Hostname: page-component-f554764f5-rvxtl Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2025-04-22T20:10:49.578Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Diverging cognitive benefits from education between rural and urban middle-aged and older adults in the USA

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  17 April 2025

Roger Wong*
Affiliation:
Department of Public Health and Preventive Medicine, Norton College of Medicine, SUNY Upstate Medical University, Syracuse, NY, USA Department of Geriatrics, Norton College of Medicine, SUNY Upstate Medical University, Syracuse, NY, USA
Amer Mansour
Affiliation:
Norton College of Medicine, SUNY Upstate Medical University, Syracuse, NY, USA
*
Correspondence: Roger Wong. Email: [email protected]
Rights & Permissions [Opens in a new window]

Abstract

Background

Subjective cognitive decline (SCD) is defined as self-reported increase in confusion or memory loss. There is limited research on the interplay between rural–urban residence and education on SCD.

Aims

Examine rural–urban differences in SCD, and whether education moderates this relationship.

Method

Respondents aged ≥45 years were queried about SCD in the 2022 Behavioral Risk Factor Surveillance System data, creating a sample size of 63 890. A logistic regression analysed the association between rural–urban residence and SCD, and moderation was tested by an interaction with education.

Results

SCD was more common among rural (12.0%) compared with urban (10.7%) residents. Rural residence was associated with 9% significantly higher odds of SCD compared with urban residence after adjusting for sociodemographic and health covariates (adjusted odds ratio (aOR) = 1.09, P = 0.01). There was a negative relationship between education level and SCD, including the association of college degree with 15% lower odds of SCD compared with less than high school degree (aOR = 0.85, P < 0.01). Education was a significant moderator, with higher education associated with lower odds of SCD for urban, but not rural, residents.

Conclusions

Rural setting and lower education were associated with higher odds of SCD, but higher education was protective for only urban residents. These results indicate that higher education may be a gateway for more opportunities and resources in urban settings, with cascading impacts on cognition. Future research should examine reasons for the diverging cognitive benefits from education depending on rural–urban residence.

Type
Paper
Creative Commons
Creative Common License - CCCreative Common License - BY
This is an Open Access article, distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution licence (https://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/), which permits unrestricted re-use, distribution and reproduction, provided the original article is properly cited.
Copyright
© The Author(s), 2025. Published by Cambridge University Press on behalf of Royal College of Psychiatrists

Subjective cognitive decline (SCD) is a self-experienced decline in cognitive ability with limited or no impairment of performance on clinical standardised cognitive tests. Reference Jessen, Amariglio, Van Boxtel, Breteler, Ceccaldi and Chételat1 One study on the epidemiology of SCD estimated its prevalence to be around 24% in patients above the age of 60 years. Reference Röhr, Pabst, Riedel-Heller, Jessen, Turana and Handajani2 Research has identified a potential association between SCD and future cognitive impairment, with one systematic review reporting a 2.2-fold increase in risk of dementia for patients who suffer from SCD when compared with normal ageing. Reference Parfenov, Zakharov, Kabaeva and Vakhnina3 Similarly, a cohort study of nearly 560 000 patients above the age of 66 years noted a dementia incidence of 8.59 per 1000 person-years in patients with SCD compared with 5.66 in those without SCD. Reference Lee, Kang, Lee, Kim, Kim and Yu4 SCD, therefore, represents an important early indicator of dementia, which currently has limited strategies for management or prevention.

