Hostname: page-component-78c5997874-4rdpn Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-11-05T03:34:00.481Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Assessing aetiological overlap between child and adult attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder symptoms in an extended family design

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  06 September 2023

Daniel L. Wechsler*
Affiliation:
Department of Psychology, Institute of Psychiatry, Psychology and Neuroscience, King's College London, UK
Fruhling V. Rijsdijk
Affiliation:
Social, Genetic and Developmental Psychiatry Centre, Institute of Psychiatry, Psychology and Neuroscience, King's College London, UK; and Department of Psychology, Faculty of Social Sciences, Anton de Kom University, Suriname
Nicoletta Adamo
Affiliation:
Social, Genetic and Developmental Psychiatry Centre, Institute of Psychiatry, Psychology and Neuroscience, King's College London, UK; and South London and Maudsley NHS Foundation Trust, London, UK
Espen M. Eilertsen
Affiliation:
PROMENTA Research Centre, University of Oslo, Norway; and Centre for Fertility and Health, Norwegian Institute of Public Health, Oslo, Norway
Yasmin I. Ahmadzadeh
Affiliation:
Social, Genetic and Developmental Psychiatry Centre, Institute of Psychiatry, Psychology and Neuroscience, King's College London, UK
Isabella Badini
Affiliation:
Social, Genetic and Developmental Psychiatry Centre, Institute of Psychiatry, Psychology and Neuroscience, King's College London, UK
Laurie J. Hannigan
Affiliation:
Department of Mental Disorders, Norwegian Institute of Public Health, Oslo, Norway; Nic Waals Institute, Lovisenberg Diaconal Hospital, Oslo, Norway; and Medical Research Council Integrative Epidemiology Unit, University of Bristol, UK
Eivind Ystrom
Affiliation:
PROMENTA Research Centre, University of Oslo, Norway; Department of Mental Disorders, Norwegian Institute of Public Health, Oslo, Norway; and School of Pharmacy, University of Oslo, Norway
Tom A. McAdams
Affiliation:
Social, Genetic and Developmental Psychiatry Centre, Institute of Psychiatry, Psychology and Neuroscience, King's College London, UK; and PROMENTA Research Centre, University of Oslo, Norway
*
Correspondence: Daniel L. Wechsler. Email: [email protected]
Rights & Permissions [Opens in a new window]

Abstract

Background

Several longitudinal studies have cast doubt on the aetiological overlap between child and adult attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder (ADHD). However, a lack of genetically sensitive data following children across adulthood precludes direct evaluation of aetiological overlap between child and adult ADHD.

Aims

We circumvent the existing gap in longitudinal data by exploring genetic overlap between maternal (adult) and offspring (child) ADHD and comorbid symptoms in an extended family cohort.

Method

Data were drawn from the Norwegian Mother, Father and Child Cohort Study, a Norwegian birth registry cohort of 114 500 children and their parents. Medical Birth Registry of Norway data were used to link extended families. Mothers self-reported their own ADHD symptoms when children were aged 3 years; reported children's ADHD symptoms at age 5 years; and children's ADHD, oppositional defiant disorder (ODD), conduct disorder, anxiety and depression symptoms at age 8 years. Genetic correlations were derived from Multiple-Children-of-Twins-and-Siblings and extended bivariate twin models.

Results

Phenotypic correlations between adult ADHD symptoms and child ADHD, ODD, conduct disorder, anxiety and depression symptoms at age 8 years were underpinned by medium-to-large genetic correlations (child ADHD: rG = 0.55, 95% CI 0.43−0.93; ODD: rG = 0.80, 95% CI 0.46−1; conduct disorder: rG = 0.44, 95% CI 0.28−1; anxiety: rG = 0.72, 95% CI 0.48−1; depression: rG = 1, 95% CI 0.66−1). These cross-generational adult–child genetic correlations were of a comparable magnitude to equivalent child–child genetic correlations with ADHD symptoms at age 5 years.

Conclusions

Our findings provide genetically sensitive evidence that ADHD symptoms in adulthood share a common genetic architecture with symptoms of ADHD and four comorbid disorders at age 8 years. These findings suggest that in the majority of cases, ADHD symptoms in adulthood are not aetiologically distinct from in childhood.

Type
Paper
Creative Commons
Creative Common License - CCCreative Common License - BY
This is an Open Access article, distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution licence (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/), which permits unrestricted re-use, distribution and reproduction, provided the original article is properly cited.
Copyright
Copyright © The Author(s), 2023. Published by Cambridge University Press on behalf of the Royal College of Psychiatrists

Attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder (ADHD) is a highly heritable neurodevelopmental disorder featuring impairing core symptoms of hyperactivity, impulsivity and inattention. The past decade has seen a shift toward widespread recognition that ADHD can affect adults as well as children, and a general consensus that most cases of childhood ADHD persist into at least young adulthood.Reference Kooij, Bijlenga, Salerno, Jaeschke, Bitter and Balazs1 Although most children with ADHD experience a reduction in their hyperactivity and impulsivity symptoms over time, inattention symptoms tend to remain relatively stable,Reference Willcutt2 and in a small but substantial portion of cases, one or more of these core symptoms have been shown to worsen over time.Reference Shaw and Sudre3 Despite these age-related differences, longitudinal twin studies have shown that ADHD symptoms in childhood, adolescence and early adulthood rely on partially overlapping genetic influences.Reference Chang, Lichtenstein, Asherson and Larsson4,Reference Kuntsi, Rijsdijk, Ronald, Asherson and Plomin5 However, studies of several longitudinal cohorts have reported that a majority of participants who met the diagnostic criteria for ADHD in adulthood did not meet the criteria in childhood, and vice versa.Reference Moffitt, Houts, Asherson, Belsky, Corcoran and Hammerle6Reference Caye, Rocha, Anselmi, Murray, Menezes and Barros8 These findings represented a challenge to the notion of ADHD as an aetiologically consistent condition across the lifespan, renewing longstanding doubts as to whether putative cases of ADHD in adults should be as readily diagnosed and treated as undiagnosed or later-onset presentations of the neurodevelopmental disorder that has long been studied and treated in children.Reference Castellanos9

Since the initial studies suggesting the distinctiveness of childhood and adult ADHD first surfaced, various studies have sought to shed light on the topic.Reference Faraone and Biederman10,Reference Sibley, Rohde, Swanson, Hechtman, Molina and Mitchell11 However, the evidence base on lifespan trajectories of ADHD remains quite limited. A recent review of longitudinal research by Asherson and Agnew-BlaisReference Asherson and Agnew-Blais12 found evidence for a late-onset group who met the diagnostic criteria for ADHD by early adulthood despite not having met the criteria in childhood.Reference Asherson and Agnew-Blais12 In many of these cases, symptoms occurred exclusively in the context of other disorders, broadly supporting suggestions that adult-onset ADHD symptoms could be better explained by other disorders or environmental factors, including substance use disorders.Reference Taylor, Kaplan-Kahn, Lighthall and Antshel13 However, refuting the notion of overall aetiological distinction, Asherson and Agnew-BlaisReference Asherson and Agnew-Blais12 noted that many late-onset cases displayed subthreshold symptoms of ADHD as children, or met the criteria for common comorbid disorders, particularly oppositional defiant disorder (ODD). They also suggested that most late-onset cases could more accurately be classified as ‘adolescent-onset’, with symptoms first emerging at ages 12–16 years. Among their conclusions, they posited that there may be variation in age at onset for ADHD similar to that seen in other neurodevelopmental disorders, such as schizophrenia. This would suggest that most putative cases of ADHD in adults actually represent continuations (or otherwise later-onset presentations) of the neurodevelopmental disorder of ADHD seen in childhood.

Addressing a gap in longitudinal data on ADHD symptoms across adulthood

Asherson and Agnew-BlaisReference Asherson and Agnew-Blais12 highlighted several important gaps in extant research, which preclude clear conclusions as to the overall continuity of ADHD symptoms from childhood to adulthood. Key among them was that most longitudinal childhood cohorts extend only to late adolescence or young adulthood, with a marked lack of data extending further into adulthood. Therefore, although it has been shown that most children with ADHD will still meet the diagnostic criteria by early adulthood, it remains unclear whether they will continue to do so across adulthood. Furthermore, the lack of genetically informative cohort data spanning this longer period prevents the direct assessment of aetiological overlap between ADHD symptoms in childhood and across adulthood. This gap in data is a key obstacle to drawing clear conclusions about whether ADHD in adulthood represents the same aetiological entity as childhood ADHD.

Recent genomic research has attempted to bridge this gap, providing some evidence for genetic overlap between ADHD in childhood and adulthood. Rovira et alReference Rovira, Demontis, Sánchez-Mora, Zayats, Klein and Mota14 reported a high correlation (rG = 0.81) between polygenic scores for ADHD ascertained in children (mean age 10.14 years, s.d. 3.24) and adults (mean age 33.46 years, s.d. 9.76). They also found significant correlations between child and adult ADHD polygenic scores and related phenotypes, including smoking, early pregnancy, academic performance and intelligence. An important caveat is that genomic findings continue to suffer from the missing heritability problem, explaining only a small portion of ADHD's total heritability compared with quantitative genetic estimates. For example, the genome-wide association study meta-analysis used by Rovira et alReference Rovira, Demontis, Sánchez-Mora, Zayats, Klein and Mota14 explained 17–19% of variance in ADHD symptoms in their child-only and combined child and adult samples, compared with estimates of 70–75% found in twin studies.Reference Faraone and Larsson15 Therefore, further research using methods capable of capturing all genetic variance in ADHD is also needed.

Very few studies currently exist that can directly address the above questions by following people from childhood to adulthood. One way to circumvent this problem is to use intergenerational family data to estimate genetic covariance between adult and child ADHD across generations. In the present article, we use this novel extended family approach to investigate the aetiological overlap between ADHD in childhood and adulthood, by assessing the extent to which adult ADHD symptoms in mothers are genetically related to symptoms of ADHD and several common comorbid disorders in their offspring. In doing so, we provide an indication of overall aetiological overlap between ADHD symptoms in adulthood and a broader pattern of ADHD-related symptoms in childhood. We do this using the Norwegian Mother, Father and Child Cohort Study (MoBa), a large data-set of related parents and their children.

