Hostname: page-component-cd9895bd7-gbm5v Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-12-26T08:29:42.613Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Linguistic factors modulating gender assignment in Spanish–English bilingual speech

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  12 December 2022

Abel Cruz*
Affiliation:
Santa Clara University, Santa Clara, CA, USA
*
Address for correspondence: Abel Cruz, Department of Modern Languages & Literatures Santa Clara University 500 El Camino Real, CA 95053 [email protected]
Rights & Permissions [Opens in a new window]

Abstract

Drawing on naturally-occurring bilingual speech from a well-defined codeswitching community in Southern Arizona, this study examined the influence of semantic gender (a.k.a. biological gender), analogical gender, and other-language phonemic cues in modulating gender assignment in Spanish–English codeswitched speech. Thirty-four Spanish–English early bilinguals completed a forced-choice elicitation task involving two codeswitching environments: Spanish determiner–English noun switches (Task 1) and English–Spanish switched copula constructions (Task 2). The results revealed that for human-denoting nouns, bilinguals assigned grammatical gender based on the presupposed sex of a noun's referent in both syntactic environments tested. As for inanimate nouns, bilinguals were more likely to assign masculine over feminine gender to such nouns in determiner–noun switches, but not in switched copula constructions. Other-language phonemic cues did not influence the assignment mechanism. A methodological implication is that the study replicated the codeswitching patterns observed in naturally-occurring bilingual speech from the same bilingual community.

Type
Research Article
Creative Commons
Creative Common License - CCCreative Common License - BY
This is an Open Access article, distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution licence (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/), which permits unrestricted re-use, distribution and reproduction, provided the original article is properly cited.
Copyright
Copyright © The Author(s), 2022. Published by Cambridge University Press

Introduction

A unique feature of bilingual communication is that many bilinguals intentionally and fluidly alternate between languages when interacting with other bilinguals. This hallmark of bilingual speakers is known as codeswitching, and occurs at particular syntactic or prosodic boundaries (Bullock & Toribio, Reference Bullock, Toribio, Bullock and Toribio2009; Torres Cacoullos & Travis, Reference Torres Cacoullos and Travis2018; Poplack, Reference Poplack1980; Zentella, Reference Zentella1997). For instance, codeswitching at the det(erminer)–noun syntactic juncture illustrated in (1) is a prevalent linguistic behavior among Spanish–English bilinguals; these examples were taken from the Corpus del Español en el Sur de Arizona (The CESA Corpus, Carvalho, Reference Carvalho2012).

  1. (1)

    1. (a) y siempre tenía un wand […] pero él cargaba el wand

      and always have-IMP.3SG a.M wand […] but he carry-IMP.3SG the.M wand

      ‘and he always had a wand […] but he would carry the wand’ (CESA020)

    2. (b) él hasta a veces lo saco sin la leash

      ‘he [a dog] sometimes CL.ACC take-PST.1SG out without the.F leash’

      ‘sometimes I even take him [the dog] out without the leash’ (CESA028)

An intriguing linguistic property of the codeswitched junctures in (1) is that the English words wand in (1a) and leash in (1b) are assigned to the masculine (M) and feminine (F) Spanish gender categories, respectively. The sorting of nouns into two (or more) gender categories demonstrated in (1) is known as gender assignment in linguistic research (Corbett, Reference Corbett1991; Comrie, Reference Comrie1999). The syntactic dependencies that different gender categories trigger upon agreeing targets like determiners and/or adjectives serve as a bootstrap for the establishment of gender assignment in a speaker's mental grammar, whether or not this mental grammar is bilingual (Corbett, Reference Corbett1991; Cruz, Reference Cruz2021; Delgado, Reference Delgado and López2018; Poplack, Pousada & Sankoff, Reference Poplack, Pousada and Sankoff1982). Because Spanish exhibits grammatical gender and English does not, this raises the question of how bilinguals assign gender to English nouns occurring in Spanish–English codeswitched speech. In the case of the switched syntactic junctures in (1), gender assignment is overtly expressed in the corresponding Spanish determiners ‘el’ and ‘la’ to the English nouns wand and leash. Gender can also trigger agreement via a Spanish adjective marked for gender in English–Spanish switched copula constructions (e.g., I'm not terca ‘stubborn-F’) (Pfaff, Reference Pfaff1979; Woolford, Reference Woolford1983).

Codeswitching is tied to social and pragmatic bilingual community norms, and it remains primarily a spoken language phenomenon (Bullock & Toribio, Reference Bullock, Toribio, Bullock and Toribio2009; Torres Cacoullos & Travis, Reference Torres Cacoullos and Travis2018; Zentella, Reference Zentella1997). However, researchers agree that experimental techniques that are carefully designed to reflect the codeswitching behavior of a well-defined codeswitching community can serve as a complementary source of valuable data for a better characterization of the bilingual lexicon and the bilingual experience more generally (Beatty-Martínez, Navarro-Torres & Dussias, Reference Beatty-Martínez, Navarro-Torres and Dussias2020; Beatty-Martínez, Valdés Kroff & Dussias, Reference Beatty-Martínez, Valdés Kroff and Dussias2018; Beatty-Martínez & Dussias, Reference Beatty-Martínez and Dussias2017; Gullberg, Indefrey & Muysken, Reference Gullberg, Indefrey, Muysken, Bullock and Toribio2009; Guzzardo Tamargo, Valdés Kroff & Dussias, Reference Guzzardo Tamargo, Valdés Kroff and Dussias2016; Munarriz-Ibarrola, Ezeizabarrena, de Castro Arrazola & Parafita Couto, Reference Munarriz-Ibarrola, Ezeizabarrena, de Castro Arrazola and Parafita Couto2021; Valdés Kroff, Dussias, Gerfen, Perrotti & Bajo, Reference Valdés Kroff, Dussias, Gerfen, Perrotti and and Bajo2017; Valdés Kroff & Fernández-Duque, Reference Valdés Kroff, and Fernández-Duque, Bellamy, Child, González, Muntendam and Parafita Couto2017).

Spanish–English bilingual speakers who live and work in Southern Arizona, U.S. are well-known to engage in habitual codeswitching practices (Besset, Reference Besset2017; Casillas, Reference Casillas, Carvalho and Beaudrie2013; Cruz, Reference Cruz2016, Reference Cruz and MacDonald2018; DuBord, Reference DuBord2004; Kern, Reference Kern2019). Drawing on naturally-occurring bilingual speech from this well-defined codeswitching community in Southern Arizona, in the present experimental study I examine the linguistic information that Spanish–English bilinguals who live and work in this bilingual community may deploy when asked to assign Spanish gender to English nouns in experimentally elicited responses to Spanish det–English noun switches (e.g., el/la key ‘the.M/F’) and English–Spanish switched copula constructions (e.g., The key está rot-o/a ‘is broken-M/F’). I focus on three linguistic factors previously identified to modulate gender assignment in such syntactic environments: semantic gender (or the presupposed sex of a noun's referent) and analogical gender (or the gender of the Spanish translation equivalent). Assuming that analogical gender is applied, I also ask whether the /o/-/a/ phonemic cues that strongly correlate with gender assignment in Spanish (Teschner & Russell, Reference Teschner and Russell1984) influence the assignment mechanism applied in codeswitched speech.

This experimental study aims to supplement the spontaneously elicited data from the CESA Corpus reported in Cruz (Reference Cruz2021). By doing so, the study seeks to provide a better characterization of the codeswitching practices and language use of a well-defined codeswitching community in Southern Arizona. This experimental design may also shed new light on how representative experimental research is of naturally-occurring codeswitched speech.

The distribution of gender assignment in Spanish and English

Gender assignment is fundamentally about sorting nouns into different gender categories (or noun classes) on the basis of linguistic properties that correlate with gender assignment such as animacy and/or humanness; this assignment mechanism can also operate in an arbitrary fashion with artifacts (Corbett, Reference Corbett1991; Kramer, Reference Kramer2015).Footnote 1 Spanish and English, the languages that concern us here, exhibit different distributional pattens in terms of gender assignment. In particular, Spanish has a binary-gender system in which every noun is assigned to the masculine (M) or feminine (F) gender categories, and the distribution of these gender categories is approximately equal in the Spanish lexicon (53% masculine, 47% feminine) (Teschner & Russell, Reference Teschner and Russell1984). While most Spanish nouns are presumably assigned gender arbitrarily (Harris, Reference Harris1991), some phonemes strongly correlate with gender assignment in this language. For example, Spanish nouns ending in the phoneme /o/ are masculine in 99.87% of cases (el libro ‘the.M book’) and those ending in the phoneme /a/ are feminine in 96% of cases (la mesa ‘the.F table’), respectively (Teschner & Russell, Reference Teschner and Russell1984).

In fact, Spanish-speaking children are sensitive to the /o/-/a/ phonemic gender contrast when determining the gender of a Spanish noun or a novel word with Spanish phonotactics (Mariscal, Reference Mariscal2008; Lindsey & Gerken, Reference Lindsey and Gerken2012; Pérez-Pereira, Reference Pérez-Pereira1991). Similarly, Spanish second language (L2) learners and heritage speakers exploit such gender cues when establishing gender assignment/agreement in Spanish (Alarcón, Reference Alarcón2011; Montrul, Foote & Perpiñán, Reference Montrul, Foote and Perpiñán2008). Therefore, in the present study I take a step further and ask whether the /o/-/a/ gender cues of the Spanish translation equivalents influence how Spanish–English bilinguals assign gender to English nouns occurring in elicited responses to Spanish–English codeswitched speech. It is important to mention that this potential assignment strategy should not be confused with a ‘shape-based assignment strategy’ where the phonemic make of the switched noun, and not the phonemic make up of the translation equivalent of the switched noun, serves as a cue to gender assignment in codeswitched speech (e.g., see Munarriz-Ibarrola et al., Reference Munarriz-Ibarrola, Ezeizabarrena, de Castro Arrazola and Parafita Couto2021).