Environment and cognition

Prior research has linked environmental and geographical factors to cognitive health. For example, one study examined how neighbourhood physical disorder and social cohesion were associated with dementia risk, using 9 years of data from a national sample of US Medicare beneficiaries. Reference Wong and Wang5 One of the key findings was that high physical disorder (litter, graffiti, housing/storefront vacancies) was associated with an 11% significantly higher dementia risk, which is probably explained by unfavourable settings that limit the ability to engage in exercise and other health behaviours. In rural ageing research, a 2018 paper using Health and Retirement Study (HRS) data found there was a 79% increased dementia risk, which may be explained by well-documented gaps in a variety of medical, public health and social services infrastructure. Reference Weden, Shih, Kabeto and Langa6 More recently, a separate HRS study published in 2022 found that rural residence was associated with lower cognitive function, which was measured based on performance in word recall, serial subtraction of sevens and backwards-counting testing. Reference Glauber7 Likewise, a 2023 study using a national US sample in the Reasons for Geographic and Racial Differences in Stroke Study data also found that rural residents had 34% significantly higher odds of incident cognitive impairment after adjusting for sociodemographic factors. Reference Harris, Bennion, Magnusson, Howard, Wadley and McClure8 The association between rural environments and the development of dementia may also be explained by the decreased opportunity for social interactions that exist in rural areas when compared with urban. Several studies have identified a link between decreased social interaction and cognitive decline, Reference Duffner, Deckers, Cadar, Steptoe, de Vugt and Köhler9,Reference Fancourt, Steptoe and Cadar10 suggesting this as a potential underlying factor for these rural–urban disparities.

Education and cognition

Based on recent data examining the relationship between educational attainment and cognition, low education status has been hypothesised as an additional risk factor for cognitive impairment. One 2024 study using a US sample of middle-aged and older adults from the 2021 Behavioral Risk Factor Surveillance System (BRFSS) found that, compared with respondents with less than a high school degree, some college education and a college degree were associated with 55 and 54% significantly lower odds of SCD, respectively, after adjusting for sociodemographic and health factors. Reference Chen and Wong11 Another 2024 study, using a 10-year prospective national US sample of older adults, found that, compared with those with less than a high school degree, obtaining a high school degree was associated with 29% significantly lower dementia risk; obtaining a college degree was further associated with a 43% significantly lower dementia risk after adjusting for sociodemographics and health. Reference Wong and Soong12 One theory that has been hypothesised to explain this protective effect of education on cognitive decline is related to the concept of cognitive reserve, which is defined as the ability of the brain to maintain cognition in response to ageing or disease. Reference Stern, Arenaza-Urquijo, Bartrés-Faz, Belleville, Cantilon and Chetelat13 Higher educational background is widely considered to be a potential factor associated with improved cognitive reserve, and has therefore been theorised to be protective against cognitive decline. Reference Stern, Arenaza-Urquijo, Bartrés-Faz, Belleville, Cantilon and Chetelat13

Innovation and research aim

Although there has been research linking rural–urban residence and education to cognitive decline, to our knowledge there is no existing research that has examined the intersection among these three constructs. Therefore, our present study has two objectives: (a) to examine the association between rural–urban residence and SCD among US middle-aged and older adults; and (b) to examine whether education level moderates the association between rural–urban residence and SCD. Building on prior research, we hypothesised that residing in rural settings would be associated with increased odds of SCD. With this assumption, we also hypothesised that higher educational attainment would be more beneficial for rural residents, and would thus attenuate the probability of SCD compared with urban residents.

Method

Data source

Cross-sectional data were retrieved from the 2022 BRFSS through telephone surveys on health behaviours, conditions and preventive services. The BRFSS cognitive decline module was eligible only for adults of at least age 45 years, and this optional module was administered to 12 US states (FL, ID, IN, ME, NV, OR, RI, SC, UT, VA, VT and WI). The sample size was 63 890 respondents who completed this module and met the minimum age inclusion criteria.

The authors assert that all procedures contributing to this work comply with the ethical standards of the relevant national and institutional committees on human experimentation, and with the Helsinki Declaration of 1975 as revised in 2013. All procedures involving human subjects/patients were approved by the SUNY Upstate Institutional Review Board for the Protection of Human Subjects (no. 2221941-1). Verbal consent was witnessed and formally recorded. The BRFSS interviewers obtained verbal informed consent from all respondents during the initial contact and screening process, preceding all survey questions.

