Hostname: page-component-cd9895bd7-lnqnp Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-12-25T09:03:09.826Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

The effects of aging on bilingual language: What changes, what doesn't, and why

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  26 August 2020

Jana Reifegerste*
Affiliation:
Potsdam Research Institute for Multilingualism, University of Potsdam, Germany Brain and Language Laboratory, Department of Neuroscience, Georgetown University, WashingtonDC, USA
*
Address for correspondence: Jana Reifegerste, Email: [email protected]

Abstract

Substantial research has examined cognition in aging bilinguals. However, less work has investigated the effects of aging on language itself in bilingualism. In this article I comprehensively review prior research on this topic, and interpret the evidence in light of current theories of aging and theories of bilingualism. First, aging indeed appears to affect bilinguals’ language performance, though there is considerable variability in the trajectory across adulthood (declines, age-invariance, and improvements) and in the extent to which these trajectories resemble those found in monolinguals. I argue that these age effects are likely explained by the key opposing forces of increasing experience and cognitive declines in aging. Second, consistent with some theoretical work on bilingual language processing, the grammatical processing mechanisms do not seem to change between younger and older bilingual adults, even after decades of immersion. I conclude by discussing how future research can further advance the field.

Type
Review Article
Copyright
Copyright © The Author(s), 2020. Published by Cambridge University Press

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

Abutalebi, J and Green, DW (2016) Neuroimaging of language control in bilinguals: neural adaptation and reserve. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 19, 689698. https://doi.org/10.1017/S1366728916000225CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Abutalebi, J, Guidi, L, Borsa, VM, Canini, M, Della Rosa, PA, Parris, BA and Weekes, BS (2015) Bilingualism provides a neural reserve for aging populations. Neuropsychologia, 69, 201210. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2015.01.040CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Allen, PA, Madden, DJ and Crozier, LC (1991) Adult Age Differences in Letter-Level and Word-Level Processing. Psychology and Aging, 6, 261271. https://doi.org/10.1037//0882-7974.6.2.261CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Allen, PA, Madden, DJ, Weber, TA and Groth, KE (1993) Influence of age and processing stage on visual word recognition. Psychology and Aging, 8, 274282.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Allen, PA, Sliwinski, MJ and Bowie, T (2002) Differential Age Effects in Semantic and Episodic Memory, Part II: Slope and Intercept Analyses. Experimental Aging Research, 28, 111142.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Altmann, LJP and Kemper, S (2006) Effects of age, animacy and activation order on sentence production. Language and Cognitive Processes, 21, 322354. https://doi.org/10.1080/0169096054400006CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Anderson, JAE, Saleemi, S and Bialystok, E (2017) Neuropsychological assessments of cognitive aging in monolingual and bilingual older adults. Journal of Neurolinguistics, 43, 1727. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jneuroling.2016.08.001CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Antón, E, García, YF, Carreiras, M and Duñabeitia, JA (2016) Does bilingualism shape inhibitory control in the elderly? Journal of Memory and Language, 90, 147160. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jml.2016.04.007CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Baayen, H, Vasishth, S, Kliegl, R and Bates, D (2017) The cave of shadows: Addressing the human factor with generalized additive mixed models. Journal of Memory and Language, 94, 206234. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jml.2016.11.006CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bak, TH, Nissan, JJ, Allerhand, MM and Deary, IJ (2014) Does bilingualism influence cognitive aging? Annals of Neurology, 75, 959963. https://doi.org/10.1002/ana.24158CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bak, TH, Vega-Mendoza, M and Sorace, A (2014) Never too late? An advantage on tests of auditory attention extends to late bilinguals. Frontiers in Psychology, 5, 16. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2014.00485CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Baker, C and Prys Jones, S (1998) Encyclopedia of Bilingualism and Bilingual Education. Clevedon, UK: Multilingual Matters.Google Scholar
Balota, DA, Cortese, MJ, Sergent-Marshall, SD, Spieler, DH and Yap, MJ (2004) Visual Word Recognition of Single-Syllable Words. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 133, 283316. https://doi.org/10.1037/0096-3445.133.2.283CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Banks, B, Gowen, E, Munro, KJ and Adank, P (2015) Cognitive predictors of perceptual adaptation to accented speech: Cognitive predictors of perceptual adaptation to accented speech. The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 137, 20152024. https://doi.org/10.1121/1.4916265CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Barresi, BA, Nicholas, M, Connor, LT, Obler, LK and Albert, ML (2000) Semantic degradation and lexical access in age-related naming failures. Aging, Neuropsychology, and Cognition, 7(3), 169178. https://doi.org/10.1076/1382-5585(200009)7:3;1-Q;FT169CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bel, A, Sagarra, N, Comínguez, JP and García-Alcaraz, E (2016) Transfer and proficiency effects in L2 processing of subject anaphora. Lingua, 184, 134159. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.lingua.2016.07.001CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bialystok, E, Craik, FIM and Klein, R (2004) Bilingualism, Aging, and Cognitive Control: Evidence From the Simon Task. Psychology and Aging, 19, 290303. https://doi.org/10.1037/0882-7974.19.2.290CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bialystok, E, Craik, FIM and Luk, G (2008a) Cognitive Control and Lexical Access in Younger and Older Bilinguals. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 34, 859873. https://doi.org/10.1037/0278-7393.34.4.859Google Scholar
Bialystok, E, Craik, FIM and Luk, G (2008b) Lexical access in bilinguals: Effects of vocabulary size and executive control. Journal of Neurolinguistics, 21, 522538. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jneuroling.2007.07.001CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bialystok, E, Craik, FIM and Luk, G (2013) Bilingualism: Consequences for Mind and Brain. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 16, 240250. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tics.2012.03.001CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bialystok, E and Luk, G (2012) Receptive vocabulary differences in monolingual and bilingual adults. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 15, 397401. https://doi.org/10.1017/S136672891100040XCrossRefGoogle Scholar
Birdsong, D (2014) Dominance and Age in Bilingualism. Applied Linguistics, 35, 374392. https://doi.org/10.1093/applin/amu031CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Birdsong, D and Flege, JE (2001) Regular-Irregular Dissociations in L2 Acquisition of English Morphology. In BUCLD 25: Proceedings of the 25th Annual Boston University Conference on Language Development. Boston, MA: Cascadilla Press, pp. 123132.Google Scholar
