with global cognitive function remained similar after excluding individuals with less than a high school education, although we observed a significant and positive association between factor 4 and global cognitive function (online Supplementary Table S4). Factor 2 remained significantly and positively associated with DSS and B-SEVLT-Sum neurocognitive tests in sensitivity analyses excluding individuals with CVD (online Supplementary Table S5) or less than a high school education (online Supplementary Table S6). In contrast, effect estimates strengthened between factor 2 and WF, but associations with B-SEVLT-Recall were not statistically significant (online Supplementary Tables S5 and S6).

Global cognitive function was regressed on to fatty acid subgroups: short- and medium-chain SFA, long-chain SFA, very-long-chain SFA, plant-based MUFA, animal-based MUFA, LA, n-3 PUFA, n-6 PUFA, trans-hexadecenoic acid (ruminant TFA) and industrial TFA (online Supplementary Table S7). Using survey linear regression, most fatty acid subgroups were not significantly associated with global cognitive function. However, we observed that the predicted change in global cognitive score was 0·13 (sd 0·06) units for a 1 % of energy increase in short- and medium-chain SFA consumption in exchange for an equivalent amount of energy from carbohydrates (P = 0·04, online Supplementary Table S7).

Considering the observed significant and positive associations of factor 2 (short- and medium-chain length SFA) with global cognitive function and multiple individual neurocognitive tests examined, we conducted food rankings (online Supplementary Table S8) to identify foods contributing to food categories that were most correlated with factor 2 (online Supplementary Table S1). Both reduced and regular fat milk, as well as regular fat cheese, ice cream and yogurt were the main contributors to the ‘Milk, cheese, and related products’ food category. Butter was the main contributor to animal fat. Lastly, masa-based mixed dishes (e.g. enchiladas and tamales); pizzas/calzones; quesadillas; burritos; and dumplings, turnovers and fritters were the main contributors to the ‘Miscellaneous Grain Recipes without Meat, Poultry, and Fish’ food category.

Discussion

Using data from HCHS/SOL, we identified four DFA patterns that varied in their associations with cognitive function. Most consistently, the DFA pattern distinguished by greater short- to medium-chain SFA (factor 2) was associated with greater global cognitive function, as well as better scores on each neurocognitive test. The DFA pattern consistent with the consumption of plant-based fatty acids (factor 4) tended to be associated with better global cognitive function, and significantly so with the DSS test. There was a lack of evidence for the very-long-chain n-3 DFA pattern (factor 3) or the DFA pattern characterised by greater long-chain SFA, animal-based MUFA and TFA (factor 1) in relation to global cognitive function, although the latter was positively associated with the DSS test.

Observational studies examining associations between total SFA intake and dementia, mild cognitive impairment and cognitive decline have reported null and adverse associations(Reference Barnard, Bunner and Agarwal13Reference Ruan, Tang and Guo15). This inconclusive evidence could be because SFA chain lengths may differentially impact cognitive function and metabolic risk factors that contribute to cognitive decline (e.g. inflammation and insulin resistance). We observed that the short- to medium-chain SFA pattern was associated with better scores on all cognitive measures examined. In animal models, the short chain SFA, butyric acid, has been shown to improve learning and memory performance, as well as restore cognitive function post neurodegeneration(Reference Stilling, van de Wouw and Clarke56), although human trials are lacking. Conversely, medium-chain SFA supplementation has been shown to benefit cognitive performance in individuals with mild cognitive impairment or Alzheimer’s disease(Reference Avgerinos, Egan and Mattson57). Murine studies demonstrated that short- and medium-chain SFA exhibit insulin sensitising(Reference Geng, Zhu and Xie58,Reference Gao, Yin and Zhang59) and anti-inflammatory properties(Reference Geng, Zhu and Xie58,Reference Kim, Kang and Park60) . This is supported by data from a large prospective cohort study, where intake of these SFA was associated with lower risk of type 2 diabetes(Reference Ericson, Hellstrand and Brunkwall17). The anti-inflammatory and insulin sensitising effects of SCFA may be through activation of G-protein coupled receptors and inhibition of histone deacetylase(Reference McNabney and Henagan61,Reference Vinolo, Rodrigues and Nachbar62) .