SCD

SCD was the dependent variable. In the BRFSS cognitive decline module, respondents were asked, ‘During the past 12 months, have you experienced confusion or memory loss that is happening more often or is getting worse?’. Responses were binary (no or yes) and other responses (don’t know/not sure or refused) were coded as missing. A prior study has reported this BRFSS SCD variable to have high specificity and adequate sensitivity in psychometric testing. Reference Olivari, Baumgart, Taylor and McGuire14

Rural–urban residence

Rural–urban residence is a BRFSS-calculated binary variable in which respondents were categorised as residing in either a rural or urban county. This variable was developed using the National Center for Health Statistics Urban–Rural Classification Scheme for Counties, which uses the US Census as the underlying data. All counties are classified based on: (a) their status under the Office of Management and Budget designation as metropolitan, micropolitan or non-core, (b) the population size of metropolitan statistical areas (MSAs) and (c) the location of principal city populations within the largest MSAs. Rural was defined as non-metropolitan counties that did not qualify as micropolitan, and rural counties had a median population size of 11 218 people and population density of 18 persons per square mile. The BRFSS classified all other counties as urban.

Education level

The highest level of education was a BRFSS-calculated variable in which respondents were classified into four categories: (a) less than high school (did not graduate), (b) graduated from high school, (c) some college (attended 1–3 years of college or technical school) and (d) graduated from college (at least 4 years).

Covariates

Several sociodemographic and health variables were included as covariates in all logistic regression analyses. Sociodemographic covariates included age group (45–49, 50–54, 55–59, 60–64, 65–69, 70–74, 75–79 or 80+ years), sex (male or female), marital status (married or not married) and race and ethnicity (non-Hispanic White (hereafter, White), non-Hispanic Black (hereafter, Black), Hispanic, non-Hispanic Asian (hereafter, Asian) or non-Hispanic Other (hereafter, Other)). Health covariates included self-rated general health (poor, fair, good, very good or excellent), body mass index (BMI) category (underweight, normal weight, overweight or obese), cardiovascular disease (CVD) history, diabetes history, stroke history and depression history.

Analysis plan

A Pearson chi-square test was conducted to examine the unadjusted association between rural–urban residence and SCD. We constructed three logistic regression models that sequentially adjusted for sociodemographic and health covariates. Model A was an unadjusted crude model containing only rural–urban residence, and model B was adjusted for all sociodemographic covariates. Our fully adjusted final model, model C, adjusted for all sociodemographic and health covariates. Our final logistic regression model had an average variance inflation factor of 1.11, which indicated that no multicollinearity was present. For our interaction model, we regressed SCD on the interaction between rural–urban residence and education, adjusting for all sociodemographic and health covariates. Finally, we also performed a sensitivity analysis in which we examined the rural–SCD association, along with the rural–education interaction on SCD, stratified by each of the eight age groups to account for differential life experiences.

Approximately 9% of the data was lost from listwise deletion and we did not identify any missing data patterns. The variable with the most missingness was BMI category (6.5%) followed by CVD history (1.2%). To maximise the full number of eligible respondents in the data and minimise bias from missing data, we utilised multiple imputation by chained equations (MICE) to generate 15 imputed data files with 10 iterations each for logistic regression analyses. Results from the MICE models are reported, but the conclusions were the same from listwise deletion. All analyses were conducted using Stata 18 with two-tailed tests at a 0.05 significance level.

Results

Sample characteristics

Among 63 890 US middle-aged and older adults in the BRFSS, about 81% were urban and 19% resided in rural geographic areas (Table 1). About 11% reported SCD, which was significantly more common among rural (12.0%) compared with urban (10.7%) residents (X 2[1] = 15.5, P < 0.001). Regardless of rural–urban residence, most respondents completed college (43.7%) and were in the 65–69 years age group (15.6%). Slightly more than half identified as female (55.4%) and married (57%). The sample was predominantly White (86.9%), followed by lower proportions of Black (6.6.%), Hispanic (3.6%) and Asian (0.7%). For health-related characteristics, most reported self-rated health as ‘very good’ (34.1%) and had a BMI overweight (36.8%). History of chronic and mental health conditions was fairly infrequent, with the most common being depression (19.4%) and diabetes (17.8%).

Table 1 Sample characteristics

SCD, subjective cognitive decline; BMI, body mass index.