Blackwell, A and Bates, E (1995) Inducing Agrammatic Profiles in Normals: Evidence for the Selective Vulnerability of Morphology under Cognitive Resource Limitation. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 7, 228257.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Blom, E and Baayen, HR (2013) The impact of verb form, sentence position, home language, and second language proficiency on subject–verb agreement in child second language Dutch. Applied Psycholinguistics, 34, 777811.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bock, K (2004) Psycholinguistically speaking: Some matters of meaning, marking and morphing. In Ross, BH (ed), The Psychology of Learning and Motivation (Vol. 44). San Diego, CA: Elsevier, pp. 109144 https://doi.org/10.1016/S0079-7421(03)44004-8.Google Scholar
Bock, K and Cutting, JC (1992) Regulating mental energy: Performance units in language production. Journal of Memory and Language, 31, 99127. https://doi.org/10.1016/0749-596X(92)90007-KCrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bock, K and Eberhard, KM (1993) Meaning, Sound and Syntax in English Number Agreement. Language and Cognitive Processes, 8, 5799. https://doi.org/10.1080/01690969308406949CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bock, K and Miller, CA (1991) Broken Agreement. Cognitive Psychology, 23, 4593.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Borsa, VM, Perani, D, Della Rosa, PA, Videsott, G, Guidi, L, Weekes, BS and Abutalebi, J (2018) Bilingualism and healthy aging: Aging effects and neural maintenance. Neuropsychologia, 111, 5161. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2018.01.012CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bowden, HW, Steinhauer, K, Sanz, C and Ullman, MT (2013) Native-like brain processing of syntax can be attained by university foreign language learners. Neuropsychologia, 51, 24922511. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2013.09.004CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Breadmore, HL (2007) Inflectional morphology in the literacy of deaf children. Ph.D. dissertation, University of Birmingham.Google Scholar
Brickman, AM, Paul, RH, Cohen, RA, Williams, LM, Macgregor, KL, Jefferson, AL and Gordon, E (2005) Category and letter verbal fluency across the adult lifespan: relationship to EEG theta power. Archives of Clinical Neuropsychology, 20, 561573. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.acn.2004.12.006CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bruhn De Garavito, J and White, L (2002) The second language acquisition of Spanish DPs: The status of grammatical features. In Pérez-Leroux, AT and Liceras, JM (eds), The Acquisition of Spanish Morphosyntax. Dordrecht: Springer, pp. 153178 https://doi.org/10.1007/978-94-010-0291-2.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Brysbaert, M and Nazir, TA (2005) Visual constraints in written word recognition: evidence from the optimal viewing-position effect. Journal of Research in Reading, 28, 216228.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Burke, DM and Osborne, G (2007) Aging and Inhibition Deficits: Where are the Effects? In Gorfein, D and MacLeod, C (eds), On the place of inhibitory processes in cognition. Washington, DC: American Psychological Association Press, pp. 163183.Google Scholar
Burke, DM and Shafto, MA (2008) Language and aging. In Craik, FIM and Salthouse, TA (eds), The handbook of aging and cognition. New York: Psychology Press, pp. 373443 https://doi.org/10.1097/00011363-198109000-00010.Google Scholar
Carpenter, PA, Miyake, A and Just, MA (1994) Working memory constraints in comprehension: Evidence from individual difference, aphasia and aging. In Gemsbacher, MA (ed), Handbook of psycholinguistics. New York, NY: Academic Press, pp. 10751122.Google Scholar
Carroll, R, Warzybok, A, Kollmeier, B and Ruigendijk, E (2016) Age-Related Differences in Lexical Access Relate to Speech Recognition in Noise. Frontiers in Psychology, 7, 990. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2016.00990CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Caza, N and Moscovitch, M (2005) Effects of cumulative frequency, but not of frequency trajectory, in lexical decision times of older adults and patients with Alzheimer's disease. Journal of Memory and Language, 53, 456471.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Chamorro, G, Sorace, A and Sturt, P (2016) What is the source of L1 attrition? The effect of recent L1 re-exposure on Spanish speakers under L1 attrition. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 19, 520532. https://doi.org/10.1017/S1366728915000152CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Chen, L, Shu, H, Liu, Y, Zhao, J and Li, P (2007) ERP signatures of subject–verb agreement in L2 learning. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 10, 161174. https://doi.org/10.1017/S136672890700291XCrossRefGoogle Scholar
Clahsen, H (1999) Lexical entries and rules of language: A multidisciplinary study of German inflection. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 22, 9911060. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0140525X99002228CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Clahsen, H, Eisenbeiss, S and Sonnenstuhl, I (1997) Morphological structure and the processing of inflected words. Theoretical Linguistics, 23, 201249. https://doi.org/10.1515/thli.1997.23.3.201CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Clahsen, H and Felser, C (2006a) Continuity and shallow structures in language processing. Applied Psycholinguistics, 27, 107126.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Clahsen, H and Felser, C (2006b) Grammatical processing in language learners. Applied Psycholinguistics, 27, 342. https://doi.org/10.1017.S0142716406060024CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Clahsen, H and Felser, C (2006c) How native-like is non-native language processing? Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 10, 564570. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tics.2006.10.002CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Clahsen, H and Felser, C (2018) Some notes on the shallow structure hypothesis. Studies in Second Language Acquisition, 40, 693706.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Clahsen, H and Reifegerste, J (2017) Morphological processing in old-age bilinguals. In Libben, M, Goral, M and Libben, G (eds), Bilingualism: A Framework for Understanding the Mental Lexicon. Amsterdam: Benjamins, pp. 217248CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Connor, LT, Spiro, A, Obler, LK and Albert, ML (2004) Change in Object Naming Ability During Adulthood. The Journals of Gerontology Series B: Psychological Sciences and Social Sciences, 59, P203P209. https://doi.org/10.1093/geronb/59.5.P203CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Coppens, KM, Tellings, A, Van der Veld, W and Schreuder, R (2012) Vocabulary development in children with hearing loss: The role of child, family, and educational variables. Research in Developmental Disabilities, 33, 119128. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.ridd.2011.08.030CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Coughlin, CE and Tremblay, A (2013) Proficiency and working memory based explanations for nonnative speakers’ sensitivity to agreement in sentence processing. Applied Psycholinguistics, 34, 615646. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0142716412000616CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Covington, NV and Duff, MC (2016) Expanding the Language Network: Direct Contributions from the Hippocampus. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 20, 869870. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tics.2016.10.006CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cox, SR, Bak, TH, Allerhand, M, Redmond, P, Starr, JM, Deary, IJ and MacPherson, SE (2016) Bilingualism, social cognition and executive functions: A tale of chickens and eggs. Neuropsychologia, 91, 299306. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2016.08.029CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cunnings, I (2017) Parsing and Working memory in bilingual sentence Processing. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 20, 659678. https://doi.org/10.1017/S1366728916000675CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Davidson, DJ, Zacks, RT and Ferreira, F (2003) Age preservation of the syntactic processor in production. Journal of Psycholinguistic Research, 32, 541566. https://doi.org/10.1023/A:1025402517111CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Davis, MH and Gaskell, MG (2009) A complementary systems account of word learning: Neural and behavioural evidence. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 364, 37733800. https://doi.org/10.1098/rstb.2009.0111CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