Method

Sample

MoBa is a prospective, population-based birth registry cohort of 114 500 children, 95 200 birth mothers and 75 200 birth fathers in Norway.Reference Magnus, Birke, Vejrup, Haugan, Alsaker and Daltveit16 Data collection covered pregnancies across all of Norway from 1999 to 2008, with 40.6% of eligible pregnant women consenting to participate in the study. The current study is based on version 12 of the quality-assured data files released for research in January 2019. Using pedigree data from the Medical Birth Registry of Norway (MBRN), a national health registry containing information about all births in Norway, we used deterministic linkage via unique birth and parent numbers spanning several generations, to group MoBa participants into extended families of twins, siblings, half-siblings and cousins in both the parent and child generations. This analytic sample consisted of a total of 25 469 mothers who reported their own ADHD symptoms and/or at least one child measure for 30 833 of their children (51% male). Mothers were aged 17–45 years when children were born (mean age 29.97 years, s.d. 4.21). Supplementary Table 1 available at https://doi.org/10.1192/bjo.2023.554 displays frequencies of mothers and children stratified by maternal relatedness groups and child relatedness groups.

Ethics

The establishment of MoBa and initial data collection was based on a license from the Norwegian Data Protection Agency and approval from The Regional Committees for Medical and Health Research Ethics. The MoBa cohort is currently regulated by the Norwegian Health Registry Act. The current study was approved by The Regional Committees for Medical and Health Research Ethics (approval codes PDB981/PDB2330).

Measures

Figure 1 provides an overview of symptom measures used in the current study, wherein two ADHD measures (one of children's ADHD symptoms in early childhood and one of mothers’ ADHD symptoms in adulthood) were assessed for aetiological overlap with a set of five measures of children's ADHD and comorbid symptoms in mid-childhood. Internal consistency indices for all measures are displayed in Supplementary Table 2.

Fig. 1 Overview of child and parent symptom measures used in the current study, wherein children's ADHD symptoms in early childhood (left) and maternal ADHD symptoms in adulthood (right) were assessed for aetiological overlap with children's ADHD, ODD, conduct disorder, anxiety and depression symptoms in mid-childhood (centre). ADHD, attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder; ASRS, Adult Self-Report Scale; CPRS-R, Conners’ Parent Rating Scale-Revised Short Form; MCoTS, Multiple-Children-of-Twins-and-Siblings; ODD, oppositional defiant disorder; RS-DBD, Parent/Teacher Rating Scale for Disruptive Behavior Disorders; SCARED, Screen for Child Anxiety Related Emotional Disorders; sMFQ, Short Moods and Feelings Questionnaire.

Adult ADHD symptoms (reported by mothers when children were aged 3 years)

Mothers reported on their own ADHD symptoms by completing the Adult Self-Report Scale, a self-report screening tool for ADHD in adults which has high levels of concordance (area under the curve of 0.90) with clinical diagnoses.Reference Kessler, Adler, Gruber, Sarawate, Spencer and Van Brunt17 It includes six items measuring problems with sustained attention, task initiation and completion, organisation and age-appropriate indicators of hyperactivity (e.g. fidgeting when sitting still for prolonged periods). Mothers rated the frequency with which they experienced these problems in the past 6 months, on a five-point Likert scale of ‘never’, ‘rarely’, ‘sometimes’, ‘often’ and ‘very often’. Mothers were 20–48 years of age when reporting their ADHD symptoms (mean age of approximately 32.95 years, approximate s.d. 4.17).

Child ADHD symptoms at age 5 years

Mothers reported children's ADHD symptoms at age 5 years by completing the Conners’ Parent Rating Scale-Revised Short Form, a well-validated measure of parent-reported child ADHD symptoms, including 12 items covering a range of hyperactive, impulsive and inattentive symptoms in children.Reference Conners, Sitarenios, Parker and Epstein18 Mothers rated the extent to which each item described their child's behaviour in the past month, on a four-point Likert scale of ‘not true’, ‘somewhat true’, ‘quite true’ and ‘very true’.

Child ADHD and comorbid symptoms at age 8 years

Mothers reported children's ADHD, ODD and conduct disorder symptoms at age 8 years by completing the Parent/Teacher Rating Scale for Disruptive Behavior Disorders. This measure includes 18 items for ADHD symptoms, eight items for ODD symptoms and eight items for conduct disorder symptoms, based on DSM-IV criteria.Reference Silva, Alpert, Pouget, Silva, Trosper and Reyes19 Mothers rated the extent to which each item described their child's behaviour in the past year, on a four-point Likert scale of ‘never’, ‘rarely’, ‘sometimes’ and ‘often’. Mothers reported children's anxiety symptoms by completing the short version of the Screen for Child Anxiety Related Emotional Disorders, a measure covering symptoms of several DSM-IV anxiety disorders. The five-item short version has similar psychometric properties to the full 41-item scale.Reference Birmaher, Brent, Chiappetta, Bridge, Monga and Baugher20 Mothers rated the extent to which each item described their child's recent behaviour, on a three-point Likert scale of ‘not true’, ‘sometimes true’ and ‘very true’. Finally, mothers reported children's depression symptoms by completing the Short Moods and Feelings Questionnaire, a 13-item measure of recent feelings, thoughts and behaviours related to low mood and based on the DSM-III-TR criteria of depression.Reference Messer, Angold, Costello, Loeber, Van Kammen and Stouthamer-Loeber21 Mothers rated the extent to which each item described their child's behaviour in the past 2 weeks, on a three-point Likert scale of ‘not true’, ‘sometimes true’ and ‘true’.

