Hostname: page-component-586b7cd67f-2brh9 Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-11-25T15:01:57.977Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

The relationship between language control and cognitive control in bilingual aphasia*

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  20 May 2015

TERESA GRAY*
Affiliation:
Department of Speech and Hearing Sciences, Boston University
SWATHI KIRAN
Affiliation:
Department of Speech and Hearing Sciences, Boston University
*
Address for correspondence: Teresa Gray Speech Language and Hearing Sciences Boston University Sargent College 635 Commonwealth Ave. Boston, MA 02215[email protected]

Abstract

This study examines language control deficits in bilingual aphasia in terms of domain specific cognitive control and domain general cognitive control. Thirty Spanish–English controls and ten Spanish–English adults with aphasia completed the flanker task and a word-pair relatedness judgment task. All participants exhibited congruency effects on the flanker task. On the linguistic task, controls did not show the congruency effect on the first level analysis. However, conflict ratios revealed that the control group exhibited significant effects of language control. Additionally, individual patient analysis revealed overall positive and negative effects of language control impairment and a benefit from semantically related word-pairs. Patient data suggest a dissociation between the mechanisms of language control and cognitive control, thus providing evidence for domain specific cognitive control. The influence of language proficiency on speed of translation was also examined. Generally, controls were faster when translating into their dominant language, whereas the patients did not show the same trends.

Type
Research Article
Copyright
Copyright © Cambridge University Press 2015 

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

Footnotes

*

We would like to thank all participants for their time and effort to complete the study. We also thank the members of the Aphasia Research Laboratory for their intellectual input.