It is also thought that medium-chain SFA may serve as an alternative fuel source in the impaired glucose metabolism(Reference Fernando, Martins and Goozee63) observed in mild cognitive impairment and dementia(Reference Mullins, Diehl and Chia64,Reference Willette, Modanlo and Kapogiannis65) . Although the brain primarily uses glucose for energy metabolism, it has the capacity to utilise ketone bodies. Medium-chain SFA are unique compared with longer chain fatty acids in that they bypass the general circulation post-absorption and are directed to the liver where ketogenesis occurs. Indeed, experimental studies demonstrate that the consumption of medium-chain SFA increases circulating ketone bodies as compared with longer chain fatty acids(Reference Avgerinos, Egan and Mattson57) and up-regulates brain ketone metabolism(Reference Fortier, Castellano and Croteau66). In the current study, we observed that the short- and medium-chain SFA pattern was distinctly correlated with milk and milk product consumption. Systematic reviews of primarily observational studies suggest that the impact of dairy consumption on cognitive function is inconclusive, and that randomised controlled trials are needed to clarify these associations(Reference Lee, Fu and Chung67,Reference Crichton, Bryan and Murphy68) .

We observed that a DFA pattern correlated with very-long-chain SFA (arachidic and behenic acid) and plant-based MUFA and PUFA (linolenic and linoleic acids), tended to be associated with better global cognitive function score and was significantly and positively associated with processing speed (DSS score). Greater adherence to this DFA pattern correlated with intakes of vegetable fats and oils and nuts and nut butters. There is a lack of evidence examining very-long-chain SFA intake with cognitive function. However, these fatty acids may positively impact cognitive performance through improvement in cardiometabolic risk profiles. Greater intake or circulating concentrations of very-long-chain SFA have been related to lower risk of the metabolic syndrome(Reference Lee, Cho and Shin69), type 2 diabetes(Reference Lemaitre, Fretts and Sitlani18,Reference Forouhi, Koulman and Sharp19) and CVD(Reference Malik, Chiuve and Campos20). We know of no previous epidemiological studies that have examined plant-based MUFA in relation to cognitive outcomes. Our results are consistent with findings from the PREDIMED (Prevención con Dieta Mediterránea) trial conducted among older Spaniards with high CVD risk. Compared with the low-fat diet control arm, the Mediterranean diet with olive oil or mixed nuts arms improved cognitive function after 4·1 years of follow-up(Reference Valls-Pedret, Sala-Vila and Serra-Mir70). Olive oil is mainly composed of MUFA, whereas nuts are a source of PUFA and very-long-chain SFA.

Due to limitations of the nutrient database, we could not examine undifferentiated LA, which is composed of the n-3 ALA and the n-6 γ-linolenic acid. A paucity of evidence is available on the impacts of ALA, γ-linolenic acid and LA consumption on cognitive function or dementia. Circulating ALA concentration has been shown to be lower among individuals with dementia(Reference Cherubini, Andres-Lacueva and Martin71) and, inversely, associated with incident dementia(Reference Yamagishi, Ikeda and Chei72). The nature of the relationship between LA consumption and cognitive function is inconclusive. A recent study among older Puerto Rican adults found that LA consumption and circulating concentration were not associated with 2-year change in cognitive function, as assessed using the Mini-Mental-State Exam or an aggregate neurocognitive test score indicative of executive function(Reference Bigornia, Scott and Harris73). Similarly, LA intake was not related to cognitive decline in a study among older Dutch men(Reference Kalmijn, Feskens and Launer74). In contrast, greater consumption of total n-6 FA, primarily composed of LA (90 %), was cross-sectionally associated with better score on the Audio Recorded Cognitive Screen test, but not the Mini-Mental-State Exam(Reference MacDonald-Wicks, McEvoy and Magennis75) among Australian adults. Theoretically, ALA may improve cognitive function through reduction in CVD risk(Reference Pan, Chen and Chowdhury76). Evidence from randomised controlled trial supports that γ-linolenic acid and LA intake improves total cholesterol(Reference Hooper, Al-Khudairy and Abdelhamid77), which is adversely related to dementia(Reference Anstey, Ashby-Mitchell and Peters78).