Analyses

We set out to evaluate genetic overlap between adult (maternal) ADHD symptoms, and child (offspring) symptoms of ADHD and several comorbid disorders in mid-childhood. Mothers reported on their own ADHD symptoms several years before reporting on their children's symptoms of ADHD, ODD, conduct disorder, anxiety and depression (i.e. when children were aged 3 v. 8 years), reducing the risk of time-specific shared rater bias affecting our results. To contextualise our findings on adult ADHD, we also assessed associations between the above child symptoms at age 8 years and children's own earlier ADHD symptoms at age 5 years. Child ADHD symptoms at ages 5 and 8 years were assessed with distinct measures (see above).

Two sets of analyses were conducted, each using the OpenMx 2.18.1 packageReference Neale, Hunter, Pritikin, Zahery, Brick and Kirkpatrick22 in R Statistics version 4.0.3 for Windows (https://cran.r-project.org/bin/windows/base/old/4.0.3). First, five Multiple-Children-of-Twins-and-Siblings (MCoTS) models assessed genetic relationships between mothers’ adult ADHD symptoms and child ADHD, ODD, conduct disorder, anxiety and depression symptoms at age 8 years. Second, five extended bivariate twin models assessed genetic relationships between children's early ADHD symptoms at age 5 years and their later ADHD, ODD, conduct disorder, anxiety and depression symptoms at age 8 years. We adapted these bivariate twin models to account for the additional degrees of genetic relatedness between siblings (50%), half-siblings (25%) and cousins (12.5%), and to constrain the shared environmental effect in cousins and paternal half-siblings to zero (as most cousins and paternal half-siblings do not share a household).

MCoTS analyses are an extension of the Children-of-Twins (CoT) design, a quasi-experimental method of determining the extent to which a parent–offspring association is attributable to shared genetic influences.Reference McAdams, Hannigan, Eilertsen, Gjerde, Ystrom and Rijsdijk23 Where bivariate twin models can estimate genetic influences on phenotypic covariance between twins, CoT models can estimate genetic influences on phenotypic covariance between parents and children. Using extended families of twin parents and children, these models derive their power from the comparison of parent–offspring correlations and avuncular correlations (those between children and their aunt/uncle). Namely, children of identical twins share the same proportion of genes with their parent as they do with their aunt or uncle, but share a rearing environment only with their own parent. This separation of genetic and environmental influences allows for inferences as to the extent to which covariance between parents and their offspring is explained by shared genetic influences, with any excess parent–offspring similarity suggesting direct phenotypic transmission through exposure to the parent phenotype. The MCoTS design extends CoT analyses to include sibling, half-sibling and cousin parents, and multiple children per parent. This allows for larger data-sets of extended families to be used, increasing statistical power. In previous work, we have demonstrated that MCoTS models are adequately statistically powered in the MoBa sample.Reference McAdams, Hannigan, Eilertsen, Gjerde, Ystrom and Rijsdijk23 The full MCoTS model specification is shown in Supplementary Fig. 3, and the extended bivariate twin model specification is shown in Supplementary Fig. 4.

To control for covariates, we regressed all symptom measures on maternal age, parity (mothers’ number of previous births) and children's year of birth, and all child symptom measures on child gender. The residual measures were then used in analyses.

Results

Descriptive statistics of all measures are shown in Table 1. Model fit comparisons and standardised parameter estimates from preliminary univariate models testing the significance of genetic and shared environmental effects on adult ADHD symptoms are shown in Supplementary Tables 5a and 5b. Model fit comparisons from MCoTS and extended bivariate twin models, testing genetic effects on covariance between adult and early child ADHD symptoms and each child symptom measure at age 8 years, are shown in Supplementary Tables 6 and 7.

Table 1 Descriptive statistics of all raw measures (all mother-reported)

ADHD, attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder; ASRS, Adult Self-Report Scale; CPRS-R, Conners’ Parent Rating Scale-Revised Short Form; RS-DBD, Parent/Teacher Rating Scale for Disruptive Behavior Disorders; ODD, oppositional defiant disorder; SCARED, Screen for Child Anxiety Related Emotional Disorders; sMFQ, Short Moods and Feelings Questionnaire.

a. Includes 6435 mothers who provided self-ratings of ADHD symptoms, but did not provide ratings of child symptom ratings at the waves studied (these extra maternal ADHD data were included to improve heritability estimates of adult ADHD symptoms). All other descriptors include only those mothers who provided at least one child symptom rating.