De Chastelaine, M, Mattson, JT, Wang, TH, Donley, BE and Rugg, MD (2015) Sensitivity of Negative Subsequent Memory and Task-Negative Effects to Age and Associative Memory Performance. Brain Research, 1612, 1629. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.brainres.2014.09.045CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
De Chastelaine, M, Mattson, JT, Wang, TH, Donley, BE and Rugg, MD (2016) The neural correlates of recollection and retrieval monitoring: relationships with age and recollection performance. NeuroImage, 138, 164175. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2016.04.071.TheCrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
DeKeyser, RM (2005) What Makes Learning Second-Language Grammar Difficult? A Review of Issues. Language Learning, 55, 125.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Diaz, MT, Rizio, AA and Zhuang, J (2016) The neural language systems that support healthy aging: Integrating function, structure, and behavior. Language and Linguistics Compass, 10, 314334. https://doi.org/10.1586/14737175.2015.1028369CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Diependaele, K, Lemhöfer, K and Brysbaert, M (2013) The word frequency effect in first- and second-language word recognition: A lexical entrenchment account. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 66, 843863. https://doi.org/10.1080/17470218.2012.720994CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Dobbs, AR and Rule, BG (1989) Adult age differences in working memory. Psychology and Aging, 4, 500503. https://doi.org/10.1037/0882-7974.4.4.500CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
du Boisgueheneuc, F, Levy, R, Volle, E, Seassau, M, Duffau, H, Kinkingnehun, S and Dubois, B (2006) Functions of the left superior frontal gyrus in humans: A lesion study. Brain, 129, 33153328. https://doi.org/10.1093/brain/awl244CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Duchek, JM, Balota, DA, Storandt, M and Larsen, R (2007) The Power of Personality in Discriminating Between Healthy Aging and Early-Stage Alzheimer's Disease. Journal of Gerontology: Psychological Sciences, 62B, P353P361.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Duff, MC and Brown-Schmidt, S (2012) The hippocampus and the flexible use and processing of language. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience, 6, 89. https://doi.org/10.3389/fnhum.2012.00069CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Duff, MC and Brown-Schmidt, S (2017) Hippocampal Contributions to Language Use and Processing Hippocampal Contributions to Language Use and Processing. In Hannula, DE and Duff, MC (eds), The hippocampus from cells to systems. Springer, pp. 503536 https://doi.org/10.1007/978-3-319-50406-3.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Dussias, PE, Valdés Kroff, JR, Guzzardo Tamargo, RE and Gerfen, C (2013) When Gender and Looking Go Hand in Hand. Studies in Second Language Acquisition, 35, 353387. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0272263112000915CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Eberhard, KM (1997) The Marked Effect of Number on Subject–Verb Agreement. Journal of Memory and Language, 36, 147164.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Eberhard, KM, Cutting, JC and Bock, K (2005) Making Syntax of Sense: Number Agreement in Sentence Production. Psychological Review, 112, 531559. https://doi.org/10.1037/0033-295X.112.3.000CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Facal, D, Juncos-Rabadán, O, Rodríguez, MS and Pereiro, AX (2012) Tip-of-the-tongue in aging: influence of vocabulary, working memory and processing speed. Aging Clinical and Experimental Research, 24, 647656. https://doi.org/10.3275/8586Google ScholarPubMed
Federmeier, KD, McLennan, DB and De Ochoa, E (2002) The impact of semantic memory organization and sentence context information on spoken language processing by younger and older adults: An ERP study. Psychophysiology, 39, 133146.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Felser, C and Roberts, L (2007) Processing wh-dependencies in a second language: a cross-modal priming study. Second Language Research, 23, 936.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Felser, C, Roberts, L, Marinis, T and Gross, R (2003) The processing of ambiguous sentences by first and second language learners of English. Applied Psycholinguistics, 24, 453489.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Felser, C, Sato, M and Bertenshaw, N (2009) The on-line application of binding Principle A in English as a second language. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 12, 485502. https://doi.org/10.1017/S1366728909990228CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Feyereisen, P (1997) A Meta-Analytic Procedure Shows an Age-Related Decline in Picture Naming. Journal of Speech Language and Hearing Research, 40, 1328. https://doi.org/10.1044/jslhr.4006.1328CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Feyereisen, P, Demaeght, N and Samson, D (1998) Why Do Picture Naming Latencies Increase With Age: General Slowing, Greater Sensitivity to Interference, or Task-Specific Deficits? Experimental Aging Research, 24, 2151. https://doi.org/10.1080/036107398244346CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Finkel, D, Reynolds, CA, Mcardle, JJ and Pedersen, NL (2007) Age Changes in Processing Speed as a Leading Indicator of Cognitive Aging. Psychology and Aging, 22, 558568. https://doi.org/10.1037/0882-7974.22.3.558CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Fjell, AM, Sneve, MH, Grydeland, H, Storsve, AB and Walhovd, KB (2017) The Disconnected Brain and Executive Function Decline in Aging. Cerebral Cortex, 27, 23032317. https://doi.org/10.1093/cercor/bhw082Google Scholar
Flege, JE, Yeni-Komshian, GH and Liu, S (1999) Age Constraints on Second-Language Acquisition. Journal of Memory and Language, 41, 78104. https://doi.org/10.1006/jmla.1999.2638CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Foos, PW (1989) Adult Age Differences in Working Memory. Psychology and Aging, 4, 269275.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Foote, R (2010) Age of acquisition and proficiency as factors in language production: Agreement in bilinguals. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 13, 99118. https://doi.org/10.1017/S136672890999040XCrossRefGoogle Scholar