Inconsistent with our hypotheses, the DFA pattern characterised by greater consumption of long-chain SFA, animal-based MUFA and TFA (factor 1) was not adversely related to global cognitive function. Further, it was positively associated with the DSS test to evaluate psychomotor speed. This DFA pattern was positively correlated with red meat, processed and organ meat consumption. Although we considered neuroprotective nutrients found in red meat including B6, B12 and niacin, the association with DSS still remained. Additional research is needed to replicate this finding. In particular, longitudinally designed studies should be implemented to overcome the limitations of the cross-sectional analysis used in our study.

Our findings do not support our a priori hypothesis that an n-3 DFA pattern (factor 3) would be beneficially associated with cognitive function. Similarly, another cross-sectional study in Australians reported a null association between total n-3 intake and global cognitive function(Reference MacDonald-Wicks, McEvoy and Magennis75). In contrast, among Puerto Rican adults, total n-3 fatty acid (EPA, DPA and DHA) consumption and circulating concentrations were associated with better executive function after 2 years of follow-up(Reference Bigornia, Scott and Harris73). This is consistent with evidence from randomised controlled trial where n-3 supplementation reduced the rate of cognitive decline among patients with mild to moderate cognitive impairment(Reference Zhang, Hou and Li26). One possible explanation for the null association observed in our study is that the impact of the very-long-chain n-3 PUFA may have been offset by ARA intake, which also loaded onto factor 3. Circulating ARA has been adversely related to cognitive function in prospective studies(Reference Bigornia, Scott and Harris73,Reference Beydoun, Kaufman and Satia79) . This n-6 fatty acid is a direct precursor to proinflammatory eicosanoids, whereas those from EPA and DHA are anti-inflammatory(Reference Calder27). Eicosanoid receptors demonstrate a stronger affinity for ARA-derived eicosanoids, as compared with those derived from EPA(Reference Calder27,Reference Wada, DeLong and Hong80) . Thus, higher ARA consumption may have blunted the anti-inflammatory potential of the very-long-chain n-3 PUFA.

Our study has several notable strengths. We used a novel approach of constructing DFA patterns to assess associations with cognitive function. This method allowed us to evaluate the net impact of combinations of individual fatty acids on cognitive function. The large sample size (n 8942) increased our ability to detect associations. Further, we provide evidence for Hispanic/Latino adults, an underrepresented population in cognitive function research. Our research findings should be interpreted in consideration of some limitations. The cross-sectional design prohibits any causal relationships. For example, reverse causation may be at play if individuals with higher cognitive function choose to consume fatty acids that are thought to be healthy (e.g. n-3 fatty acid and plant-based MUFA)(Reference Crichton, Elias and Davey81). Another limitation of the dietary data is measurement error, as the data are self-reported. The HCHS/SOL SOLNAS ancillary study validated their intake measures by comparing with biomarkers of energy, protein, Na and K using doubly labelled water, urinary N, Na and K, and showed substantial under-reporting of energy and protein that varied by BMI and Hispanic/Latino background(Reference Mossavar-Rahmani, Shaw and Wong82,Reference Mossavar-Rahmani, Sotres-Alvarez and Wong83) .

Among participants of the HCHS/SOL study, a DFA pattern distinguished by greater intakes of short- and medium-chain SFA was associated with better cognitive function using individual and aggregate measures from a battery of neurocognitive tests. Our results also suggest that intake of plant-based MUFA and PUFA and very-long-chain SFA may benefit global cognitive function and processing speed. Although these results are cross-sectional, they provide evidence that food sources of these fatty acids, including dairy products, vegetable oils and nuts, as part of an overall healthy dietary pattern, may benefit cognitive function among Hispanic/Latino adults. Future research in similar and additional cohorts should be conducted using prospective study designs to replicate these novel findings.