Results from adult ADHD (MCoTS) and child ADHD (extended bivariate twin) models are shown in Figs 2 and 3. Phenotypic correlations followed an expected pattern, with child–child correlations being larger than intergenerational adult–child correlations. Genetic influences on adult ADHD symptoms were correlated with genetic influences on children's depression (rG = 1), ODD (rG = 0.80), anxiety (rG = 0.72), ADHD (rG = 0.55) and conduct disorder (rG = 0.44) symptoms at age 8 years. Similarly, genetic influences on child ADHD symptoms at age 5 years were correlated with genetic influences on child ADHD (rG = 0.84), ODD (rG = 0.70), depression (rG = 0.64), conduct disorder (rG = 0.43) and anxiety (rG = 0.41) symptoms at age 8 years. Standardised parameter estimates of all phenotypic and genetic correlations, including 95% confidence intervals, are shown in Supplementary Table 8.

Fig. 2 Bar plots displaying estimates (95% CI) of phenotypic correlations between each child symptom measure at age 8 years (on x-axis) and early child ADHD (blue bars) and adult ADHD (red bars). ADHD, attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder; ODD, oppositional defiant disorder.

Fig. 3 Bar plots displaying estimates (95% CI) of genetic correlations between each child symptom measure at age 8 years (on x-axis) and early child ADHD (blue bars) and adult ADHD (red bars). ADHD, attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder; ODD, oppositional defiant disorder.

Children's ADHD and comorbid symptoms at age 8 years shared roughly equal genetic overlap with adult ADHD symptoms and child ADHD symptoms at age 5 years (Fig. 3). Notably, for child ODD, conduct disorder, anxiety and depression symptoms, point estimates of genetic correlations with adult ADHD symptoms were higher than those of correlations with child ADHD symptoms at age 5 years. However, confidence intervals were markedly wider for adult ADHD estimates (with some upper bounds reaching 1), likely because of the lower power to estimate intergenerational genetic correlations in MCoTS models compared with twin genetic correlations in bivariate twin models.Reference McAdams, Hannigan, Eilertsen, Gjerde, Ystrom and Rijsdijk23 Importantly, none of the lower bounds of genetic correlations were close to zero, suggesting confidence in the genetic associations observed. Overall, confidence intervals of all adult ADHD genetic correlations were overlapping with their equivalent child ADHD correlations.

Discussion

We set out to address a gap in research regarding the aetiological overlap between ADHD symptoms in childhood and adulthood. We did this by assessing the extent to which mothers’ adult ADHD symptoms and children's own early ADHD symptoms at age 5 years were genetically correlated with children's mid-childhood symptoms of ADHD and several common comorbid disorders at age 8 years. At the phenotypic level, children's own early ADHD symptoms were, unsurprisingly, more highly correlated with their later ADHD and comorbid symptoms by mid-childhood, as compared with mothers’ adult ADHD symptoms. However, genetic influences on both adult ADHD symptoms and early child ADHD symptoms shared similarly high correlations with genetic influences on children's ADHD and comorbid symptoms in mid-childhood.

Our findings provide novel evidence for a shared genetic architecture between ADHD symptoms in childhood and adulthood, insofar as the genetic influences on ADHD symptoms in mothers were highly correlated with genetic influences on ADHD symptoms in their children at age 8 years. Although past twin research has provided evidence for overlap between ADHD symptoms in childhood and late adolescence to early adulthood, no research to date has directly assessed genetic overlap between ADHD symptoms in childhood and across a broader age range in adulthood (i.e. beyond young adulthood).

More broadly, although there has been research on genetic overlap between ADHD and comorbid disorders in childhood,Reference Tuvblad, Zheng, Raine and Baker24,Reference Michelini, Eley, Gregory and McAdams25 we are not aware of any research assessing the extent to which ADHD symptoms in adults share genetic overlap with children's symptoms of comorbid disorders. Our results demonstrate a very similar pattern of genetic overlap between ADHD symptoms measured at a range of ages in adulthood, and symptoms of ADHD and several common comorbid disorders in mid-childhood. This suggests that genetic influences on ADHD symptoms in adulthood substantially overlap with those on symptoms of ADHD and comorbid disorders in mid-childhood. In other words, it is unlikely that adult ADHD symptoms in our sample represent a distinct aetiological entity from child ADHD symptoms. Our results do not directly address nor rule out the possibility of a late-onset form of ADHD. However, if an aetiologically distinct later-onset form of ADHD exists in some adults, our findings suggest that it is quite rare in our sample, since it did not drastically affect our estimates of genetic overlap with children's ADHD and comorbid symptoms.