English, on the other hand, has a pronominal gender system whereby pronouns agree with the presupposed sex of human referents (e.g., The doctor said she/he could see me tomorrow.) (Corbett, Reference Corbett1991; Comrie, Reference Comrie1999). Moreover, English has a handful of human-denoting nouns that are morphologically marked for feminine gender (e.g., actress, princess, duchess, etc.) (McConnell-Ginet, Reference McConnell-Ginet and Corbett2013). English then has a pronominal gender system which operates on the basis of the presupposed sex of a noun's referent, but grammatical gender is absolute with non-human referents.

Gender assignment strategies in spontaneously elicited bilingual speech

Gender assignment in codeswitched speech is a promising research agenda in bilingualism (Balam, Reference Balam2016; Jake, Myers-Scotton & Gross, Reference Jake, Myers-Scotton and Gross2002; Herring, Deuchar, Parafita Couto & Moro Quintanilla, Reference Herring, Deuchar, Parafita Couto and Moro Quintanilla2010; Parafita Couto, Munarriz, Epelde, Deuchar & Oyharçabal, Reference Parafita Couto, Munarriz, Epelde, Deuchar and and Oyharçabal2015; Pfaff, Reference Pfaff1979, among many others). The main question in this line of research is how bilingual speakers determine the grammatical gender of an otherwise genderless noun occurring in switched determiner phrases, as illustrated in (1). In (2), I list the most commonly-attested gender assignment strategies that may promote gender assignment in spontaneously elicited codeswitched speech (see Bellamy & Parafita Couto, Reference Bellamy, Parafita Couto and Ayoun2022 for comprehensive review):

  1. (2) Gender assignment strategies in spontaneously elicited codeswitched speech

    1. (a) A semantic gender assignment strategy (a.k.a. biological gender) where the presupposed sex of a noun's referent determines the grammatical gender of human-denoting nouns occurring in codeswitched speech.

    2. (b) A gender transfer strategy in which the genderless noun is assigned the grammatical gender of its translation equivalent (a.k.a. analogical gender).

    3. (c) A shape-based assignment strategy in which some phonemic cues of the otherwise genderless noun occurring in codeswitched speech correlate with a certain gender category in the gendered language.

    4. (d) A default assignment strategy attributed to bilingual community norms rather than the internal mechanisms of gender assignment in the gendered language.

While the assignment strategies in (2) are well-attested across bi-/multilingual communities, they are not representative of a specific bilingual community. For instance, Spanish–English bilinguals in the Bangor Miami Corpus (Deuchar, Davies, Herring, Parafita Couto & Carter, Reference Deuchar, Davies, Herring, Parafita Couto, Carter, Thomas and Mennen2014) overwhelmingly adopted a masculine default assignment strategy even with human-denoting nouns (Valdés Kroff, Reference Valdés Kroff, Guzzardo Tamargo, Mazak and MC2016), whereas Spanish–English bilinguals from the New Mexico Spanish–English Bilingual Corpus (Torres Cacoullos & Travis, Reference Torres Cacoullos and Travis2018) applied semantic gender as defined in (2a) in a categorical fashion with human nouns (Trawick & Bero, Reference Trawick and Bero2021). The default assignment strategy defined in (2d) is a prevalent codeswitching behavior across Spanish–English bilingual communities in the U.S. and has been found to be present as early as age seven for Spanish–English bilingual children (Balam, Lakshmanan & Parafita Couto, Reference Balam, Lakshmanan and Parafita Couto2021). The shape-based assignment strategy defined in (2c) is attested in spontaneous Spanish–Basque codeswitched speech (Parafita Couto et al., Reference Parafita Couto, Munarriz, Epelde, Deuchar and and Oyharçabal2015) and further confirmed in experimental settings in the same language pair (Munarriz-Ibarrola et al., Reference Munarriz-Ibarrola, Ezeizabarrena, de Castro Arrazola and Parafita Couto2021).

Gender assignment in the spontaneous codeswitched speech of a Spanish–English bilingual community from Southern Arizona, U.S. is particularly relevant for the purpose of the present study. Spanish is in consistent contact with English in Southern Arizona, and Spanish–English bilinguals who live and work in this geographical region are well-documented to engage in codeswitching practices on a daily basis (Besset, Reference Besset2017; Casillas, Reference Casillas, Carvalho and Beaudrie2013; Cruz, Reference Cruz2016, Reference Cruz and MacDonald2018; DuBord, Reference DuBord2004; Kern, Reference Kern2019). A large sample of Spanish–English bilingual speakers from this well-defined codeswitching community is documented in the CESA Corpus (Carvalho, Reference Carvalho2012). Recently, Cruz (2021) examined gender assignment in Spanish det–English noun switches (e.g., el wand ‘the.M’) in 76 sociolinguistic interviews of approximately one hour each from the CESA Corpus. Fifty-three Spanish–English bilinguals from the 76 interviews analyzed in Cruz's study produced Spanish det–English noun switches. These bilinguals self-reported being exposed to Spanish and English either simultaneously from birth or sequentially but at a very young age, mainly receiving exposure to English through daycare and/or school. Importantly, they also reported to engage in habitual codeswitching practices.

In his analysis, Cruz found that feminine analogical gender was restricted to a small set of English inanimate nouns with Spanish feminine translation equivalents, whereas bilinguals overwhelmingly adopted a masculine default assignment strategy with the majority of inanimate nouns. Human-denoting nouns were an exception to the masculine default strategy observed with inanimate nouns. With this set of nouns, bilinguals assigned grammatical gender based on the presupposed sex of a noun's referent, so that male nouns are masculine and female nouns are feminine in a categorical fashion; see also DuBord (Reference DuBord2004) for similar findings from another sample of Spanish–English bilinguals from Southern Arizona.

The present experimental study is a follow-up to the gender assignment strategies observed for the bilinguals in the CESA Corpus. This experimental design aims to delimit the extent to which the Spanish gender system (or analogical gender) mediates gender assignment in experimentally elicited responses to Spanish–English codeswitched speech by a bilingual sample from the same community as the informants of the CESA Corpus. This experimental design is important because (i) feminine gender is much less frequent than masculine gender with non-human nouns and (ii) human nouns are infrequent in the CESA Corpus. But most importantly, this study may shed new light on how representative experimental research is of naturally-occurring codeswitched speech.

Experimental studies on gender assignment in bilingual speech

Codeswitching is by definition a pragmatically and socially driven language phenomenon, and experimental designs that seek to minimize variables of no interest compromise its spontaneous nature. Yet, scholars agree that experimental research can contribute to a better understanding of this linguistic behavior unique to bilinguals (Beatty-Martínez et al., Reference Beatty-Martínez, Valdés Kroff and Dussias2018; Gullberg et al., Reference Gullberg, Indefrey, Muysken, Bullock and Toribio2009; Munarriz-Ibarrola et al., Reference Munarriz-Ibarrola, Ezeizabarrena, de Castro Arrazola and Parafita Couto2021; Valdés Kroff et al., Reference Valdés Kroff, and Fernández-Duque, Bellamy, Child, González, Muntendam and Parafita Couto2017; Valdés Kroff & Fernández-Duque, Reference Valdés Kroff, Dussias, Gerfen, Perrotti and and Bajo2017). Given the asymmetry of grammatical gender in Spanish and English, a logical question from an experimental perspective is whether Spanish–English bilinguals who engage in codeswitching practices apply analogical gender as defined in (2b) when asked to assign gender to English nouns occurring in different codeswitching environments. Although the empirical evidence indicates that Spanish–English bilinguals gravitate toward a masculine default assignment strategy, feminine analogical gender also plays a role in experimentally elicited responses to codeswitched speech.

For instance, in a codeswitching picture naming task where the labels of the target pictures were equally divided for masculine and feminine Spanish labels, Fairchild and Van Hell (Reference Fairchild and Van Hell2017) found that Spanish–English bilinguals produced the masculine determiner ‘el’ with English nouns whose Spanish translation equivalents are feminine only 7.2% of time. This finding suggests that bilinguals applied feminine analogical gender with most English nouns with feminine Spanish translations. On the other hand, Denbaum and de Prada Pérez (Reference Denbaum and de Prada Pérez2020) found that Spanish–English bilinguals were more likely to adopt a masculine default strategy with most English nouns occurring in codeswitched speech, rather than applying analogical gender, in a story telling task. Similarly, Valenzuela, Faure, Ramírez-Trujillo, Barski, Pangtay and Diez (Reference Valenzuela, Faure, Ramírez-Trujillo, Barski, Pangtay and Diez2012) did not find support for analogical gender in an acceptability judgement task targeting Spanish det–English noun switches. Importantly, these last two studies also examined the potential influence of the /o/-/a/ gender cues of the Spanish translation equivalents in modulating gender assignment in codeswitched speech. The results, however, indicated that such phonemic cues did not modulate the assignment mechanism since masculine was predominantly the default gender.