References

Abutalebi, J., & Green, D. W. (2008). Control mechanisms in bilingual language production: Neural evidence from language switching studies. Language and Cognitive Processes, 23 (4), 557582.Google Scholar
Abutalebi, J., Miozzo, A., & Cappa, S.F. (2000). Do Subcortical Structures Control ‘Language Selection’ in Polyglots? Evidence from Pathological Language Mixing. Neurocase, 6, 5156.Google Scholar
Adrover-Roig, D., Galparsoro-Izagirre, N., Marcotte, K., Ferré, P., Wilson, M. A., & Ansaldo, A. I. (2011). Impaired L1 and executive control after left basal ganglia damage in a bilingual Basque-Spanish person with aphasia. Clinical Linguistics & Phonetics, 25 (6–7), 480498.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ansaldo, A. I., & Marcotte, K. (2007). Language switching and mixing in the context of bilingual aphasia. In Centeno, J. G., Obler, L. K., & Anderson, R. T. (Eds.), Studying communication disorders in Spanish speakers: Theoretical, research, and clinical aspects (pp. 1221). Clevedon, UK: Multilingual Matters.Google Scholar
Ansaldo, A. I., Saidi, L. G., & Ruiz, A. (2010). Model-driven intervention in bilingual aphasia: Evidence from a case of pathological language mixing. Aphasiology, 24 (2), 309324.Google Scholar
Basnight-Brown, D. M., & Altarriba, J. (2007). Differences in semantic and translation priming across languages: The role of language direction and language dominance. Memory & Cognition, 35 (5), 953965.Google Scholar
Bialystok, E. (2010). Global-local and trail-making tasks by monolingual and bilingual children: Beyond inhibition. Developmental Psychology, 46 (1), 93.Google Scholar
Bialystok, E. (2011). Reshaping the mind: The benefits of bilingualism. Canadian Journal of Experimental Psychology, 65 (4), 229235.Google Scholar
Bialystok, E., Craik, F. I., Green, D. W., & Gollan, T. H. (2009). Bilingual minds. Psychological Science in the Public Interest, 10 (3), 89129.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bialystok, E, Craik, F. I., Klein, R., Viswanathan, M. (2004). Bilingualism, aging, and cognitive control: Evidence from the Simon task. Psychology and Aging, 19, 290303.Google Scholar
Bialystok, E., Craik, F. I., & Luk, G. (2008). Cognitive control and lexical access in younger and older bilinguals. Journal of experimental psychology. Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 34 (4), 859873.Google Scholar
Calabria, M., Branzi, F., Marne, P., Hernández, M., & Costa, A. (2013). Age-related effects over bilingual Language control and executive control. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 114. doi: 10.1017/S1366728913000138.Google Scholar
Calabria, M., Hernández, M., Branzi, F. M., & Costa, A. (2011). Qualitative differences between bilingual language control and executive control: evidence from task-switching. Frontiers in Psychology, 2, 110.Google Scholar
Chee, M. W., Hon, N., Lee, H. L., & Soon, C. S. (2001). Relative language proficiency modulates BOLD signal change when bilinguals perform semantic judgments. Neuroimage, 13 (6), 11551163.Google Scholar
Chen, H. C., & Ng, M. L. (1989). Semantic facilitation and translation priming effects in Chinese-English bilinguals. Memory & Cognition, 17 (4), 454462.Google Scholar
Collins, A. M., & Loftus, E. F. (1975). A spreading-activation theory of semantic processing. Psychological Review, 82 (6), 407428.Google Scholar
Colomé, A. (2001). Lexical activation in bilinguals’ speech production: Language-specific or language-independent? Journal of Memory and Language, 45 (4), 721736.Google Scholar
Costa, A. (2005). Lexical access in bilingual production. In Kroll, J. F. & De Groot, A. M. (Eds.), Handbook of bilingualism: Psycholinguistic approaches (pp.308325). New York: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Costa, A., & Caramazza, A. (1999). Is lexical selection in bilingual speech production language-specific? Further evidence from Spanish–English and English–Spanish bilinguals. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 2 (3), 231244.Google Scholar
Costa, A., Hernández, M., Costa-Faidella, J., & Sebastián-Gallés, N. (2009). On the bilingual advantage in conflict processing: now you see it, now you don’t. Cognition-International Journal of Cognitive Science, 113 (2), 135149.Google Scholar
Costa, A., Hernández, M., & Sebastián-Gallés, N. (2008). Bilingualism aids conflict resolution: Evidence from the ANT task. Cognition, 106 (1), 5986.Google Scholar
Costa, A., & Santesteban, M. (2004). Lexical access in bilingual speech production: Evidence from language switching in highly proficient bilinguals and L2 learners. Journal of Memory and Language, 50, 491511.Google Scholar
Costa, A., Santesteban, M., & Ivanova, I. (2006). How do highly proficient bilinguals control their lexicalization process? Inhibitory and language-specific selection mechanisms are both functional. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 32 (5), 10571074.Google ScholarPubMed
Dash, T., & Kar, B. R. (2014). Bilingual language control and general purpose cognitive control among individuals with bilingual aphasia: Evidence based on negative priming and flanker tasks. Behavioural Neurology, 2014, 120.Google Scholar
De Bot, K. (1992). A bilingual production model: Levelt's speaking model adapted. Applied Linguistics, 13, 124.Google Scholar
Dijkstra, T., & Van Heuven, W. J. B. (2002). The architecture of the bilingual word recognition system: From identification to decision. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 5 (3), 175197.Google Scholar
Edmonds, L. A., & Kiran, S. (2006). Effect of semantic naming treatment on crosslinguistic generalization in bilingual aphasia. Journal of Speech, Language and Hearing Research, 49 (4), 729748.Google Scholar
Eriksen, B. A., & Eriksen, C. W. (1974). Effects of noise letters upon the identification of a target letter in a nonsearch task. Perception and Psychophysics, 16, 143149.Google Scholar
García-Caballero, A., García-Lado, I., González-Hermida, J., Area, R., Recimil, M. J., Rabadán, O. J., Lamas, S., Ozaita, G., & Jorge, F. J. (2007). Paradoxical recovery in a bilingual patient with aphasia after right capsuloputaminal infarction. Journal of Neurology, Neurosurgery & Psychiatry, 78 (1), 8991.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Grainger, J., & Frenck-Mestre, C. (1998). Masked priming by translation equivalents in proficient bilinguals. Language and Cognitive Processes, 13 (6), 601623.Google Scholar