Main effects

In our unadjusted logistic regression model (model A), rural residence was associated with 13% significantly higher odds of SCD compared with urban residents (odds ratio 1.13, 95% CI 1.06–1.20, P < 0.001; Table 2). This association remained significant after adjusting for sociodemographic covariates in model B (adjusted odds ratio (aOR) 1.10, 95% CI 1.03–1.17, P < 0.01). Our fully adjusted model (model C), with all sociodemographic and health covariates indicated residing in rural areas, was associated with about 9% increased odds of SCD (aOR 1.09, 95% CI 1.02–1.16, P = 0.01).

Table 2 Adjusted odds of subjective cognitive decline, by rural–urban residence

aOR, adjusted odds ratio; CI, confidence interval; BMI, body mass index; CVD, cardiovascular disease.

Adjusted for sociodemographics (model B), increasingly higher levels of education were also significantly associated with lower odds of SCD. For instance, compared with those who did not complete high school, high school degree and college degree were associated with 28% (aOR 0.72, 95% CI 0.65–0.80, P < 0.001) and 51% (aOR 0.49, 95% CI 0.44–0.55, P < 0.001) lower odds of SCD, respectively. In the fully adjusted model, only a college degree was significantly associated with 15% lower odds of SCD (aOR 0.85, 95% CI 0.76–0.96, P = 0.01).

Interaction effect

An interaction between rural–urban residence and education level was included in our fully adjusted logistic regression model. A statistically significant Wald test evaluated the joint significance of the interaction between rural–urban residence and education (X 2[6] = 13.5, P = 0.04). In particular, urban residents with increasingly higher levels of education had lower predicted probabilities of SCD (Fig. 1). For example, the predicted probability of SCD for college-educated urban residents was 10.2% (95% CI 9.7–10.6%) compared with 11.9% (95% CI 10.8–13.0%) for urban residents who did not complete high school. This contrast between the two educational levels among urban residents had a difference of 1.7%, which was statistically significant (95% CI 0.1–3.0%, P < 0.01). These educational benefits were not observed for rural residents, such as the predicted probability of 11.5% (95% CI 10.5–12.4%) for college graduates versus 11.3% (95% CI 9.2–13.5%) for those who did not complete high school, with the difference of 0.2% being non-significant (95% CI −2.2 to –2.5%, P = 0.93).

Fig. 1 Moderating role of education level on association between rural–urban residence and SCD (subjective cognitive decline).

Sensitivity analysis

We performed a sensitivity analysis examining the association between rural–urban residence and SCD, stratified by the eight age groups (Supplementary Table 1). In general, rural residence was associated with higher odds of SCD, although this relationship was significant for only three age groups throughout the 45–80+ years age range (50–54, 65–69 and 80+ years). For the urban–education interaction, higher levels of education did not have significant associations with SCD for generally the oldest age groups (55–59, 70–74, 75–79 and 80+ years).

Discussion

This study analysed the association between rural–urban residence and SCD, and whether education may moderate this relationship. Our results indicate that, compared with urban settings, residing in rural areas was associated with significantly higher odds of SCD among US middle-aged and older adults. Higher levels of education were associated with significantly lower odds of SCD, and we found the probabilities of SCD to be lower only among urban residents who had obtained higher levels of education.

Our finding on the association between rurality and cognitive decline is consistent with numerous recent literature surrounding this topic. For example, a 2023 study identified a positive association between rural residence during childhood and poor cognition later in life. Reference Peterson, Gilsanz, Lor, George, Ko and Wagner15 Similarly, a cross-sectional study conducted in 2021 identified significantly higher rates of dementia and mild cognitive impairment in rural individuals when compared with urban and suburban populations. Reference Liu, Liu, Sun, Lee, Tang and Chiu16 While the results demonstrated in our study are aligned with these findings, the extent to which rurality plays a role in cognition remains unclear depending on the severity of cognitive impairment. For instance, one study reported an odds ratio of 3.76 when comparing the prevalence of mild cognitive impairment between rural and urban individuals. Reference Liu, Li, An, Cheng, Chen and Zou17 Another study identified a relative risk of dementia of about 79%, Reference Weden, Shih, Kabeto and Langa6 while our data in this present study suggested 9–13% increased odds for SCD for rural residents.