Of relevance to the present study, Valenzuela et al. (Reference Valenzuela, Faure, Ramírez-Trujillo, Barski, Pangtay and Diez2012) also examined gender assignment in Spanish copula constructions where a Spanish adjective controlled for gender agreement of an English noun (e.g., Está crud-o/a ‘[the meat] is raw-M/F’). They reported that gender congruency based on the gender of the Spanish translation equivalent was relatively low for feminine gender in Spanish det–English noun switches (56%) compared to Spanish copula constructions (71%), whereas masculine gender was at ceiling in both syntactic environments. This particular finding opens the possibility that feminine gender may be more prevalent in switched copula constructions than switched determiner phrases – a question I explore in the present study.

Language processing studies have also revealed important findings in terms of gender assignment in Spanish–English codeswitched speech. For instance, Beatty-Martínez and Dussias (Reference Beatty-Martínez and Dussias2017) emphasized that feminine gender is restricted to English nouns with Spanish feminine translations (e.g., la spoon ‘the.F’, congruent condition with feminine translation equivalent) in spontaneously elicited codeswitched speech, whereas masculine gender does not extend to feminine Spanish translations (e.g., la fork ‘the.F’, incongruent condition with masculine transition equivalent). Using event-related potentials (ERPs), they found that Spanish–English bilinguals who habitually engage in codeswitching practices exhibited greater difficulty when processing incongruent switches (e.g., la fork ‘the.F’) compared to the congruent condition, suggesting that these bilinguals are rarely exposed to the incongruent condition. Taking the same gender asymmetry studied in Beatty-Martínez and Dussias (Reference Beatty-Martínez and Dussias2017), Valdés Kroff et al. (Reference Valdés Kroff, Dussias, Gerfen, Perrotti and and Bajo2017) employed the visual world paradigm technique (eye-tracking data) to codeswitching and showed that only the feminine article ‘la’, but not its masculine counterpart ‘el’, facilitated the upcoming of English nouns with Spanish feminine translations (e.g., la house ‘the.F’). Together, these language processing studies confirmed that Spanish–English bilinguals attended to both masculine and feminine analogical genders when processing Spanish–English codeswitched speech on real time.

While current experimental studies indicate that masculine gender is the prevailing gender in Spanish–English codeswitched speech similar to spontaneously elicited data, current studies have not been able to validate their experimental data with spontaneously elicited data from the same bilingual community, with the exception of Beatty-Martínez and Dussias (Reference Beatty-Martínez and Dussias2017). The present study is a step toward this goal.

The present study

The present study aims to delimit the extent to which analogical gender is empirically supported in experimentally elicited responses to codeswitched speech from a well-defined codeswitching community in Southern Arizona, U.S. If analogical gender is at work, I further explore the potential influence of other-language phonemic cues in modulating the assignment mechanism. I also study the role of semantic gender in determining the grammatical gender of human-denoting nouns. These linguistic factors are examined in two codeswitching environments: Spanish det–English noun switches (e.g., el/la key ‘the.M/F’) and English–Spanish switched copula constructions (e.g., The key está rot-o/a ‘is broken-M/F’). While codeswitching within the determiner phrase is well-attested in spontaneous codeswitched speech, English–Spanish copula constructions are admittedly less common (Pfaff, Reference Pfaff1979; Poplack, Reference Poplack1980; Woolford, Reference Woolford1983). Yet, the existing experimental evidence indicates that Spanish–English bilinguals exhibited higher gender congruency for feminine analogical gender in Spanish copula constructions compared to switched determiner phrases (Valenzuela et al., Reference Valenzuela, Faure, Ramírez-Trujillo, Barski, Pangtay and Diez2012). Unlike Valenzuela et al.'s study where Spanish copula constructions were used, the present study includes switched copula constructions consisting of an English determiner and a noun followed by one of the Spanish copula verbs ser or estar, both meaning ‘to be’.

Methods

Participants

Thirty-four Spanish–English bilingual speakers (27 females) between the ages of 18 and 29 (M = 20.65; SD = 2.71) participated in this study. They represented early bilinguals immersed in a bilingual experience from birth or as early sequential bilinguals – that is, they were first exposed to Spanish in the family at an age that ranged from 0 to 5 years (M = .34, SD = 1.08) and to English at an age that ranged from 0 to 12 years (M = 3.63, SD = 2.56). All participants were recruited in Tucson, Arizona, U.S. This particular geographical region of the U.S. was chosen because Spanish–English bilinguals who live and work in Southern Arizona, including Tucson, are well-known to engage in habitual codeswitching practices (Besset, Reference Besset2017; Casillas, Reference Casillas, Carvalho and Beaudrie2013; Cruz, Reference Cruz2016, Reference Cruz and MacDonald2018; DuBord, Reference DuBord2004; Kern, Reference Kern2019).

All participants completed a modified version of the Language and Social Background Questionnaire (LSBQ) originally developed by Anderson, Mak, Keyvani Chahi and Bialystok (Reference Anderson, Mak, Keyvani Chahi and Bialystok2018). The LSBQ elicited participants’ demographics, language background information, and self-ratings of proficiency in both Spanish and English. All participants gave informed consent approved by Georgetown University Institutional Review Board and were paid $15 for their participation. Twenty-nine (or 85.3%) of the 34 sample were born and raised in Southern Arizona, including Tucson; four participants were born and raised in California but were working or studying in Arizona at the time of the study, and only one participant was born in Oklahoma but raised in Arizona. The majority of the participants (31 of the 34 total sample, or 91.17%) were enrolled in an undergraduate degree program at a large public institution in Southern Arizona; two participants (or 5.89%) were enrolled at a community college in the same region, and one participant (or 2.94%) had completed only high school at the time of the study.

A self-report measure where 1 indicates not proficient at all and 7 indicates highly proficient was used to assess participants’ linguistic proficiency in reading, writing, speaking, and listening in both Spanish and English. Proficiency in Spanish was also assessed through the Spanish Elicited Imitation Task (EIT) in Bowden (Reference Bowden2016), a slightly modified version of a shortcut measure of language proficiency developed by Ortega, Iwashita, Norris, and Rabie (Reference Ortega, Iwashita, Norris and Rabie2002) and widely used for research purposes in many languages including Spanish (Park, Solon, Henderson & Dehghan-Chaleshtori, Reference Park, Solon, Henderson and Dehghan-Chaleshtori2020) and English (Wu, Tio & Ortega, Reference Wu, Tio and Ortega2022). The EIT scores are taken as independent evidence for proficiency that complements the self-report data collected in the language questionnaire. Participants were also asked whether they codeswitch and what percentage of the time they codeswitch on a weekly basis with parents, siblings, friends, classmates, and social media. Importantly, self-reported measures of codeswitching are a reliable tool to determine a Spanish–English bilingual's production of English in otherwise Spanish discourse (Cox, LaBoda & Mendes, Reference Cox, LaBoda and Mendes2020; Valdés Kroff & Fernández-Duque, Reference Valdés Kroff, Dussias, Gerfen, Perrotti and and Bajo2017). The full set of participants’ bilingual experience and language proficiency is reported in Table 1.

Table 1. Characteristics of Spanish–English early bilinguals

Note. Self-ratings are from 1 = not proficient at all to 7 = highly proficient. Spanish EIT maximum total score is 120. Codeswitching is expressed in percent per week.

All participants reported that they learned Spanish at home but were fully schooled in English. More specifically, participants’ mean onset age of exposure to Spanish was .34 years (SD = 1.08, min = 0 and max = 5 years), whereas mean age of first exposure to English was 3.62 years (SD = 2.56, min = 0 and max = 12 years). In other words, the bilingual profile of this bilingual sample indicates that some of the participants were immersed in a bilingual experience from birth (simultaneous bilinguals), while others are early sequential bilinguals who experienced a short period of monolingual learning and were subsequently exposed to English during the first years of life through daycare and/or preschool (e.g., Armon-Lotem & Meir, Reference Armon-Lotem, Meir, De Houwer and Ortega2019; Ortega, Reference Ortega2020). As for language use, participants reported that they speak Spanish and English with their siblings at home but only Spanish with their parents. With the exception of a few participants (or 5.89% for the 34 sample), 32 (or 94.11%) participants reported that they codeswitch on a daily basis, especially with friends and social media.

In terms of language proficiency, participants assigned themselves high proficiency for both languages in the self-reporting measure. Based on these scores, a one-factor multivariate analysis of variance (MANOVA) was conducted to compare self-reported scores for proficiency in English vs. Spanish. The four language skills in the self-reporting measure served as the dependent variables in the analysis, and language (Spanish vs. English) comprised the independent variable. The results from the MANOVA were statistically significant according to Wilk's Λ (0.79), F(4, 63) =3.950, p = .006. Separate univariate tests on the outcome variables revealed statistically significantly differences on reading F(1, 66) =9.56, p = .003 and writing F(1, 66) =14.48, p = .000, but non-significant differences on speaking F(1, 66) =3.43, p = .068 or listening F(1, 66) =0.788, p = .378. In other words, participants scored themselves higher in reading and writing in English compared to Spanish, but they are balanced bilinguals when it comes to oral communication. These results are not surprising since participants indicated that they were fully schooled in English, whereas Spanish is often limited to the home environment. The EIT global proficiency measure confirmed participants’ very high proficiency in Spanish with a 113.23 mean accuracy rate, very ‘high’ in the sense that the highest mean reported in Bowden (Reference Bowden2016) for her ‘very advanced’ group was a 109.3 mean accuracy.

