Hostname: page-component-cd9895bd7-hc48f Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-12-28T06:36:33.870Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Minimal-pair word learning by bilingual toddlers: the Catalan /e/-/ɛ/ contrast revisited*

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  18 November 2016

MARTA RAMON-CASAS
Affiliation:
Department of Cognition, Development and Education Psychology, University of Barcelona
CHRISTOPHER T. FENNELL
Affiliation:
School of Psychology, Department of Linguistics, University of Ottawa
LAURA BOSCH*
Affiliation:
Department of Cognition, Development and Education Psychology, University of Barcelona. Institute of Neurosciences, University of Barcelona
*
Address for correspondence: Laura Bosch, Department of Cognition, Development and Education Psychology, University of Barcelona, 171, Passeig Vall d'Hebron, 08035-Barcelona (Spain) [email protected]

Abstract

Twelve-month-old bilingual and monolingual infants show comparable phonetic discrimination skills for vowels belonging to their native language/s. However, Catalan–Spanish bilingual toddlers, but not Catalan monolinguals, appear insensitive to a vowel mispronunciation in familiar words involving the Catalan–Specific /e/-/ɛ/ contrast. Here bilingual and monolingual toddlers were tested in a challenging minimal-pair word learning task involving that contrast (i.e., [bepi]-[bɛpi]). Both groups succeeded, suggesting that bilinguals can successfully use their phonetic categories to phonologically encode novel words. It is argued that bilinguals’ impoverished vowel representations in familiar words might be the result of experiential input factors (e.g., cognate words and mispronunciations due to accented speech).

Type
Research Notes
Copyright
Copyright © Cambridge University Press 2016 

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

Footnotes

*

This research has received funding from the Spanish MINECO (projects PSI2011-25376 and PSI2014-55105P to LB) and the Natural Sciences and Engineering Research Council of Canada to CTF. The authors want to thank all the infants and the families who participated in this research.