Foote, R (2011) Integrated knowledge of agreement in early and late English–Spanish bilinguals. Applied Psycholinguistics, 32, 187220. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0142716410000342CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Frensch, P and Miner, CS (1994) Effects of presentation rate and individual differences in short-term memory capacity on an indirect measure of serial learning. Memory & Cognition, 22, 95110. https://doi.org/10.3758/BF03202765CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Friederici, AD, Steinhauer, K and Pfeifer, E (2002) Brain signatures of artificial language processing: Evidence challenging the critical period hypothesis. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 99, 529534.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gathercole, VCM, Thomas, EM, Kennedy, I, Prys, C, Young, N, Viñas Guasch, N and Jones, L (2014) Does language dominance affect cognitive performance in bilinguals? Lifespan evidence from preschoolers through older adults on card sorting, Simon, and metalinguistic tasks. Frontiers in Psychology, 5, 11. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2014.00011CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gilhooly, KJ (1984) Word Age-of-Acquisition and Residence Time in Lexical Memory as Factors in Word Naming. Current Psychological Research & Reviews, 3, 2431.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gold, BT, Kim, C, Johnson, NF, Kryscio, RJ and Smith, CD (2013) Lifelong bilingualism maintains neural efficiency for cognitive control in aging. Journal of Neuroscience, 33, 387396. https://doi.org/10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3837-12.2013CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gollan, TH, Montoya, RI, Cera, C and Sandoval, TC (2008) More use almost always a means a smaller frequency effect: Aging, bilingualism, and the weaker links hypothesis. Journal of Memory and Language, 58, 787814. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jml.2007.07.001CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gollan, TH, Weissberger, GH, Runnqvist, E, Montoya, RI and Cera, CM (2012) Self-ratings of spoken language dominance: A Multilingual Naming Test (MINT) and preliminary norms for young and aging Spanish–English bilinguals. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 15, 594615. https://doi.org/10.1017/S1366728911000332CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gomez, R (2002) Word Frequency Effects in Priming Performance in Young and Older Adults. Journal of Gerontology: Psychological Sciences, 57B, 233240.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Goral, M (2004) First-language decline in healthy aging: implications for attrition in bilingualism. Journal of Neurolinguistics, 17, 3152. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0911-6044(03)00052-6CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Goral, M, Libben, G, Obler, LK, Jarema, G and Ohayon, K (2008) Lexical attrition in younger and older bilingual adults. Clinical Linguistics & Phonetics, 22, 509522. https://doi.org/10.1080/02699200801912237CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Green, DW (2003) Neural basis of lexicon and grammar in L2 acquisition: The convergence hypothesis. In Hout, Rv, Hulk, A, Kuiken, F and Towell, R (eds), The interface between syntax and the lexicon in second language acquisition. Amsterdam: John Benjamins, pp. 197208.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Green, DW and Abutalebi, J (2013) Language control in bilinguals: The adaptive control hypothesis. Journal of Cognitive Psychology, 25, 515530. https://doi.org/10.1080/20445911.2013.796377CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Grognet, AG (1997) Elderly refugees and language learning. Aging, 359, 811.Google Scholar
Grosjean, F (2010) Bilingual: Life and Reality. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Grüter, T, Lew-Williams, C and Fernald, A (2012) Grammatical gender in L2: A production or a real-time processing problem? Second Language Research, 28, 191215. https://doi.org/10.1177/0267658312437990CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hamrick, P, Lum, JAG and Ullman, MT (2018) Child first language and adult second language are both tied to general-purpose learning systems. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 115, 14871492. https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.1713975115CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Han, Y and Ellis, R (1998) Implicit knowledge, explicit knowledge and general language proficiency. Language Teaching Research, 2, 123. https://doi.org/10.1177/136216889800200102CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Harris, RJ and Nelson McGhee, EM (1992) Bilingualism: Not the Exception Any More. In Harris, RJ (ed), Advances in Psychology (Volume 83). Amsterdam: Elsevier, pp. 314.Google Scholar
Hasher, L and Zacks, RT (1988) Working Memory, Comprehension, and Aging: A review and a new view. In Bower, GH (ed), The Psychology of Learning and Motivation (Vol. 22). San Diego, CA: Academic Press, pp. 193225.Google Scholar
Haskell, TR and MacDonald, MC (2005) Constituent Structure and Linear Order in Language Production: Evidence From Subject–Verb Agreement. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 31, 891904. https://doi.org/10.1037/0278-7393.31.5.891Google ScholarPubMed
Hedden, T, Lautenschlager, G and Park, DC (2005) Contributions of processing ability and knowledge to verbal memory tasks across the adult life-span. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 58A, 169190. https://doi.org/10.1080/02724980443000179CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hedden, T and Park, DC (2001) Aging and Interference in Verbal Working Memory. Psychology and Aging, 16, 666681. https://doi.org/10.1037//0882-7974.16.4.666CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Herbay, AC, Gonnerman, LM and Baum, SR (2018) How Do French–English Bilinguals Pull Verb Particle Constructions Off? Factors Influencing Second Language Processing of Unfamiliar Structures at the Syntax-Semantics Interface. Frontiers in Psychology, 9, 1885. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2018.01885CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hertzog, C, Raskind, CL and Cannon, CJ (1986) Age-Related Slowing in Semantic Information Processing Speed: An Individual Differences Analysis. Journal of Gerontology, 41, 500502.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hopp, H (2006) Syntactic features and reanalysis in near-native processing. Second Language Research, 22, 369397. https://doi.org/10.1191/0267658306sr272oaCrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hopp, H (2010) Ultimate attainment in L2 inflection: Performance similarities between non-native and native speakers. Lingua, 120, 901931. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.lingua.2009.06.004CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hopp, H (2014) Working Memory Effects in the L2 Processing of Ambiguous Relative Clauses. Language Acquisition, 21, 250278. https://doi.org/10.1080/10489223.2014.892943CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Howard, JH, Dennis, NA, Howard, DV, Yankovich, H and Vaidya, CJ (2004) Implicit Spatial Contextual Learning in Healthy Aging. Neuropsychology, 18, 124134. https://doi.org/10.1097/OPX.0b013e3182540562.TheCrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Howard, DV and Howard, JH (1989) Age differences in learning serial patterns: direct versus indirect measures. Psychology and Aging, 4, 357364. https://doi.org/10.1037/0882-7974.4.3.357CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Howard, DV and Howard, JH (1992) Adult Age Differences in the Rate of Learning Serial Patterns: Evidence From Direct and Indirect Tests. Psychology and Aging, 7, 232241.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Howieson, DB, Dame, A, Camicioli, R, Sexton, G, Payami, H and Kaye, JA (1997) Cognitive Markers Preceding Alzheimer's Dementia in the Healthy Oldest Old. Journal of the American Geriatrics Society, 45, 584589.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ingvalson, E, Nowicki, C, Zong, A and Wong, PCM (2017) Non-native speech learning in older adults. Frontiers in Psychology, 8, 148. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2017.00148CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Jackson, CN and Bobb, SC (2009) The processing and comprehension of wh-questions among second language speakers of German. Applied Psycholinguistics, 30, 603636.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Jacob, G, Fleischhauer, E and Clahsen, H (2013) Allomorphy and affixation in morphological processing: A cross-modal priming study with late bilinguals. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 16, 924933. https://doi.org/10.1017/S1366728913000291CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Jacob, G, Heyer, V and Veríssimo, J (2017) Aiming at the same target: A masked priming study directly comparing derivation and inflection in the second language. International Journal of Bilingualism, 22, 619637. https://doi.org/10.1177/1367006916688333CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Jardim de Paula, J, de Souza Costa, D, Laiss, B, de Miranda, D and Malloy-Diniz, LF (2013) Verbal fluency in older adults with low educational level: what is the role of executive functions and processing speed? Brazilian Journal of Psychiatry, 35, 440442.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Johns, BT, Sheppard, CL, Jones, MN and Taler, V (2016) The role of semantic diversity in word recognition across aging and bilingualism. Frontiers in Psychology, 7, 703. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2016.00703CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Juncos-Rabadán, O (1994) The assessment of bilingualism in normal aging with the bilingual aphasia test. Journal of Neurolinguistics, 8, 6773. https://doi.org/10.1016/0911-6044(94)90008-6CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kaan, E, Ballantyne, JC and Wijnen, F (2015) Effects of reading speed on second-language sentence processing. Applied Psycholinguistics, 36, 799830. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0142716413000519CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kachlicka, M, Saito, K and Tierney, A (2019) Successful second language learning is tied to robust domain-general auditory processing and stable neural representation of sound. Brain and Language, 192, 1524. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.bandl.2019.02.004CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kail, RV and Salthouse, TA (1994) Processing speed as a mental capacity. Acta Psychologica, 86, 199225.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kaplan, E, Goodglass, H and Weintraub, S (1983) Boston Naming Test. Philadelphia: Lea & Febiger.Google Scholar
Kasparian, K and Steinhauer, K (2017) When the second language takes the lead: Neurocognitive processing changes in the first language of adult attriters. Frontiers in Psychology, 8, 389. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2017.00389CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Keating, GD (2009) Sensitivity to violations of gender agreement in native and nonnative Spanish: An eye-movement investigation. Language Learning, 59, 503535.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Keijzer, M (2013) Working Memory Capacity, Inhibitory Control and the Role of L2 Proficiency in Aging L1 Dutch Speakers of Near-Native L2 English. Brain Sciences, 3, 12611281. https://doi.org/10.3390/brainsci3031261CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kelly, LP (1993) Recall of English Function Words and Inflections by Skilled and Average deaf Readers. American Annals of the Deaf, 138, 288296.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kelly, LP (1996) The Interaction of Syntactic Competence and Vocabulary During Reading by Deaf Students. Journal of Deaf Studies and Deaf Education, 1, 7590.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kemtes, KA and Kemper, S (1997) Younger and Older Adults’ On-Line Processing of Syntactically Ambiguous Sentences. Psychology and Aging, 12, 362371. https://doi.org/10.1037//0882-7974.12.2.362CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kilborn, K (1992) On-line Integration of Grammatical Information in a Second Language. In Harris, RJ (ed), Cognitive Processing in Bilinguals (pp. 337350). Amsterdam: Elsevier.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kirk, NW, Fiala, L, Scott-Brown, KC and Kempe, V (2014) No evidence for reduced Simon cost in elderly bilinguals and bidialectals. Journal of Cognitive Psychology, 26(6), 640648. https://doi.org/10.1080/20445911.2014.929580CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Köpke, B and Schmid, MS (2004) Language Attrition: The next phase. In Schmid, MS, Köpke, B, Keijzer, M and Weilemar, L (eds), First Language Attrition: Interdisciplinary perspectives on methodological issues. Amsterdam: John Benjamins, pp. 143.Google Scholar
Kousaie, S and Phillips, NA (2012) Ageing and bilingualism: Absence of a “bilingual advantage” in Stroop interference in a nonimmigrant sample. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 65, 356369. https://doi.org/10.1080/17470218.2011.604788 https://doi.org/10.1080/20445911.2014.929580 https://doi.org/10.1037//0882-7974.12.2.362CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kousaie, S, Sheppard, C, Lemieux, M, Monetta, L and Taler, V (2014) Executive function and bilingualism in young and older adults. Frontiers in Behavioral Neuroscience, 8, 250. https://doi.org/10.3389/fnbeh.2014.00250CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kramer, AF and Kray, J (2006) Aging and Attention. In Craik, FIM and Bialystok, E (eds), Lifespan cognition: Mechanisms of change. New York, NY: Oxford University Press, pp. 5769 https://doi.org/10.1093/acprof:oso/9780195169539.003.0005.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kürten, J, De Vries, MH, Kowal, K, Zwitserlood, P and Flöel, A (2012) Age affects chunk-based, but not rule-based learning in artificial grammar acquisition. Neurobiology of Aging, 33, 13111317. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neurobiolaging.2010.10.008CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kwong See, ST and Bouchard Ryan, E (1995) Cognitive Mediation of Adult Age Differences in Language Performance. Psychology and Aging, 10, 458468.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
LaBarge, E, Edwards, D and Knesevich, JW (1986) Performance of Normal Elderly on the Boston Naming Test. Brain and Language, 384, 380384.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lardiere, D (1998) Dissociating syntax from morphology in a divergent L2 end-state grammar. Second Language Research, 14, 359375. https://doi.org/10.1191/026765898672500216CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lawrence, B, Myerson, J and Hale, S (1998) Differential Decline of Verbal and Visuospatial Processing Speed Across the Adult Life Span. Aging, Neuropsychology, and Cognition, 5, 129146.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Leal, T, Slabakova, R and Farmer, TA (2016) The fine-tuning of linguistic expectations over the course of L2 learning. Studies in Second Language Acquisition, 39, 493525. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0272263116000164CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lenet, AE, Sanz, C, Lado, B, Howard, JH and Howard, DV (2011) Aging, Pedagogical Conditions, and Differential Success in SLA: An Empirical Study. In Sanz, C and Leow, RP (eds), Implicit and explicit language learning: Conditions, processes, and knowledge in SLA and bilingualism. Washington, DC: Georgetown University Press, pp. 7384.Google Scholar
Light, LL and Anderson, PA (1985) Working-Memory Capacity, Age, and Memory for Discourse. Journal of Gerontology, 40, 737747. https://doi.org/10.1093/geronj/40.6.737CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Linck, JA, Osthus, P, Koeth, JT and Bunting, MF (2014) Working memory and second language comprehension and production: A meta-analysis. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 21, 861883. https://doi.org/10.3758/s13423-013-0565-2CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lindenberger, U and Baltes, PB (1994) Sensory Functioning and Intelligence in Old Age: A Strong Connection. Psychology and Aging, 9, 339355.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