In summary, participants are highly proficient Spanish–English bilinguals who frequently engage in codeswitching practices. Since they live and work in a well-defined codeswitching community in Southern Arizona, it is also fair to suggest that they are exposed to codeswitched speech on a daily basis. Importantly, the bilingual profiles of these participants mirror the language profiles of the bilinguals studied in Cruz (Reference Cruz2021).

Materials

The critical stimuli included 80 English nouns equally divided into human-denoting nouns (e.g., boy, girl, nephew, etc.) and inanimate nouns (e.g., spoon, door, key, etc.). 68 nouns from another set were used as distractors, including 12 human-denoting nouns with context-dependent gender which could occur more than once in the distractor item condition (e.g., coach, friend, witness, etc.) and 56 English inanimate nouns with masculine and feminine Spanish translations for a total of 68 nouns (see Appendix S1 in Supplementary Materials for list of target and distractor items). All target nouns (n = 80) were equally divided for grammatical gender based on their Spanish translation equivalents (analogical gender). Target nouns were further subdivided into three conditions representing the three linguistic variables that concern us in the present study.

Variable 1 is ‘semantic gender,’ represented in 40 human nouns equally divided into male and female referents whose Spanish translations are masculine and feminine, respectively. These human nouns have lexico-semantic gender (e.g., nephew vs. niece), as opposed to context-dependent gender (e.g., coach or doctor). Variable 2 is ‘analogical gender,’ represented in 40 English inanimate nouns equally divided into masculine and feminine analogical gender based on the gender of their Spanish translation equivalents (e.g., English key is feminine in Spanish, la llave). Variable 3 represents ‘other-language phonemic cues.’ For this variable, the 80 target nouns were equally divided into four conditions based on the phonemic make up of their Spanish translations: highly reliable phonemic cue for masculine gender (e.g., -o in vestido ‘dress,’ n = 20), no phonemic cue for masculine gender (e.g., -Ø in lápiz ‘pencil,’ n = 20), highly reliable phonemic cue for feminine gender (e.g., -a in puerta ‘door,’ n = 20), and no phonemic cue for feminine gender (e.g., -Ø in sal ‘salt,’ n = 20).

The majority of inanimate nouns used in the present study are frequent nouns found in the CESA Corpus analyzed in Cruz (Reference Cruz2021). However, most of the human-denoting nouns may be less representative of occurring in codeswitched speech since such nouns are relatively infrequent in the CESA Corpus. These 80 target nouns occurred in two syntactic environments which are described next.

Procedure

Although codeswitching is a socially and pragmatically driven linguistic behavior (Bullock & Toribio, Reference Bullock, Toribio, Bullock and Toribio2009; Torres Cacoullos & Travis, Reference Torres Cacoullos and Travis2018; Zentella, Reference Zentella1997), every effort was made in order to foster ecological validity in this experimental design. Two forced-choice elicitation tasks were developed to examine the deployment of linguistic information when determining the gender of an English noun occurring in codeswitched speech. Task 1 elicited evidence for gender assignment in Spanish det–English noun switches. For Task 1, a total of 80 Spanish–English codeswitched sentences embedding a target noun along with a Spanish verb and prepositions was created by the researcher (e.g., Ya estamos en __ plane ‘We are already in __ plane’). The target noun was the only switch site in these carrier sentences. Although the carrier sentences were not exactly the same utterances observed in the CESA Corpus, they were constructed after an analysis of English nouns inserted in otherwise Spanish-initiated conversations in this corpus (see Cruz, Reference Cruz2021 for relevant data), and their likelihood as a possible site for codeswitching was confirmed by two bilingual speakers who engage in codeswitching. All carrier sentences consisted of no more than 8 words whereby the target noun always appeared either after the second or the third word in the sentence. The Spanish masculine article el and the feminine article la were used as the possible syntactic elements that could complete the carrier sentences in a binary response-choice.

Another set of 75 Spanish–English switched sentences (e.g., No sabe __ password ‘S/he doesn't know __ password’) served as the carrier sentences for the distractor items in Task 1. The Spanish possessive adjectives su meaning ‘his/her/its/their’ and mi meaning ‘my’ were the possible syntactic elements that could complete the distractor carrier sentences in a binary response-choice. Importantly, only the Spanish articles el and la, but not su and mi, are marked for gender in Spanish. A participant then saw a total of 155 stimuli sentences in Task 1.

A follow-up task to Task 1 was also developed to further examine gender assignment in English–Spanish switched copula constructions (e.g., The key está rot-o/a ‘is broken-M/F’) with the same target nouns used in Task 1. This particular syntactic juncture is important to include in the present study because Valenzuela et al. (Reference Valenzuela, Faure, Ramírez-Trujillo, Barski, Pangtay and Diez2012) found that Spanish–English bilinguals exhibited higher gender congruency for feminine analogical gender in Spanish copula constructions compared to switched determiner phrases in a forced-choice elicitation task. Therefore, Task 2 aims to provide further empirical evidence of gender assignment in different syntactic environments by the same sample of bilinguals who participated in Task 1.

For the purpose of Task 2, a total of 80 carrier phrases was created by the researcher, and the phrases’ likelihood as possible sites for codeswitching was confirmed by the two bilinguals who rated the sentences of Task 1. The carrier phrases consisted of an English determiner and a target noun followed by one of the Spanish copula verbs ser or estar, both meaning ‘to be’. The carrier phrases triggered an attributive adjective as the potential syntactic element that could complete the carrier phrase (e.g., The key está ___ ‘The key is ___’). The binary response-choice consisted of a masculine (e.g., an adjective ending in -o, roto ‘broken-M’) and a feminine Spanish adjective (e.g., the same adjective ending in -a, rota ‘broken-F’). A total of 45 Spanish attributive adjectives morphologically marked for gender was used as the response-choice for Task 2.

Another set of 75 carrier phrases was also created for the same distractor items used in Task 1. The binary response-choice for the distractor items for Task 2 consisted of a Spanish adjective unmarked for gender and its English equivalent (e.g., Spanish triste and English sad). All target adjectives used in Task 2 are frequent Spanish adjectives extracted from the EsPal dataset (Duchon, Perea, Sebastián-Gallés, Martí & Carreiras, Reference Duchon, Perea, Sebastián-Gallés, Martí and Carreiras2013; retrieved from https://www.bcbl.eu/databases/espal/). Importantly, grammatical gender is the only difference between the binary response-choice in Task 2 since the target adjective marked for gender did not differ in meaning. In Task 2, a participant then saw a total of 155 carrier phrases embedding the same target nouns used in Task 1 (see Appendix S2 in Supplementary Materials for list of carrier sentences).

Participants completed the experiment individually in a quiet room at an educational institution. They completed a language questionnaire on paper and were subsequently guided to read the instructions of the experiment on a computer screen. The instructions were presented in English and indicated that participants would see a list of short sentences missing information and that their task was to complete the sentences with the information appearing underneath in a way that sounded as natural as possible to them. The carrier sentences (or phrases) embedding the target noun were presented on a 13.75″ × 9.48″ computer screen using PsychoPy3 (Peirce, Gray, Simpson, MacAskill, Höchenberger, Sogo, Kastman & Lindeløv, Reference Peirce, Gray, Simpson, MacAskill, Höchenberger, Sogo, Kastman and Lindeløv2019). The carrier sentence and the binary response-choice appeared simultaneously for both the target and distractor items and in both tasks. The 80 carrier sentences and their respective response-choices were pseudo-randomized among the 75 distractor sentences (phrases) in both tasks; the binary response-choice was also randomized so that el and la (or a Spanish adjective marked for gender in Task 2) could appear either on the left or the right of the screen. All stimuli sentences were presented in an untimed manner; this was necessary to ensure that participants were selecting the response-choice that sounded most natural to them – that is, their preferred codeswitching practice. Participants were instructed to press a key on a computer keyboard to select their responses accordingly.

Participants completed a trial of eight practice items. The researcher stood in the room while participants completed the practice trial to make sure that participants had no questions. Once the practice trail was over, the PsychoPy code guided the participants to the target trials for Task 1. When Task 1 was completed, participants were thanked for completing the task and were further invited to continue to Task 2; they could take a break between tasks if desired. When the experiment was done, participants completed the EIT proficiency assessment reported in Table 1. The whole session took about 50 minutes.

Analyses

The responses elicited from Task 1 and Task 2 were analyzed using mixed-effects logistic regression models with a logistic linking function appropriate for categorical data (Jaeger, Reference Jaeger2008; Sommet & Morselli, Reference Sommet and Morselli2017) and with random intercepts for participants and items (Baayen, Davidson & Bates, Reference Baayen, Davidson and Bates2008). Regression analyses were performed using the lme4 package (Bates, Maechler, Bolker & Walker, Reference Bates, Maechler, Bolker and Walker2015) in the statistical software application R (R Core Team, 2018). Mixed-effects logistic regression analyses are ideal for the categorical variable that concerns us here because they perform analyses on the participants’ individual responses rather than mean responses per condition (Jaeger, Reference Jaeger2008). The model was fitted to the data obtained from Task 1 and Task 2 with response (congruent = 1, incongruent = 0) as the categorical dependent variable and semantic gender, analogical gender, and other-language phonemic cues as fixed factors. Treatment coding was used for the fixed factors such that positive coefficients would reflect an increase in likelihood of congruent responses (Baayen et al., Reference Baayen, Davidson and Bates2008). The alpha level was set at p < .001, for a conservative interpretation of statistical trustworthiness (e.g., a less than one is a thousand chance of being wrong in rejecting the null hypothesis).