Acknowledgements

The views expressed in this article are those of the authors and do not necessarily represent the views of the National Heart, Lung, and Blood Institute; the National Institutes of Health; or the US Department of Health and Human Services. The authors thank the staff of and participants in HCHS/SOL for important contributions (see http://www.cscc.unc.edu/hchs/ for a list of investigators).

The Hispanic Community Health Study/Study of Latinos is a collaborative study supported by contracts from the National Heart, Lung, and Blood Institute (NHLBI) to the University of North Carolina (HHSN268201300001I/N01-HC-65233), University of Miami (HHSN268201300004I/N01-HC-65234), Albert Einstein College of Medicine (HHSN268201300002I/N01-HC-65235), University of Illinois at Chicago – HHSN268201300003I/N01-HC-65236 Northwestern Univ) and San Diego State University (HHSN268201300005I/N01-HC-65237). The following Institutes/Centers/Offices have contributed to the HCHS/SOL through a transfer of funds to the NHLBI: National Institute on Minority Health and Health Disparities, National Institute on Deafness and Other Communication Disorders, National Institute of Dental and Craniofacial Research, National Institute of Diabetes and Digestive and Kidney Diseases, National Institute of Neurological Disorders and Stroke, NIH Institution-Office of Dietary Supplements. K. L. T., T. S. and S. J. B. are supported by R01 AG055948.

N. K. and S. J. B.: created the study concept and design, analysed the data, interpreted data and wrote the manuscript; C. P.: assisted in data analysis; S. A. and T. S.: assisted in developing the study design, data interpretation and manuscript writing; J. S., S. F., H. G., Y. M-R., D. S-A., L. C. G., M. L. D., L. V. N., T. E., M. D. G., A. E. M., K. L. T. and R. C. K.: contributed to data interpretation and critical revisions to the manuscript. All authors have read and approved this manuscript.

The authors have no relevant conflicts of interest to report.

Supplementary material

For supplementary material referred to in this article, please visit https://doi.org/10.1017/S0007114521003275