The results of our study also revealed an inverse association between education level and odds for cognitive decline, which aligns with much of the recent literature on this topic. Several studies have identified improvements in language, memory, attention and executive function in individuals who had attained higher education. Reference Chen, Lv, Li, Zhang, Chen and Liu18Reference Fletcher, Topping, Zheng and Lu20 Our data similarly suggested that individuals who had attained a college degree have a 15% decreased odds for SCD when compared with those who did not complete high school. Many studies theorise that cognitive reserve is the underlying mechanism that explains the improved cognition in older adults who have a greater educational background. Reference Clouston, Smith, Mukherjee, Zhang, Hou and Link19,Reference Lövdén, Fratiglioni, Glymour, Lindenberger and Tucker-Drob21,Reference Wilson, Yu, Lamar, Schneider, Boyle and Bennett22 Additionally, a 2019 study identified decreased decline of grey and white matter structure in patients who had attained higher levels of education, Reference Chen, Lv, Li, Zhang, Chen and Liu18 offering an additional potential mechanism for improved cognition later in life.

When comparing the influence of education on SCD in rural versus urban settings, our findings indicate that this has a limited benefit on SCD in rural environments while playing a greater role in urban populations, especially for those who are middle-aged compared with older adults. Given these findings, we theorise that education-based approaches aimed at preventing or slowing cognitive decline are more effective at earlier ages. This may be due to older adults often retiring from the workforce or decreased neuroplasticity at higher ages. Additional exploration is needed to identify the underlying mechanisms that contribute to differences in educational benefits between age groups. Our results also contradict one of our initial hypotheses that theorised an increased benefit of education on SCD in rural communities when compared with urban ones. One potential explanation for this result is based on the predominant occupation and lifestyle of rural versus urban residents. Occupation has been shown to play a role in health, with higher-paying, more stable jobs being associated with improved health outcomes. Reference Lawrence23,Reference Pampel, Krueger and Denney24 These optimal jobs are generally achieved through the increased opportunity for upward mobility provided by higher education. Reference Haskins25 This upward mobility, however, is not consistent between urban and rural communities. Generally, there is more opportunity for high-paying jobs for urban residents when compared with rural ones. Reference Ananian and Dellaferrera26 Similarly, the occupations in these two communities are also different, with urban residents being more likely to be employed in cognitively demanding jobs with rural residents being more likely to be employed in demanding physical occupations. Reference DePietro27 We theorise that the limited opportunities for higher-paying jobs in rural communities signifies that higher education is less consequential for occupation status and therefore plays a decreased role in cognitive health.

Another potential factor that may explain our findings is the disparity in healthcare that exists between rural and urban environments. The difference in access to healthcare between urban and rural areas is well described in the literature, with several studies identifying poor outcomes in several key areas, such as mortality rate and life expectancy, for rural residents when compared with their urban counterparts. Reference Cosby, McDoom-Echebiri, James, Khandekar, Brown and Hanna28Reference Walker and Brown30 This disparity of health outcomes is prevalent for degenerative cognitive disease, as demonstrated in recent articles. Reference Weden, Shih, Kabeto and Langa6,Reference Liu, Liu, Sun, Lee, Tang and Chiu16,Reference Cato, Ramer, Hajjar and Kulshreshtha31,Reference Miller, Wong, Caldwell, Rodrigues, Pudumjee and John32 It is possible that, in urban areas, individuals who are highly educated are more able to exercise their health literacy and navigate the available health systems. This may allow these urban residents to utilise the health systems more appropriately in order to improve and maintain their cognitive health. Reference Murimi and Harpel33 Likewise, we theorise that limited access to preventative medicine and mental healthcare in rural settings may reduce the potential benefits of education on cognitive health. Reference Larson and Correa-de-Araujo34 This, therefore, is a potential driving factor that may underlie the differential effect of higher education on the odds of SCD when comparing urban and rural populations.