MacWhinney, B (2001) The competition model: The input, the context, and the brain. In Robinson, P (ed), Cognition and Second Language Instruction. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press, pp. 6990.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Madden, DJ (1992) Four to Ten Milliseconds Per Year: Age-Related Slowing of Visual Word Identification. Journal of Gerontology: Psychological Sciences, 47, 5968.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Madden, DJ and Whiting, WL (2004) Age-related changes in visual attention. In Costa, PT and Siegler, IC (eds), Recent advances in psychology and aging. Amsterdam: Elsevier, pp. 4188Google Scholar
Mainz, N, Shao, Z, Brysbaert, M and Meyer, AS (2017) Vocabulary Knowledge Predicts Lexical Processing: Evidence from a Group of Participants with Diverse Educational Backgrounds. Frontiers in Psychology, 8, 1164. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2017.01164CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Marinis, T, Roberts, L, Felser, C and Clahsen, H (2005) Gaps in second language sentence processing. Studies in Second Language Acquisition, 27, 5378.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Mather, M and Harley, CW (2016) The Locus Coeruleus: Essential for Maintaining Cognitive Function and the Aging Brain. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 20, 214226. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tics.2016.01.001CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Mathers, CD, Stevens, GA, Boerma, T, White, RA and Tobias, MI (2015) Causes of international increases in older age life expectancy. The Lancet, 385, 540548. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0140-6736(14)60569-9CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
McDonald, JL (2000) Grammaticality judgments in a second language: Influences of age of acquisition and native language. Applied Psycholinguistics, 21, 395423. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0142716400003064CrossRefGoogle Scholar
McDonald, JL (2006) Beyond the critical period: Processing-based explanations for poor grammaticality judgment performance by late second language learners. Journal of Memory and Language, 55, 381401. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jml.2006.06.006CrossRefGoogle Scholar
McDonald, JL and Roussel, CC (2010) Past tense grammaticality judgment and production in non-native and stressed native English speakers. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 13, 429448. https://doi.org/10.1017/S1366728909990599CrossRefGoogle Scholar
McDonough, IM, Wood, MM and Miller, WS (2019) A Review on the Trajectory of Attentional Mechanisms in Aging and the Alzheimer's Disease Continuum through the Attention Network Test. Yale Journal of Biology and Medicine, 92, 3751.Google ScholarPubMed
Meinzer, M, Flaisch, T, Seeds, L, Harnish, S, Antonenko, D, Witte, AV and Crosson, B (2012) Same modulation but different starting points: Performance modulates age differences in inferior frontal cortex activity during word-retrieval. PLoS ONE, 7, e33631. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0033631CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Meinzer, M, Seeds, L, Flaisch, T, Harnish, S, Cohen, ML, McGregor, KM and Crosson, B (2012) Impact of changed positive and negative task-related brain activity on word-retrieval in aging. Neurobiology of Aging, 33, 656669. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neurobiolaging.2010.06.020CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Meinzer, M, Wilser, L, Flaisch, T, Eulitz, C, Rockstroh, B, Conway, T and Crosson, B (2009) Neural signatures of semantic and phonemic fluency in young and old adults. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 21, 20072018. https://doi.org/10.1162/jocn.2009.21219.NeuralCrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Meir, N, Walters, J and Armon-Lotem, S (2016) Disentangling SLI and bilingualism using sentence repetition tasks: the impact of L1 and L2 properties. International Journal of Bilingualism, 20, 421452. https://doi.org/10.1177/1367006915609240CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Montrul, S (2011) Morphological Errors in Spanish Second Language Learners and Heritage Speakers. Studies in Second Language Acquisition, 33, 163192.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Morgan-Short, K, Finger, I, Grey, S and Ullman, MT (2012) Second language processing shows increased native-like neural responses after months of no exposure. PLoS ONE, 7, e32974. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0032974CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Morgan-Short, K, Sanz, C, Steinhauer, K and Ullman, MT (2010) Second Language Acquisition of Gender Agreement in Explicit and Implicit Training Conditions: An Event-Related Potential Study. Language Learning, 60, 154193.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Morgan-Short, K, Steinhauer, K, Sanz, C and Ullman, MT (2012) Explicit and implicit second language training differentially affect the achievement of native-like brain activation patterns. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 24, 933947. https://doi.org/10.1162/jocn_a_00119CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Morrison, CM, Hirsh, KW and Duggan, GB (2002) Age of acquisition, ageing and verb production: Normative and experimental data. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology: Section A, 56, 705730.Google Scholar
Mortensen, L, Meyer, AS and Humphreys, GW (2006) Age-related effects on speech production: A review. Language and Cognitive Processes, 21, 238290. https://doi.org/10.1080/01690960444000278CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Myerson, J, Ferraro, FR, Hale, S and Lima, SD (1992) General Slowing in Semantic Priming and Word Recognition. Psychology and Aging, 7, 257270.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Nichols, ES, Wild, CJ, Stojanoski, B, Battista, ME and Owen, AM (2020) Bilingualism affords no general cognitive advantages: a population study of executive function in 11,000 people. Psychological Science. https://doi.org/10.1177/0956797620903113CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Noble, KG, Houston, SM, Kan, E and Sowell, ER (2012) Neural correlates of socioeconomic status in the developing human brain. Developmental Science, 15, 516527. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-7687.2012.01147.xCrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Nyberg, L, Lövdén, M, Riklund, K, Lindenberger, U and Bäckman, L (2012) Memory aging and brain maintenance. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 16, 292305. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tics.2012.04.005CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
O'Regan, JK, Lévy-Schoen, A, Pynte, J and Brugaillère, B (1984) Convenient Fixation Location Within Isolated Words of Different Length and Structure. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 10, 250257.Google ScholarPubMed
Olsen, RK, Pangelinan, MM, Bogulski, C, Chakravarty, MM, Luk, G, Grady, CL and Bialystok, E (2015) The effect of lifelong bilingualism on regional grey and white matter volume. Brain Research, 1612, 128139. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.brainres.2015.02.034CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Papadopoulou, D and Clahsen, H (2003) Parsing Strategies in L1 and L2 Sentence Processing: A Study of Relative Clause Attachment in Greek. Studies in Second Language Acquisition, 25, 501528.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Paradis, M (1987) The assessment of bilingual aphasia. New York, NY: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates, Inc.Google Scholar