References

Organization WH (2019) Risk Reduction of Cognitive Decline and Dementia: WHO Guidelines. Geneva: WHO.Google Scholar
Wimo, A, Guerchet, M, Ali, GC, et al. (2017) The worldwide costs of dementia 2015 and comparisons with 2010. Alzheimers Dement 13, 17.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Prince, M, Bryce, R, Albanese, E, et al. (2013) The global prevalence of dementia: a systematic review and metaanalysis. Alzheimers Dement 9, 6375.e62.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Chen, C & Zissimopoulos, JM (2018) Racial and ethnic differences in trends in dementia prevalence and risk factors in the United States. Alzheimers Dement 4, 510520.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Moheet, A, Mangia, S & Seaquist, ER (2015) Impact of diabetes on cognitive function and brain structure. Ann N Y Acad Sci 1353, 6071.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Wang, F, Zhao, M, Han, Z, et al. (2017) Association of body mass index with amnestic and non-amnestic mild cognitive impairment risk in elderly. BMC Psychiatr 17, 17.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Velasco-Mondragon, E, Jimenez, A, Palladino-Davis, AG, et al. (2016) Hispanic health in the USA: a scoping review of the literature. Public Health Rev 37, 127.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Sacks, FM, Lichtenstein, AH, Wu, JHY, et al. (2017) Dietary fats and cardiovascular disease: a presidential advisory from the American Heart Association. Circulation 136, e1e23.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Iadecola, C, Duering, M, Hachinski, V, et al. (2019) vascular cognitive impairment and dementia: JACC scientific expert panel. J Am Coll Cardiol 73, 33263344.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Darweesh, SKL, Wolters, FJ, Ikram, MA, et al. (2018) Inflammatory markers and the risk of dementia and Alzheimer’s disease: a meta-analysis. Alzheimers Dement 14, 14501459.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Xue, M, Xu, W, Ou, YN, et al. (2019) Diabetes mellitus and risks of cognitive impairment and dementia: a systematic review and meta-analysis of 144 prospective studies. Ageing Res Rev 55, 100944.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Zhang, X, Tian, Q, Liu, D, et al. (2020) Causal association of circulating cholesterol levels with dementia: a Mendelian randomization meta-analysis. Transl Psychiatr 10, 145.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Barnard, ND, Bunner, AE & Agarwal, U (2014) Saturated and trans fats and dementia: a systematic review. Neurobiol Aging 35, S65S73.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Morris, MC & Tangney, CC (2014) Dietary fat composition and dementia risk. Neurobiol Aging 35, S59S64.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ruan, Y, Tang, J, Guo, X, et al. (2018) Dietary fat intake and risk of Alzheimer’s disease and dementia: a meta-analysis of cohort studies. Curr Alzheimer Res 15, 869876.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Zhang, Y, Chen, J, Qiu, J, et al. (2016) Intakes of fish and polyunsaturated fatty acids and mild-to-severe cognitive impairment risks: a dose-response meta-analysis of 21 cohort studies. Am J Clin Nutr 103, 330340.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ericson, U, Hellstrand, S, Brunkwall, L, et al. (2015) Food sources of fat may clarify the inconsistent role of dietary fat intake for incidence of type 2 diabetes. Am J Clin Nutr 101, 10651080.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lemaitre, RN, Fretts, AM, Sitlani, CM, et al. (2015) Plasma phospholipid very-long-chain saturated fatty acids and incident diabetes in older adults: the Cardiovascular Health Study. Am J Clin Nutr 101, 10471054.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Forouhi, NG, Koulman, A, Sharp, SJ, et al. (2014) Differences in the prospective association between individual plasma phospholipid saturated fatty acids and incident type 2 diabetes: the EPIC-InterAct case-cohort study. Lancet Diabetes Endocrinol 2, 810818.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Malik, VS, Chiuve, SE, Campos, H, et al. (2015) Circulating very-long-chain saturated fatty acids and incident coronary heart disease in US men and women. Circulation 132, 260268.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gebauer, SK, Chardigny, JM, Jakobsen, MU, et al. (2011) Effects of ruminant trans fatty acids on cardiovascular disease and cancer: a comprehensive review of epidemiological, clinical, and mechanistic studies. Adv Nutr 2, 332354.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