The results of our study have implications for research, clinical practice and policy in regard to improved outcomes for SCD. In terms of research, additional studies are required to better understand the mechanisms that drive SCD. In particular, it is important to identify the specific pathways that define the association between education and SCD, especially characterisation of the factors that may allow education to improve cognition in urban populations, but not rural ones. Clinically, these data highlight several potential key risk factors for degenerative cognitive disease, which can guide clinical decision-making. This study identified both low education level and rural residence as potential risk factors for SCD, which in turn can be an indicator of neurocognitive disease. A patient who presents with these risk factors may require closer surveillance when screening for potential cognitive disorder. With regard to policy, improvements in and access to higher education are vital for improving outcomes of SCD; affordable and accessible education may help mitigate SCD. Although our results indicate a statistically significant association, additional analyses are needed to identify whether these educational benefits may translate to meaningful clinical significance and economic gain for the healthcare system. Nonetheless, our findings suggest that improving rates of higher education, particularly in urban areas, may have a positive impact on the odds of SCD and future cognitive decline.

There are several limitations to our study. First, the annual BRFSS data are cross-sectional, which inhibits causal associations between rurality and cognition. The cross-sectional nature of our data set introduces a limitation with regard to longitudinal changes in cognition throughout life. Individuals with different levels of initial cognition early in life may demonstrate varying transitions of cognition over time, and self-selection into higher education or rural–urban preferences, impacting their risk of SCD. Second, our measure for cognitive decline is subjective. It is possible that perceived cognitive impairment may differ depending on education and, more specifically, on health education. Third, geographic data are limited in the BRFSS, which allowed for only two classifications as either rural or urban. Finally, due to a large amount of missing data, our analyses were unable to adjust for socioeconomic factors such as income and assets, which is important to consider because these may potentially confound the association between rural–urban residence and SCD. Despite these limitations, the BRFSS is the ideal data-set for this investigation because rural–urban residence and cognition data are publicly available for a large national US sample. In addition, our present investigation is noteworthy because we believe it is among the first studies to analyse the interplay between rurality, education and cognition. Further research is needed to examine whether these associations are observed with other demographic groups, such as across different sex and racial-ethnic groups.

This study demonstrates the associations of education level and rural–urban residence with SCD. Our findings suggest that lower level of education and rural residence are both associated with higher odds of SCD. The data also reveal that increasingly higher levels of education are a protective factor against SCD in urban populations, but do not play a significant role in SCD for rural communities. These results identify key areas of improvement: increased access to education, updated screening guidelines for individuals at risk of degenerative cognitive disease and further research into other factors that underlie the development of SCD and impaired cognition.

Supplementary material

The supplementary material is available online at https://doi.org/10.1192/bjo.2025.45

Data availability

This study uses public BRFSS data, which may be obtained through the United States Centers for Disease Control and Prevention website: https://www.cdc.gov/brfss/index.html

Author contributions

R.W. conceived the study and conducted the analysis. A.M. critically contributed to data interpretation. Both authors drafted the manuscript and approved the final submitted version.

Funding

This research received no specific grant from any funding agency, commercial or not-for-profit sectors.

Declaration of interest

R.W. is on the editorial board of BJPysch Open and did not take part in the review or decision-making process of this paper.

Transparency declaration

The manuscript is an honest, accurate and transparent account of the study being reported. No important aspects of the study have been omitted.