Gray, T., & Kiran, S. (2013). A theoretical account of lexical and semantic naming deficits in bilingual aphasia. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 56, 13141327.Google Scholar
Green, D. W. (1998). Mental control of the bilingual lexico-semantic system. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 1 (2), 6781.Google Scholar
Green, D. W., Grogan, A., Crinion, J., Ali, N., Sutton, C., & Price, C. J. (2010). Language control and parallel recovery of language in individuals with aphasia. Aphasiology, 24 (2), 188209.Google Scholar
Green, D. W., Ruffle, L., Grogan, A., Ali, N., Ramsden, S., Schofield, T., Crinon, J., & Price, C. J. (2011). Parallel recovery in a trilingual speaker: the use of the Bilingual Aphasia Test as a diagnostic complement to the Comprehensive Aphasia Test. Clinical Linguistics & Phonetics, 25 (6–7), 499512.Google Scholar
Helm-Estabrooks, N. (2001). Cognitive Linguistic Quick Test. San Antonio: Pearson.Google Scholar
Hermans, D., Bongaerts, T., De Bot, K., & Schreuder, R. (1998). Producing words in a foreign language: Can speakers prevent interference from their first language? Bilingualism: Language and Cognition 1 (3), 213–29.Google Scholar
Hernandez, A. E., Dapretto, M., Mazziotta, J., & Bookheimer, S. (2001). Language switching and language representation in Spanish–English bilinguals: An fMRI study. NeuroImage, 14 (2), 510520.Google Scholar
Howard, D., & Patterson, K. (1992). Pyramids and Palm Trees Test. San Antonio: Pearson.Google Scholar
Kaplan, E., Goodglass, H., & Weinraub, S. (2001). The Boston Naming Test. 2nd edition. Baltimore: Lippincott, Williams and Wilkins.Google Scholar
Kaushanskaya, M., & Marian, V. (2007). Bilingual language processing and interference in bilinguals: Evidence from eye tracking and picture naming. Language Learning, 57 (1), 119163.Google Scholar
Keane, C., & Kiran, S., (under revision). Cross-linguistic generalization and interference in trilingual aphasia: A case study. Special issue of Cognitive Neuropsychology.Google Scholar
Kiran, S., Balachandran, I., & Lucas, J (2014). The nature of lexical access in bilingual aphasia. Behavioral Neurology, 118. http://dx.doi.org/10.1155/2014/389565.Google ScholarPubMed
Kiran, S., Peña, E., Bedore, L., & Sheng, L. (2010). Evaluating the relationship between category generation and language use and proficiency. Paper presented at the Donostia Workshop on Neurobilingualism, San Sebastian, Spain.Google Scholar
Kiran, S., & Roberts, P. M. (2010). Semantic feature analysis treatment in Spanish–English and French–English bilingual aphasia. Aphasiology, 24 (2), 231261.Google Scholar
Kiran, S., Sandberg, C., Gray, T., Ascenso, E., & Kester, E. (2013). Rehabilitation in Bilingual Aphasia: Evidence for Within- and Between-Language Generalization. American Journal of Speech-Language Pathology, 22 (2), S298S326.Google Scholar
Kroll, J. F., Bobb, S. C., Misra, M., & Guo, T. (2008). Language selection in bilingual speech: Evidence for inhibitory processes. Acta psychologica, 128 (3), 416430.Google Scholar
Kroll, J. F., Bobb, S. C., & Wodniecka, Z. (2006). Language selectivity is the exception, not the rule: Arguments against a fixed locus of language selection in bilingual speech. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 9 (2), 119135.Google Scholar
Kroll, S. & Stewart, E. (1994). Category interference in translation and picture naming: Evidence for asymmetric connections between bilingual memory representations. Journal of Memory and Language, 33, 149174.Google Scholar
Luk, G., Anderson, J., Craik, F., Bialystok, E., & Grady, C. (2010). Distinct neural correlates for two types of inhibition in bilinguals: Response inhibition versus interference suppression. Brain and Cognition, 74, 347357.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Magezi, D. A., Khateb, A., Mouthon, M., Spierer, L., & Annoni, J. M. (2012). Cognitive control of language production in bilinguals involves a partly independent process within the domain-general cognitive control network: Evidence from task-switching and electrical brain activity. Brain and Language, 122 (1), 5563.Google Scholar
Meuter, R. & Allport, A. (1999). Bilingual language switching in naming: asymmetrical costs of language selection. Journal of Memory and Language, 40, 2540.Google Scholar
Paradis, M. (1989). The Bilingual Aphasia Test. Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates (out of print). Online at http://www.mcgill.ca/linguistics/research/bat/.Google Scholar
Prior, A., & Gollan, T. H. (2011). Good language-switchers are good task-switchers: evidence from Spanish–English and Mandarin–English bilinguals. Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society: JINS, 17 (4), 682–91.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Soveri, A., Rodriguez-Fornells, A., & Laine, M. (2011). Is there a relationship between language switching and executive functions in bilingualism? Introducing a within group analysis approach. Frontiers in Psychology, 2, 183 110.Google Scholar
Tzelgov, J., Henik, A., & Leiser, D. (1990). Controlling Stroop interference: Evidence from a bilingual task. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 16 (5), 760771.Google Scholar
Verreyt, N., De Letter, M., Hemelsoet, D., Santens, P., & Duyck, W. (2013). Cognate effects and executive control in a patient with differential bilingual aphasia. Applied Neuropsychology: Adult, 20 (3), 221230.Google Scholar
Weissberger, G. H., Wierenga, C. E., Bondi, M. W., & Gollan, T. H. (2012). Partially overlapping mechanisms of language and task control in young and older bilinguals. Psychology and Aging, 27 (4), 959.Google Scholar
Zhao, X., & Li, P. (2013). Simulating cross-language priming with a dynamic computational model of the lexicon. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 16 (02), 288303.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Zied, K.M., Phillipe, A., Pinon, K., Havet-Thomassin, V., Aubin, G., Roy, A., & Le Gall, D. (2004). Bilingualism and adult differences in inhibitory mechanisms: Evidence from a bilingual Stroop task. Brain and Cognition, 54 (3), 254256.Google Scholar