de Souza, RJ, Mente, A, Maroleanu, A, et al. (2015) Intake of saturated and trans unsaturated fatty acids and risk of all cause mortality, cardiovascular disease, and type 2 diabetes: systematic review and meta-analysis of observational studies. BMJ 351, h3978.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Schwingshackl, L & Hoffmann, G (2014) Mediterranean dietary pattern, inflammation and endothelial function: a systematic review and meta-analysis of intervention trials. Nutr Metab Cardiovasc Dis 24, 929939.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Zong, G, Li, Y, Sampson, L, et al. (2018) Monounsaturated fats from plant and animal sources in relation to risk of coronary heart disease among US men and women. Am J Clin Nutr 107, 445453.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Li, K, Huang, T, Zheng, J, et al. (2014) Effect of marine-derived n-3 polyunsaturated fatty acids on C-reactive protein, interleukin 6 and tumor necrosis factor alpha: a meta-analysis. PLoS One 9, e88103.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Zhang, XW, Hou, WS, Li, M, et al. (2016) n-3 fatty acids and risk of cognitive decline in the elderly: a meta-analysis of randomized controlled trials. Aging Clin Exp Res 28, 165166.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Calder, PC (2017) n-3 fatty acids and inflammatory processes: from molecules to man. Biochem Soc Trans 45, 11051115.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Brenna, JT, Salem, N, Sinclair, AJ, et al. (2009) Alpha-Linolenic acid supplementation and conversion to n-3 long-chain polyunsaturated fatty acids in humans. Prostaglandins Leukot Essent Fatty Acids 80, 8591.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
LaVange, LM, Kalsbeek, W, Sorlie, PD, et al. (2010) Sample design and cohort selection in the Hispanic community health study/study of Latinos. Ann Epidemiol 20, 642649.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Sorlie, PD, Avilés-Santa, LM, Wassertheil-Smoller, S, et al. (2010) Design and implementation of the Hispanic community health study/study of Latinos. Ann Epidemiol 20, 629641.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
WHO (2017) Depression and Other Common Mental Disorders: Global Health Estimates. Geneva: World Health Organization.Google Scholar
Siega-Riz, AM, Sotres-Alvarez, D, Ayala, GX, et al. (2014) Food-group and nutrient-density intakes by Hispanic and Latino backgrounds in the Hispanic Community Health Study/Study of Latinos. Am J Clin Nutr 99, 14871498.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Mattei, J, Sotres-Alvarez, D, Daviglus, ML, et al. (2016) Diet quality and its association with cardiometabolic risk factors vary by Hispanic and Latino ethnic background in the Hispanic community health study/study of Latinos. J Nutr 146, 20352044.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Tooze, JA, Kipnis, V, Buckman, DW, et al. (2010) A mixed-effects model approach for estimating the distribution of usual intake of nutrients: the NCI method. Stat Med 29, 28572868.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Zong, G, Li, Y, Sampson, L, et al. (2018) Monounsaturated fats from plant and animal sources in relation to risk of coronary heart disease among US men and women. Am J Clin Nutr 107, 445453.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bigornia, SJ, Lichtenstein, AH, Harris, WS, et al. (2016) Associations of erythrocyte fatty acid patterns with insulin resistance. Am J Clin Nutr 103, 902909.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Noel, SE, Newby, PK, Ordovas, JM, et al. (2010) Adherence to an (n-3) fatty acid/fish intake pattern is inversely associated with metabolic syndrome among Puerto Rican adults in the Greater Boston area. J Nutr 140, 18461854.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lever, J, Krzywinski, M & Altman, N (2017) Principal component analysis. Nature Methods 14, 641642.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Noel, SE, Newby, PK, Ordovas, JM, et al. (2010) Adherence to an (n-3) fatty acid/fish intake pattern is inversely associated with metabolic syndrome among Puerto Rican adults in the Greater Boston Area. J Nutr 140, 18461854.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gonzalez, HM, Tarraf, W, Gouskova, N, et al. (2015) Neurocognitive function among middle-aged and older Hispanic/Latinos: results from the Hispanic Community Health Study/Study of Latinos. Arch Clin Neuropsychol 30, 6877.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Strizich, G, Kaplan, RC, González, HM, et al. (2016) Glycemic control, cognitive function, and family support among middle-aged and older Hispanics with diabetes: the Hispanic Community Health Study/Study of Latinos. Diabetes Res Clin Pract 117, 6473.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hoos, T, Espinoza, N, Marshall, S, et al. (2012) Validity of the Global Physical Activity Questionnaire (GPAQ) in adult Latinas. J Phys Act Health 9, 698705.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Andresen, EM, Malmgren, JA, Carter, WB, et al. (1994) Screening for depression in well older adults: evaluation of a short form of the CES-D (Center for Epidemiologic Studies Depression Scale). Am J Prev Med 10, 7784.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Crichton, GE, Elias, MF & Torres, RV (2016) Sugar-sweetened soft drinks are associated with poorer cognitive function in individuals with type 2 diabetes: the Maine-Syracuse Longitudinal Study. Br J Nutr 115, 13971405.