It is also notable that in a large sample of mothers and children not selected for clinically significant ADHD (most of whom would not meet diagnostic cut-offs), genetically driven correlations were found between mothers’ adult ADHD symptoms and children's ADHD and comorbid symptoms. This suggests that even if their symptoms are at subclinical levels, mothers transmit to their children genetic risk factors for the wider array of symptoms that co-occur with ADHD in clinically significant cases. In other words, mothers with, for example, mildly elevated hyperactivity or inattention may pass on genetic risk for mildly elevated impulsivity, oppositionality or anxiety in children. This is in line with growing evidence that psychiatric and neurodevelopmental disorders represent the extreme ends of normally distributed traits in the population, and co-occur as a rule, rather than being discrete and non-overlapping categories of severe disorder only present among those with specific risk factors.Reference Kotov, Krueger and Watson26

Although our study focused on ADHD and common comorbidities, future studies could apply our approach more broadly to investigate aetiological overlap between any pair of adult and child phenotypes, wherever cohorts of large extended families can be linked by twin, sibling and cousin relationships. An advantage of this method is that it controls for intervention effects at the within-person level, where relevant. For example, in conventional bivariate twin studies, if aetiological overlap were observed between children's ADHD symptoms and their later cardiovascular outcomes, this could, in part, be explained by the effects of ADHD medications on cardiovascular health. However, since most parents in established longitudinal cohorts to date have not received pharmacological treatment for ADHD, if our methodology were applied to investigate aetiological overlap between children's ADHD symptoms and their parents’ adult cardiovascular health outcomes, any overlap observed could not be explained by the potential effects of ADHD medications on cardiovascular health.

We investigated our hypotheses by using a large representative national birth registry cohort, including longitudinal child and parent data at a range of ages. Linking this data-set by extended family relationships with birth registry data allowed us to estimate genetic correlations between adult and child traits. This novel approach enabled us to bridge the gap in genetically sensitive data on ADHD and comorbid symptoms extending from childhood into adulthood. Another key strength of the study was that mothers reported their ADHD symptoms well into adulthood, with the youngest aged 20 years and the oldest aged 48 years at the time of self-report.

One limitation of our study was a lack of available data on ADHD symptoms in fathers, which could differ in their aetiological overlap with children's ADHD and comorbid symptoms. However, despite well-known gender differences in the prevalence and presentation of ADHD symptoms, genetic and environmental influences on ADHD have been shown not to differ significantly by gender.Reference Larsson, Chang, D'Onofrio and Lichtenstein27 Nonetheless, future studies incorporating father-rated adult ADHD symptoms would be useful. Another potential limitation of our analyses was that mothers reported both their own and children's symptoms. Although maternal ratings of child symptoms are likely valid (as mothers typically spend the most time with their children), shared method bias could cause mothers’ child and self-ratings to be excessively correlated because of their own attitudes or traits.Reference Deault28 However, this is unlikely to have inflated our estimates of genetic overlap, as these rely on avuncular correlations, i.e. between an aunt's self-rated ADHD symptoms and their niece or nephew's mother-rated symptoms. Additionally, mothers reported their own ADHD symptoms and children's earlier and later symptoms several years apart (when children were aged 3, 5 and 8 years, respectively), reducing the likelihood of time-specific reporting biases. Systematic and/or heritable biases could still inflate estimates of genetic overlap; for instance, if both mothers in an extended family had higher ADHD symptoms, so tended to be less attentive to children's symptoms, and therefore consistently underreported these. However, several studies have reported a lack of evidence that maternal ADHD symptoms bias mothers’ reporting of their children's ADHD symptoms.Reference Faraone, Monuteaux, Biederman, Cohan and Mick29,Reference Jassy30

In conclusion, our analyses address a lack of genetically sensitive research assessing the aetiological overlap between ADHD symptoms in childhood and throughout adulthood, as well as broader overlap with childhood comorbid symptoms. Our findings add to the evidence base for the continuity of childhood ADHD symptom into and across adulthood, by demonstrating a genetically driven co-occurrence of adult ADHD symptoms in mothers and a typical pattern of ADHD-related symptom in their children. Although these findings do not rule out the possibility of an aetiologically distinct form of adult ADHD in a minority of cases, they do provide genetically sensitive support for a common genetic architecture underpinning ADHD symptoms in a large, representative sample of children and adults.

Future studies could apply our approach to investigate aetiological overlap between a wider range of ADHD-related phenotypes in childhood and adulthood, or indeed, the overlap between any pair of adult and child phenotypes, where population registry data enable parents and children to be linked by extended family relationships into twin, sibling and cousin pairs. This constitutes a useful method of bridging the gap in genetically sensitive data for phenotypes when measures are available in childhood, but do not extend into adulthood.

Supplementary material

Supplementary material is available online at https://doi.org/10.1192/bjo.2023.554

Data availability

No new data were collected or analysed as part of the current study. Although access to MoBa and MBRN data is restricted, the MoBa data-set and its relevant overviews and data dictionaries are available at https://www.fhi.no/en/studies/moba/.

Acknowledgements

We are grateful to all of the participating families in Norway who take part in this ongoing cohort study.

Author contributions

The corresponding author (D.L.W.) had full access to all the data in the study and had final responsibility for the decision to submit for publication. D.L.W. conceived and designed the investigation, with support from T.A.M. E.M.E., T.A.M., Y.I.A., I.B. and D.L.W. contributed to data preparation. D.L.W. carried out statistical analyses, with support from T.A.M. and F.V.R. D.L.W. wrote and revised the manuscript; all co-authors critically reviewed the manuscript. For the purposes of open access, the author has applied a Creative Commons Attribution (CC-BY) licence to any accepted author manuscript version arising from this submission.

