Hostname: page-component-cd9895bd7-jkksz Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-12-25T04:24:05.093Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

The ‘open-earedness’ hypothesis and the development of age-related aesthetic reactions to music in elementary school children

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  11 June 2008

Reinhard Kopiez
Affiliation:
Hanover University of Music and Drama, Institute for Research in Music Education, Emmichplatz 1, 30175 Hanover, [email protected], [email protected]
Marco Lehmann
Affiliation:
Hanover University of Music and Drama, Institute for Research in Music Education, Emmichplatz 1, 30175 Hanover, [email protected], [email protected]

Abstract

This study investigates age-related changes in musical preference in elementary school children. The tolerance towards unconventional musical styles has been called ‘open-earedness’ (Hargreaves, 1982a), and it is assumed to decline with increasing age. Musical preferences of 186 students from grade 1 to 4 (age range: 6–10 years) were measured by using a 5-point iconographic rating scale. Eight sound examples were presented in a sound questionnaire. Results showed a decline of open-earedness for unconventional music (classical, ethnic and avant-garde music) from grade 1 to 2 (age: 7–8 years). However, this effect disappeared when classical music was excluded from data analysis. Only a few absolute rejections of unconventional musical styles were found, and the mean preference ratings did not exceed the neutral mean range. Future studies will have to consider additional factors of influence to make clear predictions about the point in time when this transition occurs.