Results

Results for Task 1

Task 1 examined the linguistic factors that may promote the likelihood of gender assignment (masculine or feminine) to English nouns occurring in Spanish det–English noun switches (e.g., Ya estamos en ___ plane ‘We are already in ___ plane’). For sake of clarity, I describe the results in terms of gender congruency, which refers to the proportion of Spanish determiners whose gender is consistent (gender congruent or gender incongruent) with the Spanish translation equivalent of the English target noun. Table 2 reports the proportions of gender-congruent selections per condition for Task 1.

Table 2. Proportions of gender-congruent selections per condition for Task 1

Note. Total possible score per condition is 10, one point per noun.

The descriptive statistics in Table 2 show that gender-congruent selections were at ceiling with human-denoting nouns whereby nouns with male referents are masculine (Conditions 1 & 2 combined: M = .92, SD = .25) and those with female referents are feminine (Conditions 5 & 6 combined: M = .92, SD = .26), and this is true across the board and regardless of the other-language phonemic cues variable. As for inanimate nouns, proportions of gender-congruent selections were higher for masculine gender (Conditions 3 & 4 combined: M = .89, SD = .29) compared to feminine gender (Conditions 7 & 8 combined: M = .62, SD =.47). Descriptively, gender-congruent selections were higher for feminine gender with English nouns whose Spanish translations lack gender cues (e.g., honey, miel; M = .70, SD = .45) compared to translation equivalents that exhibit gender cues (e.g., border, frontera; M = .55, SD = .49).

To test the sensitivity to gender congruency for Task 1, the descriptive statistics in Table 2 were further analyzed using mixed-effects logistic regression models. The dependent variable for Task 1 was response-choice to gender selection (masculine or feminine). First, I ran a model containing only by-participant and by-item random intercepts. Since this model yielded changes of its intercept values when each predictor was added individually, I then fitted another model which included analogical gender with four levels (female + feminine, male + masculine, inanimate + masculine, inanimate + feminine) and the other-language phonemic cues variable for all items with two levels (strong, no cue) as fixed factors as well as an interaction between analogical gender and other-language phonemic cues. The data were treatment-coded with ‘inanimate + masculine’ and ‘strong cue’ as the reference levels for the model. The results of the best fitting model are reported in Table 3. As a reminder, positive estimates (β) indicate a greater likelihood of the outcome 1 (congruent response) and negative estimates a greater likelihood of 0 (incongruent response).

Table 3. Summary of mixed logistic regression model for variables predicting participants’ likelihood of gender-congruent selections for Task 1

Note. f = feminine, m = masculine; the parameter estimate (β), standard error of the parameter estimate (SE), z-value and p-value for predictor variables and their interaction.

Note. *p < .001.

Table 3 shows that there was a main effect for analogical gender with inanimate nouns. More specifically, the negative estimate for feminine analogical gender (β= -2.16; SE = 0.44; z= -4.84, p < . 001) indicates that participants are more likely to assign masculine gender over feminine gender to English inanimate nouns. Since gender assignment was almost categorical with human-denoting nouns (see Table 2), semantic gender was not a significant predictor when compared to the reference level. Contrary to our predictions, the other-language phonemic cues variable was not a significant predictor for gender assignment in Task 1 (β = .66; SE = 0.48; z = 1.36, p = .173), and no interaction between semantic or analogical gender and the other-language phonemic cues variable was observed. Next, I turn to the results for Task 2.

Results for Task 2

Task 2 tested gender assignment in English–Spanish switched copula constructions (e.g., The key está rot-o/a ‘is broken-M/F’). For Task 2, gender congruency refers to the proportions of Spanish adjectives whose gender is consistent with the Spanish translation equivalent of the English target noun. Table 4 reports the proportions of gender-congruent selections per condition for Task 2.

Table 4. Proportions of gender-congruent selections per condition for Task 2

Note. Total possible score per condition is 10, one point per noun.

Similar to Task 1, gender-congruent selection with human-denoting nouns was almost categorical in Task 2, regardless of other-language phonemic cues. Unlike Task 1 where participants exhibited higher gender-congruent selections with masculine gender, proportions of gender-congruent selections with inanimate nouns were relatively low for both masculine and feminine gender in Task 2, although masculine gender is slightly higher (Conditions 3 & 4 combined: M = .78, SD = .40) than feminine gender (Conditions 7 & 8 combined: M = .62, SD = .48).

The descriptive statistics in Table 4 were further submitted to a mixed-effects logistic regression analysis. The fixed factors for the analysis in Task 2 were operationalized as in Task 1 – that is, the data were treatment-coded with ‘inanimate + masculine’ as the reference level. Table 5 reports the results of the best-fitting model.

Table 5. Summary of mixed-logistic regression model for variables predicting participants’ likelihood of gender-congruent selections for Task 2

Note. f = feminine, m = masculine; the parameter estimate (β), standard error of the parameter estimate (SE), z-value and p-value for predictor variables and their interaction.

Note. *p < .001.

Similar to Task 1, the best fitting model for Task 2 indicates that semantic gender was the only significant predictor for gender congruency. More specifically, nouns with male referents are almost categorically masculine (β=2.40; SE = .47; z = 5.03, p < .001) and those with female referents are feminine (β=2.50; SE = .48; z = 5.16, p < .001). Although the descriptive statistics in Table 4 indicate a tendency for a masculine default strategy in Task 2, the analysis did not exert a significant value for masculine gender compared to feminine gender with inanimate nouns occurring in English–Spanish switched copula constructions, in contrast to Task 1. Similar to Task 1, other-language phonemic cues did not exert any main effects, and no interaction between semantic or analogical gender and the other-language phonemic cues variable was observed.

In summary, gender assignment with human-denoting nouns was almost categorical in both tasks, and regardless of the other-language phonemic cues variable. On the contrary, proportions of gender-congruent selections for feminine gender were relatively low with inanimate nouns in both tasks, whereas masculine gender exerted higher gender-congruent selections in these tasks. The logistic regression model picked up on the observed asymmetry of higher gender-congruent selections for feminine gender when it comes to female referents but very low proportions of gender-congruent selections for feminine gender when it comes to inanimate nouns. In the next section, I discuss how the current findings corroborate or deviate from the broader consensus on gender assignment in Spanish–English codeswitched speech and the implications of the current findings for a better understanding of the codeswitching practices of Spanish–English bilinguals who live and work in a well-defined codeswitching community in Southern Arizona, U.S.

General discussion

The point of departure in the present study was the observation that gender assignment is a ubiquitous linguistic feature in the bilingual speech of a well-defined Spanish–English codeswitching community in Southern Arizona, U.S., and that experimental evidence from the same bilingual community can be a valuable source of new insights into the distributional codeswitching patterns from this particular community. Drawing on the spontaneously elicited data documented in the CESA Corpus (Carvalho, Reference Carvalho2012), the present experimental study examined the potential influence of semantic gender (a.k.a. biological gender), analogical gender, and other-language phonemic cues in modulating gender assignment in two syntactic codeswitching environments: Spanish det–English noun switches (Task 1) and English–Spanish switched copula constructions (Task 2). Importantly, only Spanish, but not English, exhibits grammatical gender, and so the results of the present study can shed new light on the intricate question of whether the gender of the translation equivalent (analogical gender) mediates the assignment mechanism applied in codeswitched speech or whether this mechanism operates independently of the other-language gender system.

Human-denoting nouns can be classified according to properties of their real-world referents, and I reasoned that bilinguals would evoke the presupposed sex of a noun's referent when classifying such nouns into the masculine or feminine gender categories in codeswitched speech because semantic gender and grammatical gender coincide in Spanish (Harris, Reference Harris1991). I also emphasized that this set of nouns is particularly important to examine in experimental settings because such nouns are relatively infrequent in spontaneously elicited codeswitched speech across Spanish–English bilingual communities (Cruz, Reference Cruz2021), and no experimental study has tested this set of nouns. The results demonstrated that bilinguals classified male-denoting nouns to masculine gender and female-denoting nouns to feminine gender in a categorical fashion, and in both syntactic environments tested. In other words, bilinguals evoked the presupposed sex of a noun's referent to determine its grammatical gender in codeswitched speech. With the exception of the Bangor Miami Corpus (Deuchar et al., Reference Deuchar, Davies, Herring, Parafita Couto, Carter, Thomas and Mennen2014) where a masculine default strategy overrides semantic gender (Valdés Kroff, Reference Valdés Kroff, Guzzardo Tamargo, Mazak and MC2016), the results of the present study concur with the majority of corpora studies which have reported that semantic gender is a strong predictor for gender assignment in spontaneously elicited codeswitched speech across Spanish–English bilingual communities in the U.S. (Clegg & Waltermire, Reference Clegg and Waltermire2009; Cruz, Reference Cruz2021; DuBord, Reference DuBord2004; Otheguy & Lapidus, Reference Ortheguy, Naomi Lapidus, Núñez-Cedeño, López and Cameron2003; Poplack et al., Reference Poplack, Pousada and Sankoff1982; Trawick & Bero, Reference Trawick and Bero2021). From a methodological perspective, the results suggest that human nouns should not be conflated with inanimate nouns in laboratory-based studies.