Funding

D.L.W. was supported by the UK Medical Research Council (grant number MR/N013700/1) and King's College London MRC Doctoral Training Partnership in Biomedical Sciences. The positions of T.A.M. and Y.I.A. were funded by a Sir Henry Dale Fellowship awarded to T.A.M., jointly funded by the Wellcome Trust and the Royal Society (grant number 107706/Z/15/Z), and by a Senior Research Fellowship awarded to T.A.M. and funded by the Wellcome Trust (grant number 220382/Z/20/Z). The Research Council of Norway supported E.Y. and T.A.M. (grant number 288083), and E.M.E. (grant number 262177). I.B. was supported by the UK Economic and Social Research Council (ESRC) and King's College London ESRC Doctoral Training Partnership in Interdisciplinary Social Science (grant number ST11872). L.J.H. was supported by a grant from the South-Eastern Norway Regional Health Authority (grant number 2018059). The Norwegian Mother, Father and Child Cohort Study data collection was supported by the Norwegian Ministry of Health and Care Services and the Ministry of Education and Research, National Institutes of Health/National Institute of Environmental Health Sciences (NIH/NIEHS) (contract N01-ES-75558) and National Institutes of Health/National Institute of Neurological Disorders and Stroke (NIH/NINDS) (grant numbers 1 UO1 NS 047537-01 and 2 UO1 NS 047537-06A1). The funders of the study had no role in study design, data collection, data analysis, data interpretation or writing of the report.

Declaration of interest

None.

References

Kooij, JJS, Bijlenga, D, Salerno, L, Jaeschke, R, Bitter, I, Balazs, J, et al. Updated European Consensus Statement on diagnosis and treatment of adult ADHD. Eur Psychiatry 2019; 56: 1434.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Willcutt, EG. The prevalence of DSM-IV attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder: a meta-analytic review. Neurotherapeutics 2012; 9(3): 490–9.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Shaw, P, Sudre, G. Adolescent attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder: understanding teenage symptom trajectories. Biol Psychiatry 2021; 89(2): 152–61.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Chang, Z, Lichtenstein, P, Asherson, PJ, Larsson, H. Developmental twin study of attention problems: high heritabilities throughout development. JAMA Psychiatry 2013; 70(3): 311–8.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kuntsi, J, Rijsdijk, F, Ronald, A, Asherson, P, Plomin, R. Genetic influences on the stability of attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder symptoms from early to middle childhood. Biol Psychiatry 2005; 57(6): 647–54.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Moffitt, TE, Houts, R, Asherson, P, Belsky, DW, Corcoran, DL, Hammerle, M, et al. Is adult ADHD a childhood-onset neurodevelopmental disorder? Evidence from a four-decade longitudinal cohort study. Am J Psychiatry 2015; 172(10): 967–77.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Agnew-Blais, JC, Polanczyk, G, Danese, A, Wertz, J, Moffitt, TE, Arseneault, L. Persistence, remission and emergence of ADHD in young adulthood: results from a longitudinal, prospective population-based cohort. JAMA Psychiatry 2016; 73(7): 713.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Caye, A, Rocha, TB-M, Anselmi, L, Murray, J, Menezes, AM, Barros, FC, et al. Attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder trajectories from childhood to young adulthood: evidence from a birth cohort supporting a late-onset syndrome. JAMA Psychiatry 2016; 73(7): 705–12.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Castellanos, FX. Is adult-onset ADHD a distinct entity? Am J Psychiatry 2015; 172(10): 929–31.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Faraone, SV, Biederman, J. Can attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder onset occur in adulthood? JAMA Psychiatry 2016; 73(7): 655–6.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Sibley, MH, Rohde, LA, Swanson, JM, Hechtman, LT, Molina, BS, Mitchell, JT, et al. Late-onset ADHD reconsidered with comprehensive repeated assessments between ages 10 and 25. Am J Psychiatry 2018; 175(2): 140–9.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Asherson, P, Agnew-Blais, J. Annual research review: does late-onset attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder exist? J Child Psychol Psychiatry 2019; 60(4): 333–52.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Taylor, LE, Kaplan-Kahn, EA, Lighthall, RA, Antshel, KM. Adult-onset ADHD: a critical analysis and alternative explanations. Child Psychiatry Hum Dev 2022; 53(4): 635–53.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rovira, P, Demontis, D, Sánchez-Mora, C, Zayats, T, Klein, M, Mota, NR, et al. Shared genetic background between children and adults with attention deficit/hyperactivity disorder. Neuropsychopharmacology 2020; 45(10): 1617–26.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Faraone, SV, Larsson, H. Genetics of attention deficit hyperactivity disorder. Mol Psychiatry 2019; 24(4): 562.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Magnus, P, Birke, C, Vejrup, K, Haugan, A, Alsaker, E, Daltveit, AK, et al. Cohort profile update: the Norwegian Mother and Child Cohort Study (MoBa). Int J Epidemiol 2016; 45(2): 382–8.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kessler, RC, Adler, LA, Gruber, MJ, Sarawate, CA, Spencer, T, Van Brunt, DL. Validity of the World Health Organization Adult ADHD Self-Report Scale (ASRS) screener in a representative sample of health plan members. Int J Methods Psychiatr Res 2007; 16(2): 5265.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Conners, CK, Sitarenios, G, Parker, JD, Epstein, JN. The revised Conners’ Parent Rating Scale (CPRS-R): factor structure, reliability, and criterion validity. J Abnorm Child Psychol 1998; 26(4): 257–68.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Silva, RR, Alpert, M, Pouget, E, Silva, V, Trosper, S, Reyes, K, et al. A rating scale for disruptive behavior disorders, based on the DSM-IV item pool. Psychiatr Q 2005; 76(4): 327–39.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Birmaher, B, Brent, DA, Chiappetta, L, Bridge, J, Monga, S, Baugher, M. Psychometric properties of the Screen for Child Anxiety Related Emotional Disorders (SCARED): a replication study. J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry 1999; 38(10): 1230–6.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Messer, SC, Angold, A, Costello, EJ, Loeber, R, Van Kammen, W, Stouthamer-Loeber, M. Development of a short questionnaire for use in epidemiological studies of depression in children and adolescents: factor composition and structure across development. Int J Methods Psychiatr Res 1995; 5: 251–62.Google Scholar
Neale, MC, Hunter, MD, Pritikin, JN, Zahery, M, Brick, TR, Kirkpatrick, RM, et al. OpenMx 2.0: extended structural equation and statistical modeling. Psychometrika 2016; 81(2): 535–49.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
McAdams, TA, Hannigan, LJ, Eilertsen, EM, Gjerde, LC, Ystrom, E, Rijsdijk, FV. Revisiting the children-of-twins design: improving existing models for the exploration of intergenerational associations. Behav Genet 2018; 48(5): 397412.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Tuvblad, C, Zheng, M, Raine, A, Baker, LA. A common genetic factor explains the covariation among ADHD ODD and CD symptoms in 9–10 year old boys and girls. J Abnorm Child Psychol 2009; 37(2): 153–67.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Michelini, G, Eley, TC, Gregory, AM, McAdams, TA. Aetiological overlap between anxiety and attention deficit hyperactivity symptom dimensions in adolescence. J Child Psychol Psychiatry 2015; 56(4): 423–31.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kotov, R, Krueger, RF, Watson, D. A paradigm shift in psychiatric classification: the hierarchical taxonomy of psychopathology (HiTOP). World Psychiatry 2018; 17(1): 24.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Larsson, H, Chang, Z, D'Onofrio, BM, Lichtenstein, P. The heritability of clinically diagnosed attention deficit hyperactivity disorder across the lifespan. Psychol Med 2014; 44(10): 2223–9.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Deault, LC. A systematic review of parenting in relation to the development of comorbidities and functional impairments in children with attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder (ADHD). Child Psychiatry Hum Dev 2010; 41(2): 168–92.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Faraone, SV, Monuteaux, MC, Biederman, J, Cohan, SL, Mick, E. Does parental ADHD bias maternal reports of ADHD symptoms in children? J Consult Clin Psychol 2003; 71(1): 168.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Jassy, JS. Maternal ADHD symptoms and maternal ratings of child ADHD symptoms: are more inattentive mothers less accurate? Master's thesis Department of Psychology, University of British Columbia, 2009.Google Scholar
Figure 0