References

Best, C. T. (1994). The emergence of native-language phonological influences in infants: a perceptual assimilation model. In Goodman, J., & Nusbaum, H. C. (Eds.), Development of speech perception: The transition from speech sounds to spoken words (pp. 167224). Cambridge: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Best, C. T., Tyler, M. D., Gooding, T. N., Orlando, C. B., & Quann, C. A. (2009). Development of phonological constancy: toddlers' perception of native- and Jamaican-accented words. Psychological science, 20 (5), 539542. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-9280.2009.02327.x Google Scholar
Bosch, L., & Ramon-Casas, M. (2011). Variability in vowel production by bilingual speakers: Can input properties hinder the early stabilization of contrastive categories? Journal of Phonetics, 39, 514526. doi: 0.1016/j.wocn.2011.02.001 Google Scholar
Bosch, L., & Ramon-Casas, M. (2014). First translation equivalents in bilingual toddlers’ expressive vocabulary: Does form similarity matter? International Journal of Behavioral Development, 38 (4): 317322. doi: 10.1177/0165025414532559 Google Scholar
Bosch, L., & Sebastián-Gallés, N. (2001). Early language differentiation in bilingual infants. In Cenoz, J. & Genesee, F. (Eds.), Trends in bilingual acquisition (pp. 7193). Amsterdam: John Benjamins.Google Scholar
Bosch, L., & Sebastián-Gallés, N. (2003). Simultaneous bilingualism and the perception of a language-specific vowel contrast in the first year of life. Language and Speech, 46 (2-3), 217243. doi: 10.1177/00238309030460020801 Google Scholar
Byers-Heinlein, K. (2013). Parental language mixing: Its measurement and the relation of mixed input to young bilingual children's vocabulary size. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition,16, 3248. doi: 10.1017/S1366728912000120 Google Scholar
Byers-Heinlein, K., & Fennell, C. T. (2014). Perceptual narrowing in the context of increased variation: insights from bilingual infants. Developmental Psychobiology, 56 (2), 274291. doi: 10.1002/dev.21167 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cohen, L.B., Atkinson, D.J., & Chaput, H.H. (2004). Habit X: A new program for obtaining and organizing data in infant perception and cognition studies (Version 1.0). Austin, TX: University of Texas.Google Scholar
Core, C., Hoff, E., Rumiche, R., & Señor, M. (2013). Total and conceptual vocabulary in Spanish–English bilinguals from 22 to 30 months: implications for assessment. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 56 (5), 16371649. doi: 10.1044/1092-4388(2013/11-0044) Google Scholar
Cortés, S., Lleó, C., & Benet, A. (2009). Gradient merging of vowels in Barcelona Catalan under the influence of Spanish. In Braunmuller, K., & House, J. (Eds.), Convergence and Divergence in Language Contact Situations (pp. 185204). Amsterdam, The Netherlands: John Benjamins.Google Scholar
Curtin, S., Byers-Heinlein, K., & Werker, J.F. (2011). Bilingual beginnings as a lens for theory development: PRIMIR in focus. Journal of Phonetics, 39 (4), 492504. doi: 10.1016/j.wocn.2010.12.002 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Dietrich, C., Swingley, D., & Werker, J. F. (2007). Native language governs interpretation of salient speech sound differences at 18 months. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 104 (41), 1602716031. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0705270104 Google Scholar
Durrant, S., Delle Luche, C., Cattani, A., & Floccia, C. (2014). Monodialectal and multidialectal infants' representation of familiar words. Journal of Child Language, 21, 119. doi: 10.1017/S0305000914000063 Google Scholar
Escudero, P., & Boersma, P. (2004). Bridging the gap between L2 speech perception research and phonological theory. Studies on Second Language Acquisition, 26, 551585. doi: 10.10170/S02722631040-40021 Google Scholar
Escudero, P., Best, C. T., Kitamura, C., & Mulak, K. E. (2014). Magnitude of phonetic distinction predicts success at early word learning in native and non-native accents. Frontiers in Psychology, 5. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2014.01059 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Fennell, C.T., Byers-Heinlein, K., & Werker, J.F. (2007). Using speech sounds to guide word learning: The case of bilingual infants. Child Development, 78 (5), 15101525. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-8624.2007.01080.x Google Scholar
Fernald, A., Pinto, J. P., Swingley, D., Weinberg, A., & McRoberts, G. W. (1998). Rapid gains in speed of verbal processing by infants in the 2nd year. Psychological Science, 9 (3), 228231. doi: 10.1111/1467-9280.00044.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Fenson, L., Dale, P.S., Reznick, J.S., Bates, E., Thal, J., & Pethick, S.J. (1994). Variability in early communicative development. Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development, 59 (5), 1173. doi: 10.2307/1166093 Google Scholar
Graf Estes, K., & Hay, J. F. (2015). Flexibility in bilingual infants' word learning. Child Development, 86 (5), 13711385. doi: 10.1111/cdev.12392 Google Scholar
Green, J. N. (1988). Spanish. In Harris, M. & Vincent, N. (Eds.), The romance languages (pp.79130). Cambridge: CUP.Google Scholar
Hoff, E., Core, C., Place, S., Rumiche, R., Señor, M., & Parra, M. (2012). Dual language exposure and early bilingual development. Journal of Child Language, 39 (1), 127. doi:10.1017/S0305000910000759 Google Scholar
Jusczyk, P. W. (1997). The discovery of spoken language. Cambridge: MIT press.Google Scholar
Mora, J. C., & Nadeu, M. (2012). L2 effects on the perception and production of a native vowel contrast in early bilinguals. International Journal of Bilingualism, 16 (4), 484500. doi: 10.1177/1367006911429518 Google Scholar
Place, S., & Hoff, E. (2011). Properties of dual language exposure that influence two-year-olds’ bilingual proficiency. Child Development, 82 (6), 18341849. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-8624.2011.01660.x Google Scholar
Ramon-Casas, M., & Bosch, L. (2010). Are non-cognate words phonologically better specified than cognates in the early lexicon of bilingual children? Selected Proceedings of 4th Laboratory Approaches to Spanish Phonology Conference (pp. 3136). Cascadilla Press. doi: 10.13140/2.1.4167.0081 Google Scholar
Ramon-Casas, M., Swingley, D., Sebastián-Gallés, N., & Bosch, L. (2009). Vowel categorization during word recognition in bilingual toddlers. Cognitive Psychology, 59 (1), 96121. doi: 10.1016/j.cogpsych.2009.02.002 Google Scholar
Sebastián-Gallés, N., Echeverría, S., & Bosch, L. (2005). The influence of initial exposure on lexical representation: comparing early and simultaneous bilinguals. Journal of Memory and Language, 52 (2), 240255. doi: 10.1016/j.jml.2004.11.001 Google Scholar
Swingley, D., & Aslin, R. N. (2000). Spoken word recognition and lexical representation in very young children. Cognition, 76 (2), 147166. doi: 10.1016/S0010-0277(00)00081-0 Google Scholar
Swingley, D., & Aslin, R. N. (2002). Lexical neighborhoods and the word-form representations of 14-month-olds. Psychological Science, 13 (5), 480484. doi: 10.1111/1467-9280.00485 Google Scholar
Werker, J.F., & Curtin, S. (2005). PRIMIR: A developmental framework of infant speech processing. Language Learning and Development, 1 (2), 197234. doi: 10.1080/15475441.2005.9684216 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Werker, J.F., Fennell, C.T., Corcoran, K., & Stager, C.L. (2002). Infants' ability to learn phonetically similar words: Effects of age and vocabulary size. Infancy, 3 (1), 130. doi: 10.1207/S15327078IN0301_1 Google Scholar
Werker, J. F., & Tees, R. C. (1984). Cross-language speech perception: Evidence for perceptual reorganization during the first year of life. Infant Behavior and Development, 7 (1), 4963. doi: 10.1016/S0163-6383(02)00093-0 Google Scholar
Yoshida, K. A., Fennell, C. T., Swingley, D., & Werker, J. F. (2009). Fourteen month-old infants learn similar sounding words. Developmental Science, 12 (3), 412418. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-7687.2008.00789.x Google Scholar