English inanimate nouns lack gender information altogether, and both corpora and experimental studies indicate that Spanish–English bilinguals gravitate toward a masculine default assignment strategy with such a set of nouns. Feminine analogical gender, on the other hand, is restricted to some English nouns with Spanish feminine translation equivalents (Cruz, Reference Cruz2021). The present study followed a balanced two-gender system equivalence with Spanish, giving participants the same chances to respond to masculine (with 20 items) and feminine gender (with 20 items) with inanimate nouns in the syntactic environments tested. However, the logistic regression model revealed that participants were significantly more likely to assign masculine gender compared to feminine gender to inanimate nouns occurring in Spanish det–English noun switches (Task 1). In the case of English–Spanish copula constructions (Task 2), no statistical significance for masculine gender was observed. That is, proportions of gender-congruent selections were relatively low for both masculine (78%) and feminine gender (62%) in Task 2, although masculine is still descriptively higher than feminine gender.

Assuming that analogical gender is at work in codeswitched speech, I took a step further and asked whether phonemic cues that strongly correlate with gender assignment in Spanish (e.g., the /o/-/a/ contrast) modulate the assignment mechanism in experimentally elicited responses to codeswitched speech. All target nouns were equally divided into ‘strong cues’ and ‘no cues’ relevant to masculine and feminine analogical gender on the basis of the phonemic make up of their Spanish translation equivalents. The statistical analysis revealed no interaction between analogical gender and the /o/-/a/ gender cues of the Spanish translation equivalents. Valenzuela et al. (Reference Valenzuela, Faure, Ramírez-Trujillo, Barski, Pangtay and Diez2012) and Denbaum and de Prada Pérez (Reference Denbaum and de Prada Pérez2020) also reported negative results on the potential influence of other-language phonemic cues – namely, the /o/-/a/ contrast – in modulating gender assignment in Spanish–English codeswitched speech. In short, the results indicate that the prevalence for a masculine default assignment strategy trumps the potential retrieval of analogical gender in experimentally elicited responses to codeswitched speech, even when the target nouns were equally divided for analogical gender based on the Spanish translation equivalents of the target English nouns.

The gender asymmetry observed in the present study – where feminine gender elicited relatively low proportions of gender-congruent selections and masculine gender exerted a default status in Spanish det–English noun switches – merits further exploration in terms of the codeswitching behavior of our bilingual sample. At first glance, the prevailing preference for masculine gender in Spanish det–English noun switches (Task 1) seems to reflect the default status of masculine gender in Spanish, or “preferences that are internal to Spanish” in Aaron's (Reference Aaron2015) terms (p. 470). However, and despite the fact that English lacks grammatical gender, the distributional patterns of gender assignment in other bilingual communities challenge this intuition. In other words, if the preference for masculine gender in Spanish–English codeswitched speech is mediated by a default assignment mechanism internal to the morphosyntax of gender in Spanish (see Harris, Reference Harris1991 for such mechanism), it is logical to expect masculine gender to be the preferred gender in codeswitching environments where only Spanish exhibits grammatical gender. This prediction, however, does not hold up across bilingual communities where Spanish is involved in codeswitching.

For instance, Parafita Couto et al. (Reference Parafita Couto, Munarriz, Epelde, Deuchar and and Oyharçabal2015) reported that feminine gender, and not masculine gender, is the preferred gender in Spanish–Basque spontaneous codeswitched speech – a language pair where only Spanish exhibits grammatical gender. Although there is some evidence indicating that masculine is the preferred gender in Purepecha–Spanish codeswitching where a Spanish adjective controls gender agreement (Bellamy, Parafita Couto & Stadthagen-Gonzalez, Reference Bellamy, Parafita Couto and Stadthagen-Gonzalez2018), the fact that feminine gender is the preferred gender in Spanish det–Basque noun switches indicates that bilingual communities adopt different assignment strategies.

In particular, in this paper I suggest that the prevailing preference for masculine gender in Spanish det–English noun switches (e.g., el wand ‘the.M’) is better attributed to the codeswitching norms of the well-defined codeswitching community of Southern Arizona where our participants live and work. In this Spanish–English bilingual community, masculine gender is overwhelmingly preferred with English nouns in spontaneously elicited codeswitched speech regardless of the gender of their Spanish translation equivalents (Cruz, Reference Cruz2021; DuBord, Reference DuBord2004). This interpretation is congenial with the burgeoning evidence indicating that codeswitching patterns across bilingual communities conform to community-based codeswitching norms (Beatty-Martínez & Dussias, Reference Beatty-Martínez and Dussias2017; Bellamy & Parafita Couto, Reference Bellamy, Parafita Couto and Ayoun2022; Królikowska, Bierings, Beatty-Martínez, Navarro-Torres, Dussias & Parafita Couto, Reference Królikowska, Bierings, Beatty-Martínez, Navarro-Torres, Dussias and Parafita Couto2019, cited in Beatty-Martínez & Dussias, Reference Beatty-Martinez and Dussias2019; Ramírez Urbaneja, Reference Ramírez Urbaneja2020; Torres Cacoullos & Travis, Reference Torres Cacoullos and Travis2018; Valdés Kroff, Reference Valdés Kroff, Guzzardo Tamargo, Mazak and MC2016). In other words, and granted that codeswitching at the determiner phrase is a suitable and preferred syntactic juncture for Spanish–English bilinguals to codeswitch in spontaneous speech, the preference for masculine gender at this syntactic juncture reflects community-driven codeswitching patterns rather than having been triggered by some internal mechanism of the morphosyntax of Spanish. This explanation points to the observation that feminine gender applies only when bilingual speakers reflect on the translation equivalent of an English noun occurring in codeswitched speech. The question of when, or in what context, bilinguals are more likely to reflect on the translation equivalent of an English noun occurring in codeswitched speech merits a careful investigation in laboratory-based research. I leave this question for future research.

Finally, this study also asked what similarities or differences might ensue from examining gender assignment in Spanish det–English noun switches (Task 1) versus English–Spanish copula constructions (Task 2). While gender assignment with human nouns was almost categorical in both tasks, proportions of gender-congruent selections for masculine gender reached statistical significance only in Task 1, but not in Task 2, with nouns that lack gender information altogether. The rationale for including switched copula constructions in the present study was in response to Valenzuela et al. (Reference Valenzuela, Faure, Ramírez-Trujillo, Barski, Pangtay and Diez2012) who reported higher proportions of feminine gender with Spanish copula constructions (71%) compared to Spanish det–English noun switches (56%). In the present study, proportions of feminine gender were equally low with inanimate nouns in both tasks (62%). Higher proportions of feminine gender in Valenzuela et al.'s study could be due to the fact that the entire copula construction was in Spanish (e.g., Está crudo/a ‘It's raw-M/F’), whereas the target English noun initiated the switched construction in the present study (e.g., The meat está crudo/a ‘is raw-M/F’). Furthermore, it is important to note that the Spanish–English bilinguals studied in Valenzuela et al.'s study lived and worked in Canada, whereas our participants lived and worked in the U.S. at the time of the present study.

How then can we interpret the discrepancy between Task 1 and Task 2 in terms of masculine gender in the present study? First, I should emphasize that codeswitching at copula constructions is rarely attested in Spanish–English spontaneous codeswitched speech (Pfaff, Reference Pfaff1979; Poplack, Reference Poplack1980; Woolford, Reference Woolford1983). Therefore, a plausible explanation for the observed discrepancy is that participants experienced more difficulty with Task 2 compared to Task 1. If this potential difficulty can be attributed to the fact that switched copula constructions are rarely attested in spontaneous bilingual speech, then it is reasonable to suggest that participants were actually deploying their codeswitching norms in Task 1, where a preference for a default gender in Spanish det–English noun switches was observed in line with the spontaneously elicited data from the same bilingual community. Reaction times of an online task could help us determine whether bilinguals indeed experienced more difficulty with Task 2 compared to Task 1. The tasks employed in the present study were untimed for ecological validity purposes, and this is a limitation to the present study. Future studies should determine whether reaction times would be informative for the study of gender assignment in experimentally elicited responses to codeswitched speech.

The results for Task 1 align with the gender assignment strategies observed in spontaneously elicited Spanish–English codeswitched speech from a well-defined codeswitching community in Southern Arizona, U.S. (Cruz, Reference Cruz2021; DuBord, Reference DuBord2004). The experimental stimuli in the present study were modeled after the codeswitching patterns from this bilingual community studied in Cruz (Reference Cruz2021). Participants were also recruited in Southern Arizona, and they reported to engage in habitual codeswitching practices themselves. Therefore, it is fair to suggest that an experimental design that reflects the codeswitching behavior of a well-defined codeswitching community can provide insightful information for a better characterization of the bilingual lexicon and the bilingual experience more generally. Future studies can apply an experimental approach to assess variation in codeswitching practices and language use across Spanish–English bilingual communities in the U.S. In particular, an experimental approach to the study of codeswitching can help us determine whether Spanish–English bilingual communities in the U.S. show similar or different gender assignment criteria in codeswitched speech on the basis of geographical proximity (e.g., Southern U.S. versus Northeastern U.S.). I hope future studies will shed some light in this direction.