References

Jessen, F, Amariglio, RE, Van Boxtel, M, Breteler, M, Ceccaldi, M, Chételat, G, et al. A conceptual framework for research on subjective cognitive decline in preclinical Alzheimer’s disease. Alzheimer Dement 2014; 10(6): 844–52.Google ScholarPubMed
Röhr, S, Pabst, A, Riedel-Heller, SG, Jessen, F, Turana, Y, Handajani, YS, et al. Estimating prevalence of subjective cognitive decline in and across international cohort studies of aging: a COSMIC study. Alzheimers Res Ther 2020; 12(1): 167.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Parfenov, VA, Zakharov, VV, Kabaeva, AR, Vakhnina, NV. Subjective cognitive decline as a predictor of future cognitive decline: a systematic review. Dement Neuropsychol 2020; 14 (3): 248–57.Google ScholarPubMed
Lee, YC, Kang, JM, Lee, H, Kim, K, Kim, S, Yu, TY, et al. Subjective cognitive decline and subsequent dementia: a nationwide cohort study of 579,710 people aged 66 years in South Korea. Alzheimers Res Ther 2020; 12(1): 52.Google ScholarPubMed
Wong, R, Wang, Y. Role of neighborhood physical disorder and social cohesion on racial and ethnic disparities in dementia risk. J Aging Health 2022; 34(9–10): 1178–87.Google ScholarPubMed
Weden, MM, Shih, RA, Kabeto, MU, Langa, KM. Secular trends in dementia and cognitive impairment of U.S. rural and urban older adults. Am J Prev Med 2018; 54(2): 164–72.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Glauber, R. Rural depopulation and the rural-urban gap in cognitive functioning among older adults. J Rural Health 2022; 38 (4): 696704.Google ScholarPubMed
Harris, ML, Bennion, E, Magnusson, KR, Howard, VJ, Wadley, VG, McClure, LA, et al. Rural versus urban residence in adulthood and incident cognitive impairment. Neuroepidemiology 2023; 57(4): 218–28.Google ScholarPubMed
Duffner, LA, Deckers, K, Cadar, D, Steptoe, A, de Vugt, M, Köhler, S. The role of cognitive and social leisure activities in dementia risk: assessing longitudinal associations of modifiable and on-modifiable risk factors. Epidemiol Psychiatr Sci 2022; 31: e5.Google ScholarPubMed
Fancourt, D, Steptoe, A, Cadar, D. Community engagement and dementia risk: time-to-event analyses from a national cohort study. J Epidemiol Community Health (1978) 2020; 74(1): 71–7.Google ScholarPubMed
Chen, Z, Wong, R. Association between cannabis use and subjective cognitive decline: findings from the Behavioral Risk Factor Surveillance System (BRFSS). Curr Alzheimer Res 2024; 20(11): 802–10.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Wong, R, Soong, D. Disparities in neighbourhood characteristics and 10-year dementia risk by nativity status. Epidemiol Psychiatr Sci 2024; 33: e7.Google ScholarPubMed
Stern, Y, Arenaza-Urquijo, EM, Bartrés-Faz, D, Belleville, S, Cantilon, M, Chetelat, G, et al. Whitepaper: defining and investigating cognitive reserve, brain reserve, and brain maintenance. Alzheimers Dement 2020; 16: 1305–11.Google ScholarPubMed
Olivari, BS, Baumgart, M, Taylor, CA, McGuire, LC. Population measures of subjective cognitive decline: a means of advancing public health policy to address cognitive health. Alzheimers Dement Transl Res Clin Intervent 2021; 7(1): e12142.Google ScholarPubMed
Peterson, RL, Gilsanz, P, Lor, Y, George, KM, Ko, M, Wagner, J, et al. Rural residence across the life course and late-life cognitive decline in KHANDLE: a causal inference study. Alzheimer’s Dement Diagn Assess Dis Monitor 2023; 15(1): e12399.Google ScholarPubMed
Liu, CC, Liu, CH, Sun, Y, Lee, HJ, Tang, LY, Chiu, MJ. Rural-urban disparities in the prevalence of mild cognitive impairment and dementia in Taiwan: a door-to-door nationwide study. J Epidemiol 2022; 32(11): 502–9.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Liu, D, Li, L, An, L, Cheng, G, Chen, C, Zou, M, et al. Urban–rural disparities in mild cognitive impairment and its functional subtypes among community-dwelling older residents in central China. Gen Psychiatr 2021; 34(5): e100564.Google ScholarPubMed