Type
Research Article
Copyright
Copyright © Cambridge University Press 2008

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

ABELES, H. F. & CHUNG, J. W. (1996) ‘Responses to music’, in Hodges, D. A. (Ed), Handbook of Music Psychology (2nd edn, pp. 285342). San Antonio: IMR Press.Google Scholar
BEHNE, K.-E. (1996) ‘Musikgeschmack in den 90er Jahren [Musical taste in the 1990s]’, Musikforum, 32 (84): 2541.Google Scholar
BEHNE, K.-E. (1997a) ‘The development of ‘Musikerleben’ in adolescence: How and why young people listen to music’, in Deliège, I. & Sloboda, J. A. (Eds), Perception and Cognition of Music (pp. 143–59). Hove, UK: Psychology Press.Google Scholar
BEHNE, K.-E. (1997b) ‘Träumen und Tanzen - zur Funktionalität von Musikpräferenzen . . . . sowie vom unverhofften Segen der ‘Klassik’ in der Werbung [Dreaming and dancing - the functionality of music preferences and the unexpected boon of ‘classics’ in advertisement]’, Musicae Scientiae, 1 (2): 205–23.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
BEHNE, K.-E. (2007) ‘Aspekte einer Sozialpsychologie des Musikgeschmacks [Sociopsychological aspects of musical taste]’, in Motte-Haber, H. d. l. & Neuhoff, H. (Eds), Musiksoziologie (Handbuch der Systematischen Musikwissenschaft, Vol. 4, pp. 418–55). Laaber: Laaber Verlag.Google Scholar
BOURDIEU, P. (2007) Distinction: A Social Critique of the Judgements of Taste (Nice, R., Trans.). Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
COSTA, P. T. & MCCRAE, R. R. (1992) Revised NEO Personality Inventory (NEO PI-R) and NEO Five Factor Inventory. Professional Manual. Odessa, FL: Psychological Assessment Ressources.Google Scholar
DOWNEY, J. E. (1897) ‘A musical experiment’, American Journal of Psychology, 9: 63–9.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
ERDFELDER, E., FAUL, F. & BUCHNER, A. (1996) ‘GPOWER: A general power analysis program’, Behavior Research Methods, Instruments & Computers, 28 (1): 111.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
FEIERABEND, S. & MOHR, I. (2004) ‘Mediennutzung von Klein- und Vorschulkindern [Media utilization of toddlers and preschool children]’, Media Perspektiven (9): 453–61.Google Scholar
FINNÄ, L. (1989) ‘How can musical preferences be modified? A research review’, Bulletin of the Council for Research in Music Education, 102: 158.Google Scholar
FREY-VOR, G. & SCHUMACHER, G. (2004) ‘Kinder und Medien 2003 [Children and media 2003]’, Media Perspektiven (9): 426–40.Google Scholar
GEMBRIS, H. (2002) ‘The development of musical abilities’, in Colwell, R. J. & Richardson, C. (Eds), New Handbook of Research on Music Teaching and Learning (pp. 487508). New York: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
GEMBRIS, H. (2005) ‘Musikalische Präferenzen [Musical preferences]’, in Stoffer, T. H. & Oerter, R. (Eds), Spezielle Musikpsychologie (Enzyklopädie der Psychologie, Vol. D/VII/2, pp. 279342). Göttingen: Hogrefe.Google Scholar
GEMBRIS, H. & SCHELLBERG, G. (2003). Musical preferences of elementary school children. In Kopiez, R., Lehmann, A. C., Wolther, I. & Wolf, C. (Eds.), Proceedings of the 5th Triennial Conference of the European Society for the Cognitive Sciences of Music (ESCOM) (pp. 552–53). Hanover, 8–13 September [CD-ROM].Google Scholar
GILMAN, B. I. (1891) ‘Report on an experimental test of musical expressiveness’, American Journal of Psychology, 4: 558–76.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
GREER, R. D., DOROW, L. G. & RANDALL, A. (1974) ‘Music listening preferences of elementary school children’, Journal of Research in Music Education, 22 (4): 284–91.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
GREER, R. D., DOROW, L. G., WACHHAUS, G. & WHITE, E. R. (1973) ‘Adult approval and students’ music selection behavior’, Journal of Research in Music Education, 21 (4): 345–54.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
HAGER, W. (1996) ‘Zusammenhangs- und Unterschiedsmaße’, in Erdfelder, E., Mausfeld, R., Meiser, T. & Rudinger, G. (Eds), Handbuch Quantitative Methoden (pp. 8798). Weinheim, Germany: Beltz.Google Scholar
HARGREAVES, D. J. (1982a) ‘Preference and prejudice in music: A psychological approach’, Popular Music and Society, 8: 1318.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
HARGREAVES, D. J. (1982b) ‘The development of aesthetic reactions to music’, Psychology of Music (Special issue): 51–54.Google Scholar
HARGREAVES, D. J. (1987) ‘Development of liking for familiar and unfamiliar melodies’, Bulletin of the Council for Research in Music Education 91: 6569.Google Scholar
HARGREAVES, D. J. (1995) ‘Effects of age, gender, and training on musical preferences of British secondary school students’, Journal of Research in Music Education, 43 (3): 242–50.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
HARGREAVES, D. J., COMBER, C. & COLLEY, A. (1995) ‘Effects of age, gender, and training on musical preferences of British secondary school students’, Journal of Research in Music Education, 43 (3): 242–50.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
HARGREAVES, D. J., MARSHALL, N. A. & NORTH, A. C. (2003) ‘Music education in the twenty-first century: A psychological perspective’, British Journal of Music Education, 20 (2): 147–63.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
HARGREAVES, D. J., NORTH, A. C. & TARRANT, M. (2006) ‘Musical preference and taste in childhood and adolescence’, in McPherson, G. (Ed), The Child as Musician (pp. 135–54). New York: Oxford University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
KLOPPENBURG, J. (2005) ‘Musikalische Präferenzen. Einstellungen, Vorurteile, Einstellungsänderungen [Musical preferences. Attitudes, prejudices, changes]’, in de la Motte-Haber, H. & Rötter, G. (Eds), Musikpsychologie (Handbuch der Systematischen Musikwissenschaft, Vol. 3, pp. 356–93). Laaber: Laaber.Google Scholar
KREUTZ, G. (2001) ‘Musikgenres im Präferenzurteil von Schülern der 4. Grundschulklasse. Ergebnisse einer Pilotstudie [Preferences for music genres in 4th graders. Results from a pilot study]’, Diskussion Musikpädagogik, 11 (3): 7484.Google Scholar
KREUTZ, G. & LITTA, G. (2004) ‘Musikpräferenzen und aggressive Einstellungen in der vierten Grundschulklasse [Music preferences and aggressive attitudes in fourth graders]’, Musikpsychologie, 17: 7288.Google Scholar
LEBLANC, A. (1979) ‘Generic style: Music preferences of fifth-grade students’, Journal of Research in Music Education, 27 (4): 255–70.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
LEBLANC, A. (1991) Effect of maturation/aging on music listening preference: A review of the literature. Ninth National Symposium on Research in Music Behavior. Cannon Beach, Oregon, March 7–9.Google Scholar
LEBLANC, A. & COTE, R. (1983) ‘Effects of tempo and performing medium on children's music preference’, Journal of Research in Music Education, 31 (1): 5766.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
LEBLANC, A., SIMS, W. L., SIIVOLA, C. & OBERT, M. (1996) ‘Music styles preferences of different age listeners’, Journal of Research in Music Education, 44 (1): 4959.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
LEHMANN, A. C. (1994) Habituelle und situative Rezeptionsweisen beim Musikhören: eine einstellungstheoretische Untersuchung [Habitual and situational modes of listening to music: A study on a theory of attitudes]. Frankfurt a.M.: Peter Lang.Google Scholar
LITLE, P. & ZUCKERMAN, M. (1986) ‘Sensation seeking and music preferences’, Personality and Individual Differences, 7 (4): 575–77.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
MCCOWN, W., KEISER, R. & MULHEARN, S. (1997) ‘The role of personality and gender in preference for exaggerated bass in music’, Personality and Individual Differences, 23 (4): 543–47.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
MINKENBERG, H. (1991) Das Musikerleben von Kindern im Alter von fünf bis zehn Jahren [Musical experience of children aged five to ten: A longitudinal study as the basis for research into variations in reception]. Frankfurt a. m.: Peter Lang.Google Scholar
MÜLLER, R. (2000) ‘Die feinen Unterschiede zwischen verbalen und klingenden Musikpräferenzen Jugendlicher: Eine computerunterstutzte Befragung mit dem Fragebogen-Autorensystem-MultiMedia [The subtle differences between verbally-expressed and aural music preferences in teenagers: A computer-supported survey using the questionnaire-authoring software MultiMedia]’, Musikpsychologie, 15: 8798.Google Scholar
NORTH, A. C. & HARGREAVES, D. J. (1997) ‘Liking for musical styles’, Musicae Scientiae, 1 (1): 109128.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
RAWLINGS, D. & CIANCARELLI, V. (1997) ‘Music preference and the five-factor model of the NEO Personality Inventory’, Psychology of Music, 25 (2): 120–32.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
RUSSELL, P. A. (1997) ‘Musical tastes and society’, in Hargreaves, D. J. & North, A. C. (Eds), The Social Psychology of Music (pp. 141–58). Oxford: Oxford University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
SCHELLBERG, G. & GEMBRIS, H. (2003) ‘Was Grundschulkinder (nicht) hören wollen. Eine neue Studie über Musikpräferenzen von Kindern der 1. bis 4. Klasse [What elementary school children don't want to hear: A new study of musical preferences among children in grades 1–4]’, Musik in der Grundschule, (4): 48–52.Google Scholar
SCHERER, K. R. (2005) ‘What are emotions? And how can they be measured?Social Science Information, 44 (4): 695729.CrossRefGoogle Scholar