Paradis, M (2009) Declarative and procedural determinants of second languages (Vol. 40). Amsterdam: John Benjamins Publishing Company.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Park, DC, Lautenschlager, G, Hedden, T, Davidson, NS, Smith, AD and Smith, PK (2002) Models of Visuospatial and Verbal Memory Across the Adult Life Span. Psychology and Aging, 17, 299320. https://doi.org/10.1037//0882-7974.17.2.299CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Pearlmutter, NJ, Garnsey, SM and Bock, K (1999) Agreement Processes in Sentence Comprehension. Journal of Memory and Language, 41, 427456. https://doi.org/10.1006/jmla.1999.2653CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Peelle, JE, Troiani, V, Wingfield, A and Grossman, M (2010) Neural processing during older adults’ comprehension of spoken sentences: Age differences in resource allocation and connectivity. Cerebral Cortex, 20, 773782. https://doi.org/10.1093/cercor/bhp142CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Perani, D, Paulesu, E, Galles, NS, Dupoux, E, Dehaene, S, Bettinardi, V and Mehler, J (1998) The bilingual brain: Proficiency and age of acquisition of the second language. Brain, 121, 18411852.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Perpiñán, S (2015) L2 Grammar and L2 Processing in the Acquisition of Spanish Prepositional Relative Clauses. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 18, 577596. https://doi.org/10.1017/S1366728914000583CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Phillips, C (2013) Some arguments and nonarguments for reductionist accounts of syntactic phenomena. Language and Cognitive Processes, 28, 156187. https://doi.org/10.1080/01690965.2010.530960CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Pinker, S (1999) Words and rules: the ingredients of language. New York: Harper Collins.Google Scholar
Pinker, S and Ullman, MT (2002) The past and future of the past tense. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 6, 456463.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Pliatsikas, C, Veríssimo, J, Babcock, L, Pullman, MY, Glei, DA, Weinstein, M and Ullman, MT (2018) Working memory in older adults declines with age, but is modulated by sex and education. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 72, 13081327.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Prehn, K, Taud, B, Reifegerste, J, Clahsen, H and Flöel, A (2018) Neural correlates of grammatical inflection in older native and second-language speakers. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 21, 112. https://doi.org/10.1017/S1366728916001206CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Prull, M, Gabrieli, JDE and Bunge, SA (2000) Age-related changes in memory: A cognitive neuroscience perspective. In Craik, FIM and Salthouse, TA (eds), Handbook of Aging and Cognition. Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates, pp. 91153.Google Scholar
Ramscar, M, Hendrix, P, Shaoul, C, Milin, P and Baayen, RH (2014) The myth of cognitive decline: Non-linear dynamics of lifelong learning. Topics in Cognitive Science, 6, 542. https://doi.org/10.1111/tops.12078CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ratcliff, R, Thapar, A, Gomez, P and McKoon, G (2004) A diffusion model analysis of the effects of aging in the Lexical Decision Task. Psychology and Aging, 19, 278289. https://doi.org/10.1037/0882-7974.19.2.278CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Raz, N, Ghisletta, P, Rodrigue, KM, Kennedy, KM and Lindenberger, U (2010) Trajectories of brain aging in middle-aged and older adults: Regional and individual differences. NeuroImage, 51, 501511. https://doi.org/10.1038/jid.2014.371CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Reifegerste, J, Elin, K and Clahsen, H (2019) Persistent differences between native speakers and late bilinguals: Evidence from inflectional and derivational processing in older speakers. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 22, 425440. https://doi.org/10.1017/S1366728918000615CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Reifegerste, J and Felser, C (2017) Effects of aging on interference during pronoun resolution. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 60, 35733589. https://doi.org/10.1044/2017_JSLHR-L-17-0183CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Reifegerste, J, Hauer, F and Felser, C (2017) Agreement processing and attraction errors in aging: evidence from subject-verb agreement in German. Aging, Neuropsychology, and Cognition, 24, 672702. https://doi.org/10.1080/13825585.2016.1251550CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Reifegerste, J, Jarvis, R and Felser, C (2020b) Effects of chronological age on native and nonnative sentence processing: Evidence from subject-verb agreement in German. Journal of Memory and Language, 111, 104083. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jml.2019.104083CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Reifegerste, J, Meyer, AS and Zwitserlood, P (2017) Inflectional complexity and experience affect plural processing in younger and older readers of Dutch and German. Language, Cognition and Neuroscience, 32, 471487. https://doi.org/10.1080/23273798.2016.1247213CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Reifegerste, J, Russell, LE, Balota, DA, Luta, G, Meinzer, M, Rugg, MD, Shattuck, K, Turkeltaub, PE, VanMeter, JW, Veríssimo, J and Ullman, MT (2020a) What's that word again? The contribution of the hippocampus to word-finding declines in aging. Poster accepted for the 2020 Cognitive Aging Conference, Atlanta, GA.Google Scholar
Robert, C and Mathey, S (2007) Aging and Lexical Inhibition: The Effect of Orthographic Neighborhood Frequency in Young and Older Adults. Journal of Gerontology, 62, 340342.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rodríguez-Aranda, C and Jakobsen, M (2011) Differential Contribution of Cognitive and Psychomotor Functions to the Age-Related Slowing of Speech Production. Journal Of the International Neuropsychological Society, 17, 807821. https://doi.org/10.1017/S1355617711000828CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rönnlund, M, Nyberg, L, Bäckman, L and Nilsson, L-G (2005) Stability, Growth, and Decline in Adult Life Span Development of Declarative Memory: Cross-Sectional and Longitudinal Data From a Population-Based Study. Psychology and Aging, 20, 318. https://doi.org/10.1037/0882-7974.20.1.3CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ross, TP, Lichtenberg, PA and Christensen, BK (1995) Normative data on the boston naming test for elderly adults in a demographically diverse medical sample. The Clinical Neuropsychologist, 9, 321325. https://doi.org/10.1080/13854049508400496CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Rossi, E and Diaz, MT (2016) How aging and bilingualism influence language processing: Theoretical and neural models. Linguistic Approaches to Bilingualism, 6, 942. https://doi.org/10.1075/lab.14029.rosCrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ryskin, R, Qi, Z, Covington, NV, Duff, MC and Brown-Schmidt, S (2018) Knowledge and learning of verb biases in amnesia. Brain and Language, 182, 6283. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.bandl.2018.04.003CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Sagarra, N and Herschensohn, J (2010) The role of proficiency and working memory in gender and number agreement processing in L1 and L2 Spanish. Lingua, 120, 20222039. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.lingua.2010.02.004CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Sagarra, N and Herschensohn, J (2012) Processing of gender and number agreement in late Spanish bilinguals. International Journal of Bilingualism, 17, 607627. https://doi.org/10.1177/1367006912453810CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Sagarra, N, Sánchez, L and Bel, A (2019) Processing DOM in relative clauses Salience and optionality in early and late bilinguals. Linguistic Approaches to Bilingualism, 9, 120160.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Saito, K, Sun, H, Kachlicka, M, Carvayal Alayo, JR, Nakata, T and Tierney, A (under review) Domain-General Auditory Processing Explains Multiple Dimensions of L2 Acquisition in Adulthood.Google Scholar