Fig. 1 Overview of child and parent symptom measures used in the current study, wherein children's ADHD symptoms in early childhood (left) and maternal ADHD symptoms in adulthood (right) were assessed for aetiological overlap with children's ADHD, ODD, conduct disorder, anxiety and depression symptoms in mid-childhood (centre). ADHD, attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder; ASRS, Adult Self-Report Scale; CPRS-R, Conners’ Parent Rating Scale-Revised Short Form; MCoTS, Multiple-Children-of-Twins-and-Siblings; ODD, oppositional defiant disorder; RS-DBD, Parent/Teacher Rating Scale for Disruptive Behavior Disorders; SCARED, Screen for Child Anxiety Related Emotional Disorders; sMFQ, Short Moods and Feelings Questionnaire.

Figure 1

Table 1 Descriptive statistics of all raw measures (all mother-reported)

Figure 2

Fig. 2 Bar plots displaying estimates (95% CI) of phenotypic correlations between each child symptom measure at age 8 years (on x-axis) and early child ADHD (blue bars) and adult ADHD (red bars). ADHD, attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder; ODD, oppositional defiant disorder.

Figure 3

Fig. 3 Bar plots displaying estimates (95% CI) of genetic correlations between each child symptom measure at age 8 years (on x-axis) and early child ADHD (blue bars) and adult ADHD (red bars). ADHD, attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder; ODD, oppositional defiant disorder.

Supplementary material: File

S2056472423005549sup001.docx

Wechsler et al. supplementary material

Download S2056472423005549sup001.docx(File)
File 462.1 KB
Supplementary material: File

Wechsler_et_al._Dataset

Dataset

Download Wechsler_et_al._Dataset(File)
File
Submit a response

eLetters

No eLetters have been published for this article.