Conclusion

The present study adopted an experimental design to examine gender assignment in Spanish–English codeswitched speech from a well-defined codeswitching community in Southern Arizona, U.S. Importantly, the experimental data corroborated the codeswitching patterns observed in naturally-occurring bilingual speech from the same bilingual community. I suggested that the prevailing preference for masculine gender in both naturally-occurring bilingual speech and experimentally elicited responses to codeswitched speech provides strong evidence of the codeswitching norms of this bilingual community. I emphasized the importance of applying experimental designs that reflect the codeswitching behavior of bilingual communities and the need to explore variation in codeswitching practices across Spanish–English bilingual communities on the basis of geographical proximity.

Acknowledgements

I would like to thank the participants of the study for their invaluable time and the reviewers for their insightful comments on how to improve the manuscript. I am thankful to the audience of the Bilingualism, Mind, and Brain Lab for their constructive comments and to Annie Beatty-Martínez and Laura Callahan for insightful comments on previous drafts of this paper. I am grateful to Lourdes Ortega, Ronald P. Leow, and José Camacho for their constructive feedback on this research project.

Competing interests

The author declares none.

Supplementary Material

For supplementary material accompanying this paper, visit https://doi.org/10.1017/S1366728922000839

Appendix S1: List of target and distractor items

Appendix S2: List of carrier sentences for Task 1 & Task 2

Footnotes

1 Following this definition of gender assignment, throughout this paper I may interchangeably refer to gender as either (a) how speakers assign a gender to nouns or (b) which nouns go to into which gender category.