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Mottaghi, T, Amirabdollahian, F & Haghighatdoost, F (2018) Fruit and vegetable intake and cognitive impairment: a systematic review and meta-analysis of observational studies. Eur J Clin Nutr 72, 13361344.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Anstey, KJ, Mack, HA & Cherbuin, N (2009) Alcohol consumption as a risk factor for dementia and cognitive decline: meta-analysis of prospective studies. Am J Geriatr Psychiatry 17, 542555.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Qin, B, Xun, P, Jacobs, DR, et al. (2017) Intake of niacin, folate, vitamin B-6, and vitamin B-12 through young adulthood and cognitive function in midlife: the Coronary Artery Risk Development in Young Adults (CARDIA) study. Am J Clin Nutr 106, 10321040.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lee, L, Kang, SA, Lee, HO, et al. (2001) Relationships between dietary intake and cognitive function level in Korean elderly people. Public Health 115, 133138.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
La Rue, A, Koehler, KM, Wayne, SJ, et al. (1997) Nutritional status and cognitive functioning in a normally aging sample: a 6-year reassessment. Am J Clin Nutr 65, 2029.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Morris, MC, Evans, DA, Bienias, JL, et al. (2004) Dietary niacin and the risk of incident Alzheimer’s disease and of cognitive decline. J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatr 75, 10931099.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kim, H, Kim, G, Jang, W, et al. (2014) Association between intake of B vitamins and cognitive function in elderly Koreans with cognitive impairment. Nutr J 13, 118.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
An, Y, Feng, L, Zhang, X, et al. (2019) Dietary intakes and biomarker patterns of folate, vitamin B6, and vitamin B12 can be associated with cognitive impairment by hypermethylation of redox-related genes NUDT15 and TXNRD1. Clin Epigenet 11, 139.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Stefanidis, KB, Askew, CD, Greaves, K, et al. (2018) The effect of non-stroke cardiovascular disease states on risk for cognitive decline and dementia: a systematic and meta-analytic review. Neuropsychol Rev 28, 115.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kuzma, E, Lourida, I, Moore, SF, et al. (2018) Stroke and dementia risk: a systematic review and meta-analysis. Alzheimers Dement 14, 14161426.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Valenzuela, MJ & Sachdev, P (2006) Brain reserve and dementia: a systematic review. Psychol Med 36, 441454.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Stilling, RM, van de Wouw, M, Clarke, G, et al. (2016) The neuropharmacology of butyrate: the bread and butter of the microbiota-gut-brain axis? Neurochem Int 99, 110132.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Avgerinos, KI, Egan, JM, Mattson, MP, et al. (2020) Medium Chain Triglycerides induce mild ketosis and may improve cognition in Alzheimer’s disease. A systematic review and meta-analysis of human studies. Ageing Res Rev 58, 101001.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Geng, S, Zhu, W, Xie, C, et al. (2016) Medium-chain triglyceride ameliorates insulin resistance and inflammation in high fat diet-induced obese mice. Eur J Nutr 55, 931940.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gao, Z, Yin, J, Zhang, J, et al. (2009) Butyrate improves insulin sensitivity and increases energy expenditure in mice. Diabetes 58, 15091517.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kim, MH, Kang, SG, Park, JH, et al. (2013) Short-chain fatty acids activate GPR41 and GPR43 on intestinal epithelial cells to promote inflammatory responses in mice. Gastroenterology 145, 396406.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
McNabney, SM & Henagan, TM (2017) Short chain fatty acids in the colon and peripheral tissues: a focus on butyrate, colon cancer, obesity and insulin resistance. Nutrients 9, 1348.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Vinolo, MA, Rodrigues, HG, Nachbar, RT, et al. (2011) Regulation of inflammation by short chain fatty acids. Nutrients 3, 858876.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Fernando, WM, Martins, IJ, Goozee, KG, et al. (2015) The role of dietary coconut for the prevention and treatment of Alzheimer’s disease: potential mechanisms of action. Br J Nutr 114, 114.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Mullins, RJ, Diehl, TC, Chia, CW, et al. (2017) Insulin resistance as a link between amyloid-beta and tau pathologies in Alzheimer’s disease. Front Aging Neurosci 9, 118.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Willette, AA, Modanlo, N, Kapogiannis, D, et al. (2015) Insulin resistance predicts medial temporal hypermetabolism in mild cognitive impairment conversion to Alzheimer disease. Diabetes 64, 19331940.