Chen, Y, Lv, C, Li, X, Zhang, J, Chen, K, Liu, Z, et al. The positive impacts of early-life education on cognition, leisure activity, and brain structure in healthy aging. Aging 2019; 11(14): 4923–42.Google ScholarPubMed
Clouston, SAP, Smith, DM, Mukherjee, S, Zhang, Y, Hou, W, Link, BG, et al. Education and cognitive decline: an integrative analysis of global longitudinal studies of cognitive aging. J Gerontol B Psycholog Sci Soc Sci 2020; 75 (7): E15160.Google ScholarPubMed
Fletcher, J, Topping, M, Zheng, F, Lu, Q. The effects of education on cognition in older age: evidence from genotyped siblings. Soc Sci Med 2021; 280: 114044.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lövdén, M, Fratiglioni, L, Glymour, MM, Lindenberger, U, Tucker-Drob, EM. Education and cognitive functioning across the life span. Psycholog Sci Publ Int 2020; 21(1): 641.Google ScholarPubMed
Wilson, RS, Yu, L, Lamar, M, Schneider, JA, Boyle, PA, Bennett, DA. Education and cognitive reserve in old age. Neurology 2019; 92 (10): E104150.Google ScholarPubMed
Lawrence, EM. Why do college graduates behave more healthfully than those who are less educated? J Health Soc Behav 2017; 58(3): 291306.Google ScholarPubMed
Pampel, FC, Krueger, PM, Denney, JT. Socioeconomic disparities in health behaviors. Annu Rev Sociol 2010; 36: 349–70.Google ScholarPubMed
Haskins, R, Isaacs JB, Sawhill IV. Getting Ahead or Losing Ground: Economic Mobility in America. The Brookings Institution, 2008.Google Scholar
Ananian, S, Dellaferrera, G. Employment and Wage Disparities between Rural and Urban Areas. ILO, 2024.Google Scholar
DePietro, A. Here are the Jobs that Employ Most of the Workforce in 15 Major U.S. Cities in 2022. Forbes, 2022 (https://www.forbes.com/sites/andrewdepietro/2022/01/27/here-are-the-jobs-that-employ-most-of-the-workforce-in-15-major-us-cities-in-2022/).Google Scholar
Cosby, AG, McDoom-Echebiri, MM, James, W, Khandekar, H, Brown, W, Hanna, HL. Growth and persistence of place-based mortality in the United States: the rural mortality penalty. Am J Public Health 2019; 109(1): 155–62.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cross, SH, Califf, RM, Warraich, HJ. Rural–urban disparity in mortality in the US from 1999 to 2019. JAMA 2021; 325(22): 2312.Google ScholarPubMed
Walker, BH, Brown, DC. Trends in lifespan variation across the spectrum of rural and urban places in the United States, 1990–2017. SSM Popul Health 2022; 19: 101213.Google Scholar
Cato, S, Ramer, S, Hajjar, I, Kulshreshtha, A. Alzheimer’s disease mortality as a function of urbanization level: 1999–2019. J Alzheimers Dis 2022; 87(4): 1461–6.Google ScholarPubMed
Miller, JB, Wong, CG, Caldwell, JZK, Rodrigues, J, Pudumjee, S, John, SE, et al. Cognitive aging in rural communities: preliminary memory characterization of a community cohort from Southern Nevada. Front Dement 2023; 2: 1236039.Google ScholarPubMed
Murimi, MW, Harpel, T. Practicing preventive health: the underlying culture among low-income rural populations. J Rural Health 2010; 26(3): 273–82.Google ScholarPubMed
Larson, S, Correa-de-Araujo, R. Preventive health examinations: a comparison along the rural–urban continuum. Womens Health Issues 2006; 16(2): 80–8.Google ScholarPubMed
Figure 0

Table 1 Sample characteristics

Figure 1

Table 2 Adjusted odds of subjective cognitive decline, by rural–urban residence

Figure 2

Fig. 1 Moderating role of education level on association between rural–urban residence and SCD (subjective cognitive decline).

Supplementary material: File

Wong and Mansour supplementary material

Wong and Mansour supplementary material
Download Wong and Mansour supplementary material(File)
File 16.3 KB
Submit a response

eLetters

No eLetters have been published for this article.