References

Aaron, J (2015) Lone English-origin nouns in Spanish: The precedence of community norms. International Journal of Bilingualism 19, 459480. https://doi.org/10.1177/1367006913516021CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Alarcón, I (2011) Spanish gender agreement under complete and incomplete acquisition: Early and late bilinguals’ linguistic behavior within the noun phrase. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition 14, 332350. https://doi.org/10.1017/S1366728910000222CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Anderson, JAE, Mak, L, Keyvani Chahi, A and Bialystok, E (2018). The language and social background questionnaire: Assessing degree of bilingualism in a diverse population. Behavior Research Methods 50, 250263. https://doi.org/10.3758/s13428-017-0867-9CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Armon-Lotem, S and Meir, N (2019) The nature of exposure and input in early bilingualism. In De Houwer, A and Ortega, L (eds), The Cambridge handbook of bilingualism. New York, NY: Cambridge University Press, pp. 193212. https://doi.org/10.1017/9781316831922.011Google Scholar
Baayen, R, Davidson, D and Bates, D (2008) Mixed-effects modeling with crossed random effects for subjects and items. Journal of Memory and Language 59, 390412. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jml.2007.12.005CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Balam, O (2016) Semantic categories and gender assignment in contact Spanish: Type of code-switching and its relevance to linguistic outcomes. Journal of Language Contact 9, 405435. https://doi.org/10.1163/19552629-00903001CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Balam, O, Lakshmanan, U and Parafita Couto, MC (2021) Gender assignment strategies among simultaneous Spanish/English bilingual children from Miami, Florida. Studies in Hispanic and Lusophone Linguistics 14, 241280. https://doi.org/10.1515/shll-2021-2045CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bates, D, Maechler, M, Bolker, B and Walker, S (2015) Fitting linear mixed-effects models using lme4. Journal of Statistical Software 67, 148. https://doi.org/10.18637/jss.v067.i01CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Beatty-Martínez, A and Dussias, P (2017) Bilingual experience shapes language processing: Evidence from codeswitching. Journal of Memory and Language 95, 173189. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jml.2017.04.002CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Beatty-Martinez, A and Dussias, P (2019) Revisiting masculine and feminine grammatical gender in Spanish: Linguistic, psycholinguistic, and neurolinguistic evidence. Frontiers in Psychology 10, 751751. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2019.00751CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Beatty-Martínez, A, Valdés Kroff, JR and Dussias, P (2018) From the field to the lab: A converging methods approach to the study of codeswitching. Languages 3, 119. https://doi.org/10.3390/languages3020019CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Beatty-Martínez, A, Navarro-Torres, CA and Dussias, P (2020). Codeswitching: A bilingual toolkit for opportunistic speech planning. Frontiers in Psychology. 11, 112. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2020.01699CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bellamy, K, Parafita Couto, MC and Stadthagen-Gonzalez, H (2018) Investigating gender assignment strategies in mixed Purepecha–Spanish nominal constructions. Languages 3, 127. https://doi.org/10.3390/languages3030028CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bellamy, K and Parafita Couto, MC (2022) Gender assignment in mixed noun phases: State of the art. In Ayoun, D (ed), The Acquisition of Gender: Crosslinguistic perspectives. Amsterdam: John Benjamins. pp. 1348. https://doi.org/10.1075/sibil.63.02belCrossRefGoogle Scholar
Besset, R (2017) Exploring the phonological integration of lone other-language nouns in the Spanish of Southern Arizona. U. Penn Working Papers in Linguistics 23, 3139. https://repository.upenn.edu/pwpl/vol23/iss2/5/Google Scholar
Bowden, H (2016) Assessing second-language oral proficiency for research: The Spanish Elicited Imitation Task. Studies in Second Language Acquisition 38, 647675. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0272263115000443CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bullock, B and Toribio, A (2009) Themes in the study of code-switching. In Bullock, B and Toribio, A (eds), The Cambridge handbook of linguistic code-switching. New York, NY: Cambridge University Press, pp. 118. https://doi.org/10.1017/CBO9780511576331.002CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Carvalho, A (2012) Corpus del Español en el Sur de Arizona (CESA). University of Arizona: https://cesa.arizona.edu/Google Scholar
Casillas, JV (2013) La fricativización del africado /tʃ/: actitudes lingüísticas cerca de la frontera. In Carvalho, A and Beaudrie, S (eds), Selected Proceedings of the 6th Workshop on Spanish Sociolinguistics. Summerville, MA: Cascadilla Press, pp. 177188. http://www.lingref.com/cpp/wss/6/paper2867.pdfGoogle Scholar
Clegg, J and Waltermire, M (2009) Gender assignment to English-origin nouns in the Spanish of the Southern United States. Southwest Journal of Linguistics 28, 117. http://search.proquest.com/docview/85708379/Google Scholar
Comrie, B (1999) Grammatical gender systems: A linguist's assessment. Journal of Psycholinguist Research 28, 457466. https://doi.org/10.1023/A:1023212225540CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Corbett, G (1991) Gender. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. https://doi.org/10.1017/CBO9781139166119CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Cox, JG, LaBoda, A and Mendes, N (2020) “I'm gonna Spanglish it on you”: Self-reported vs. oral production of Spanish-English codeswitching. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition 23, 446458. https://doi.org/10.1017/S1366728919000129CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Cruz, A (2021) A syntactic approach to gender assignment in Spanish–English bilingual speech. Glossa: A journal of general linguistics 6, 140. https://doi.org/10.16995/glossa.5878CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Cruz, A (2016) The Spanish discourse marker o sea in the speech of bilinguals from Southern Arizona. Revista de Estudios Lingüísticos y Literarios 14, 7081 https://divergencias.arizona.edu/sites/divergencias.arizona.edu/files/articles/The%20Spanish%20Discourse%20Marker%20%27o%20sea%27%20in%20Southern%20Arizona%20Spanish_Divergencias.pdfGoogle Scholar
Cruz, A (2018) The past persists into the present: a multivariate analysis of Present Perfect in Southern Arizona Spanish. In MacDonald, J (ed), Contemporary Trends in Hispanic and Lusophone Linguistics: Selected Papers from the Hispanic Linguistics Symposium 2015. Amsterdam: John Benjamins, pp. 169190. https://doi.org/10.1075/ihll.15.09cruCrossRefGoogle Scholar
Delgado, R (2018) The familiar and the strange: Gender assignment in Spanish/English mixed DPs. In López, L (ed), Code-switching-Experimental Answers to Theoretical Questions: In honor of Kay González-Vilbazo. Amsterdam: John Benjamins, pp. 3962. https://doi.org/10.1075/ihll.19.03delCrossRefGoogle Scholar
Denbaum, N and de Prada Pérez, A (2020) How do Spanish heritage speakers in the US assign gender to English nouns in Spanish-English code-switching?: The effect of noun canonicity and codeswitcher type. Linguistic Approaches to Bilingualism 1, 127. https://doi.org/10.1075/lab.19029.denGoogle Scholar
Deuchar, M, Davies, P, Herring, J, Parafita Couto, MC, Carter, D (2014) Building bilingual corpora. In Thomas, EM and Mennen, I (eds), Advances in the Study of Bilingualism. Bristol: Multilingualism Matters, pp. 93111.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
DuBord, E (2004) Gender assignment to English words in the Spanish of Southern Arizona. Divergencias. Revista de estudios lingüísticos y literarios 2, 2740. https://scholarworks.uni.edu/cgi/viewcontent.cgi?article=1001&context=ll_facpubGoogle Scholar
Duchon, A, Perea, M, Sebastián-Gallés, N, Martí, A and Carreiras, M (2013) EsPal: One-stop shopping for Spanish word properties. Behavior Research Methods 45, 12461258. https://doi.org/10.3758/s13428-013-0326-1CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Fairchild, S and Van Hell, J (2017) Determiner-noun code-switching in Spanish heritage speakers. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition 20, 150161. https://doi.org/10.1017/S1366728915000619CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gullberg, M, Indefrey, P and Muysken, P (2009) Research techniques for the study of code-switching. In Bullock, B and Toribio, A (eds), The Cambridge handbook of linguistic code-switching. New York, NY: Cambridge University Press, pp. 2139.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Guzzardo Tamargo, RE, Valdés Kroff, JR and Dussias, PE (2016) Examining the relationship between comprehension and production processes in code-switched language. Journal of Memory and Language 89, 138–161. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jml.2015.12.002CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Harris, J (1991) The exponence of gender in Spanish. Linguistic Inquiry 22, 2762.Google Scholar
Herring, JR, Deuchar, M, Parafita Couto, MC and Moro Quintanilla, M (2010). ‘I saw the madre‘: evaluating predictions about codeswitched determiner-noun sequences using Spanish–English and Welsh–English data. International Journal of Bilingual Education and Bilingualism 13, 553573. https://doi.org/10.1080/13670050.2010.488286CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Jaeger, T (2008) Categorical data analysis: Away from ANOVAs (transformation or not) and towards logit mixed models. Journal of Memory and Language 59, 434446. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jml.2007.11.007CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Jake, J, Myers-Scotton, C and Gross, S (2002) Making a minimalist approach to codeswitching work: Adding the Matrix Language. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition 5, 6991. https://doi.org/10.1017/S1366728902000147CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kern, J (2019) Like in English and como, como que, and like in Spanish in the speech of Southern Arizona bilinguals. International Journal of Bilingualism 24, 184207. https://doi.org/10.1177/1367006919826329CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kramer, R (2015) The Morphosyntax of Gender. Oxford: Oxford University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Królikowska, M, Bierings, E, Beatty-Martínez, A, Navarro-Torres, C, Dussias, P and Parafita Couto, MC (2019) Gender-assignment strategies within the bilingual determiner phrase: Four Spanish-English communities examined. Poster presentation for Bilingualism in the Hispanic and Lusophone World 219. Leiden, NL, 9-11 January.Google Scholar
Lindsey, B and Gerken, L (2012) The role of morphophonological regularity in young Spanish-speaking children's production of gendered noun phrases. Journal of Child Language 39, 753776. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0305000911000250CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Mariscal, S (2008) Early acquisition of gender agreement in the Spanish noun phrase: starting small. Journal of Child Language 36, 143171. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0305000908008908CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
McConnell-Ginet, S (2013) Gender and its relation to sex: The myth of ‘natural’ gender. In Corbett, G (ed), The Expression of Gender. Berlin: De Gruyter Mouton, pp. 338. https://doi.org/10.1515/9783110307337.3CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Montrul, S, Foote, R and Perpiñán, S (2008) Gender agreement in adult second language learners and Spanish heritage speakers: The effects of age and context of acquisition. Language Learning 58, 503553. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-9922.2008.00449.xCrossRefGoogle Scholar
Munarriz-Ibarrola, A, Ezeizabarrena, M-J, de Castro Arrazola, V and Parafita Couto, MC (2021) Gender assignment strategies and L1 effects in the elicited production of mixed Spanish-Basque DPs. Linguistic Approaches to Bilingualism 11, 138. https://doi.org/10.1075/lab.20016.munGoogle Scholar
Ortega, L (2020) The study of heritage language development from a bilingualism and social justice perspective. Language Learning 70, 1553. https://doi.org/10.1111/lang.12347CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ortega, L, Iwashita, N, Norris, JM and Rabie, S (2002) An investigation of elicited imitation tasks in cross-linguistic SLA research. Paper presented at the 20th Second Language Research Forum, Toronto, October 3–6 [Unpublished handout retrieved from IRIS].Google Scholar
Ortheguy, R and Naomi Lapidus, S (2003) An adaptive approach to noun gender in New York contact Spanish. In Núñez-Cedeño, R, López, L and Cameron, R (eds), A Romance perspective on language knowledge and use. Amsterdam: John Benjamins, pp. 209229. https://doi.org/10.1075/cilt.238.17othCrossRefGoogle Scholar
Parafita Couto, MC, Munarriz, A, Epelde, I, Deuchar, M and Oyharçabal, , B (2015) Gender conflict resolution in Spanish-Basque mixed DPs. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition 18, 304323. https://doi.org/10.1017/S136672891400011XCrossRefGoogle Scholar
Park, HI, Solon, M, Henderson, C and Dehghan-Chaleshtori, M (2020) The roles of working memory and oral language abilities in elicited imitation performance. The Modern Language Journal 104, 133151. https://doi.org/10.1111/modl.12618CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Peirce, J, Gray, J, Simpson, S, MacAskill, M, Höchenberger, R, Sogo, H, Kastman, E and Lindeløv, J (2019) PsychoPy2: Experiments in behavior made easy. Behavior Research Methods 51, 195203. https://doi.org/10.3758/s13428-018-01193-yCrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Pérez-Pereira, M (1991) The acquisition of gender: What Spanish children tell us. Journal of Child Language 18, 571–90. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0305000900011259CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Pfaff, C (1979) Functional and structural constraints on syntactic variation in code-switching. Language 55, 297318. https://doi.org/10.2307/412586Google Scholar
Poplack, S (1980) Sometimes I'll start a sentence in Spanish y TERMINO EN ESPAÑOL: toward a typology of code-switching. Linguistics 18, 581618. https://doi.org/10.1515/ling.1980.18.7-8.581CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Poplack, S, Pousada, A and Sankoff, D (1982) Competing influence on gender assignment: Variable process, stable outcome. Lingua 57, 128. https://doi.org/10.1016/0024-3841(82)90068-7CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ramírez Urbaneja, D (2020) “¿Tú tienes una little pumpkin?” Mixed noun phrases in Spanish-English bilingual children and adults. International Journal of Bilingualism 24, 824839. https://doi.org/10.1177/1367006919888580CrossRefGoogle Scholar
R Core Team (2018) R: A language and environment for statistical computing [Computer software manual]. Vienna, Austria. Retrieved from https://www.r-project.org.Google Scholar
Sommet, N and Morselli, D (2017) Keep calm and learn multilevel logistic modeling: A simplified three-step procedure using Stata, R, Mplus, and SPSS. International Review of Social Psychology 30, 203218. https://doi.org/10.5334/irsp.90CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Teschner, R and Russell, W (1984) The gender patterns of Spanish nouns: An inverse dictionary-based analysis. Hispanic Linguistics 1, 115132.Google Scholar
Torres Cacoullos, R and Travis, CE (2018) Bilingualism in the Community: Code-Switching And Grammars In Contact. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Trawick, S and Bero, T (2021) Gender assignment: Monolingual constraints contribute to a bilingual outcome. International Journal of Bilingualism 26, 117. https://doi.org/10.1177/13670069211053466Google Scholar
Valdés Kroff, JR, Dussias, P, Gerfen, C, Perrotti, L and Bajo, , M (2017) Experience with code-switching modulates the use of grammatical gender during sentence processing. Linguistic Approaches to Bilingualism 7, 163198. https://doi.org/10.1075/lab.15010.valCrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Valdés Kroff, , JR (2016) Mixed NPs in Spanish-English bilingual speech: Using a corpus-based approach to inform models of sentence processing. In Guzzardo Tamargo, R, Mazak, C and MC, Parafita Couto (eds), Spanish-English Codeswitching in the Caribbean and the US. Amsterdam: John Benjamins, pp. 281300. https://doi.org/10.1075/ihll.11.12valCrossRefGoogle Scholar
Valdés Kroff, JR and Fernández-Duque, , M (2017) Experimentally inducing Spanish-English code-switching. In Bellamy, K, Child, MW, González, P, Muntendam, A and Parafita Couto, MC (eds), Multidisciplinary Approaches to Bilingualism in the Hispanic and Lusophone World. Amsterdam: John Benjamins, pp. 211231. https://doi.org/10.1075/ihll.13.09valCrossRefGoogle Scholar
Valenzuela, E, Faure, A, Ramírez-Trujillo, A, Barski, E, Pangtay, Y and Diez, A (2012) Gender and heritage Spanish bilingual grammars: A study of code-mixed determiner phrases and copula constructions. Hispania 95, 481494.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Woolford, E (1983) Bilingual code-switching and syntactic theory. Linguistic Inquiry 14, 520536. https://www.jstor.org/stable/4178342Google Scholar
Wu, S-L., Tio, YP and Ortega, L (2022) Elicited imitation as a measure of L2 proficiency: New insights from a comparison of two L2 English parallel forms. Studies in Second Language Acquisition 44, 271300. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0272263121000103CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Zentella, AC (1997) Growing up Bilingual: Puerto Rican children in New York. Oxford, UK/Malden, MA: Blackwell.Google Scholar
Figure 0

Table 1. Characteristics of Spanish–English early bilinguals

Figure 1

Table 2. Proportions of gender-congruent selections per condition for Task 1

Figure 2

Table 3. Summary of mixed logistic regression model for variables predicting participants’ likelihood of gender-congruent selections for Task 1

Figure 3

Table 4. Proportions of gender-congruent selections per condition for Task 2

Figure 4

Table 5. Summary of mixed-logistic regression model for variables predicting participants’ likelihood of gender-congruent selections for Task 2

Supplementary material: File

Cruz supplementary material

Cruz supplementary material 1

Download Cruz supplementary material(File)
File 13.6 KB
Supplementary material: File

Cruz supplementary material

Cruz supplementary material 2

Download Cruz supplementary material(File)
File 15.2 KB