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Fortier, M, Castellano, CA, Croteau, E, et al. (2019) A ketogenic drink improves brain energy and some measures of cognition in mild cognitive impairment. Alzheimers Dement 15, 625634.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lee, J, Fu, Z, Chung, M, et al. (2018) Role of milk and dairy intake in cognitive function in older adults: a systematic review and meta-analysis. Nutr J 17, 82.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Crichton, GE, Bryan, J, Murphy, KJ, et al. (2010) Review of dairy consumption and cognitive performance in adults: findings and methodological issues. Dement Geriatr Cogn Disord 30, 352361.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lee, YS, Cho, Y & Shin, M-J (2015) Dietary very long chain saturated fatty acids and metabolic factors: findings from the Korea National Health and Nutrition Examination Survey 2013. Clin Nutr Res 4, 182189.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Valls-Pedret, C, Sala-Vila, A, Serra-Mir, M, et al. (2015) Mediterranean diet and age-related cognitive decline: a randomized clinical trial. JAMA Intern Med 175, 10941103.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cherubini, A, Andres-Lacueva, C, Martin, A, et al. (2007) Low plasma n-3 fatty acids and dementia in older persons: the InCHIANTI study. J Gerontol A Biol Sci Med Sci 62, 11201126.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Yamagishi, K, Ikeda, A, Chei, CL, et al. (2017) Serum α-linolenic and other n-3 fatty acids, and risk of disabling dementia: community-based nested case-control study. Clin Nutr 36, 793797.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bigornia, SJ, Scott, TM, Harris, WS, et al. (2018) Prospective associations of erythrocyte composition and dietary intake of n-3 and n-6 PUFA with measures of cognitive function. Nutrients 10, 1253.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kalmijn, S, Feskens, EJ, Launer, LJ, et al. (1997) Polyunsaturated fatty acids, antioxidants, and cognitive function in very old men. Am J Epidemiol 145, 3341.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
MacDonald-Wicks, L, McEvoy, M, Magennis, E, et al. (2019) Dietary long-chain fatty acids and cognitive performance in older Australian adults. Nutrients 11, 711.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Pan, A, Chen, M, Chowdhury, R, et al. (2012) α-Linolenic acid and risk of cardiovascular disease: a systematic review and meta-analysis. Am J Clin Nutr 96, 12621273.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hooper, L, Al-Khudairy, L, Abdelhamid, AS, et al. (2018) n-6 fats for the primary and secondary prevention of cardiovascular disease. Cochrane Database Syst Rev 7, CD011094.Google Scholar
Anstey, KJ, Ashby-Mitchell, K & Peters, R (2017) Updating the evidence on the association between serum cholesterol and risk of late-life dementia: review and meta-analysis. J Alzheimers Dis 56, 215228.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Beydoun, MA, Kaufman, JS, Satia, JA, et al. (2007) Plasma n-3 fatty acids and the risk of cognitive decline in older adults: the Atherosclerosis Risk in Communities Study. Am J Clin Nutr 85, 11031111.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Wada, M, DeLong, CJ, Hong, YH, et al. (2007) Enzymes and receptors of prostaglandin pathways with arachidonic acid-derived versus eicosapentaenoic acid-derived substrates and products. J Biol Chem 282, 2225422266.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Crichton, GE, Elias, MF, Davey, A, et al. (2015) Higher cognitive performance is prospectively associated with healthy dietary choices: the Maine Syracuse Longitudinal Study. J Prev Alzheimers Dis 2, 2432.Google ScholarPubMed
Mossavar-Rahmani, Y, Shaw, PA, Wong, WW, et al. (2015) Applying recovery biomarkers to calibrate self-report measures of energy and protein in the Hispanic Community Health Study/Study of Latinos. Am J Epidemiol 181, 9961007.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Mossavar-Rahmani, Y, Sotres-Alvarez, D, Wong, WW, et al. (2017) Applying recovery biomarkers to calibrate self-report measures of sodium and potassium in the Hispanic Community Health Study/Study of Latinos. J Hum Hypertens 31, 462473.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Figure 0

Table 1. Factor loadings of dietary fatty acid patterns†(Mean values and standard deviations)

Figure 1

Table 2. Age and sex adjusted characteristics by dietary fatty acid pattern score (quintile) (n 8942)†(Mean values and standard deviations)

Figure 2

Table 3. Adjusted mean of global cognitive function score by dietary fatty acid pattern quintile (n 8942)(Mean values with their standard errors)

Figure 3

Table 4. Associations between dietary fatty acid patterns and individual neurocognitive tests (n 8942)(β-coefficients and standard error of the mean)

Supplementary material: File

Karazurna et al. supplementary material

Tables S1-S8

Download Karazurna et al. supplementary material(File)
File 55.6 KB