Salthouse, TA (1996) The processing-speed theory of adult age differences in cognition. Psychological Review, 103, 403428. https://doi.org/10.1037/0033-295X.103.3.403CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Salthouse, TA (1998) Independence of age-related influences on cognitive abilities across the life span. Developmental Psychology, 34, 851864. https://doi.org/10.1037/0012-1649.34.5.851CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Salthouse, TA and Kail, RV (1983) Memory development throughout the life span: The role of processing rate. In Baltes, PB and Brim, OG (eds), Life-span Development and Behavior. New York: Academic Press, pp. 89116Google Scholar
Salthouse, TA, Pink, JE and Tucker-Drob, EM (2008) Contextual analysis of fluid intelligence. Intelligence, 36, 464486. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.intell.2007.10.003CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Sato, M and Felser, C (2010) Sensitivity to Morphosyntactic Violations in English as a Second Language Sensitivity to Morphosyntactic Violations in English as a Second Language. Second Language, 9, 101118.Google Scholar
Schel, MA, Kühn, S, Brass, M, Haggard, P, Ridderinkhof, KR and Crone, EA (2014) Neural correlates of intentional and stimulus-driven inhibition: a comparison. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience, 8, 27. https://doi.org/10.3389/fnhum.2014.00027CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Schmitter-Edgecombe, M, Vesneski, M and Jones, DWR (2000) Aging and Word-Finding: A Comparison of Spontaneous and Constrained Naming Tests. Archives of Clinical Neuropsychology, 15, 479493.Google ScholarPubMed
Schneider, BA and Pichora-Fuller, MK (2000) Implications of perceptual deterioration for cognitive aging research. In Craik, FIM and Salthouse, TA (eds), The handbook of aging and cognition (2nd ed.). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates, pp. 155219.Google Scholar
Scialfa, CT (2002) The Role of Sensory Factors in Cognitive Aging Research. Canadian Journal of Experimental Psychology, 56, 153163.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Service, E, Simola, M, Metsänheimo, O and Maury, S (2010) Bilingual working memory span is affected by language skill. European Journal of Cognitive Psychology, 14, 383408. https://doi.org/10.1080/09541440143000140CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Shao, Z, Janse, E, Visser, K and Meyer, AS (2014) What do verbal fluency tasks measure? Predictors of verbal fluency performance in older adults. Frontiers in Psychology, 5, 772. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2014.00772CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Shao, Z, Meyer, AS and Roelofs, A (2013) Selective and nonselective inhibition of competitors in picture naming. Memory & Cognition, 41, 12001211. https://doi.org/10.3758/s13421-013-0332-7CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Shao, Z, Roelofs, A and Meyer, AS (2012) Sources of individual differences in the speed of naming objects and actions: The contribution of executive control. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 65, 19271944. https://doi.org/10.1080/17470218.2012.670252CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Shibuya, M and Wakabayashi, S (2008) Why are L2 learners not always sensitive to subject-verb agreement? EUROSLA Yearbook, 8, 235258.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Sonnenstuhl, I, Eisenbeiss, S and Clahsen, H (1999) Morphological priming in the German mental lexicon. Cognition, 72, 203-236. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0010-0277(99)00033-5CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Staub, A (2009) On the interpretation of the number attraction effect: Response time evidence. Journal of Memory and Language, 60, 308327. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jml.2008.11.002CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Steinhauer, K (2014) Event-related Potentials (ERPs) in Second Language Research: A Brief Introduction to the Technique, a Selected Review, and an Invitation to Reconsider Critical Periods in L2. Applied Linguistics, 35, 393417. https://doi.org/10.1093/applin/amu028CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Steinhauer, K, White, EJ and Drury, JE (2009) Temporal dynamics of late second language acquisition: evidence from event-related brain potentials. Second Language Research, 1, 1341. https://doi.org/10.1177/0267658308098995CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Stine-Morrow, EAL (2007) The Dumbledore Hypothesis of Cognitive Aging. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 16, 295299. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-8721.2007.00524.xCrossRefGoogle Scholar
Stine-Morrow, EAL, Loveless, MK and Soederberg, LM (1996) Resource allocation in on-line reading by younger and older adults. Psychology and Aging, 11, 475486. https://doi.org/10.1037/0882-7974.11.3.475CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Stine-Morrow, EAL, Ryan, S and Leonard, JS (2000) Age differences in on-line syntactic processing. Experimental Aging Research, 26, 315322. https://doi.org/10.1080/036107300750015714Google ScholarPubMed
Tokowicz, N and MacWhinney, B (2005) Implicit and explicit measures of sensitivity to violations in second language grammar: An event-related potential investigation. Studies in Second Language Acquisition, 27, 173204.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Tombaugh, TN, Kozak, J and Rees, L (1999) Normative Data Stratified by Age and Education for Two Measures of Verbal Fluency: FAS and Animal Naming. Archives of Clinical Neuropsychology, 14, 167177.Google ScholarPubMed
Tsimpli, I, Sorace, A, Heycock, C and Filiaci, F (2004) First language attrition and syntactic subjects: A study of Greek and Italian near-native speakers of English. International Journal of Bilingualism, 8, 257277. https://doi.org/10.1177/13670069040080030601CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Tucker, GR (1999) A Global Perspective on Bilingualism and Bilingual Education. Retrieved from http://www.cal.org/content/download/1803/19986/file/AGlobalPerspectiveonBilingualism.pdfGoogle Scholar
Ullman, MT (2001a) A neurocognitive perspective on language: the declarative/procedural model. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 2, 717727.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ullman, MT (2001b) The neural basis of lexicon and grammar in first and second language: the declarative/procedural model. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 4, 105122.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ullman, MT (2004) Contributions of memory circuits to language: the declarative/procedural model. Cognition, 92, 231270. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cognition.2003.10.008CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ullman, MT (2005) A Cognitive Neuroscience Perspective on Second Language Acquisition: The Declarative/Procedural Model. In Sanz, C (ed), Mind and Context in Adult Second Language Acquisition: Methods, Theory, and Practice. Washington, DC: Georgetown University Press, pp. 141178.Google Scholar
Ullman, MT (2016) The Declarative/Procedural Model: A Neurobiological Model of Language Learning, Knowledge and Use. In Hickok, G and Small, S (eds), The Neurobiology of Language. Amsterdam: Elsevier, pp. 953968.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ullman, MT (2020) The Declarative/Procedural Model: A Neurobiologically-Motivated Theory of First and Second Language. In VanPatten, B, Keating, GD and Wulff, S (eds), Theories in Second Language Acquisition (3rd editio). New York: Routledge.Google Scholar
Unsworth, N, Spillers, GJ and Brewer, GA (2011) Variation in verbal fluency: A latent variable analysis of clustering, switching, and overall performance. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 64, 447466. https://doi.org/10.1080/17470218.2010.505292CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
VanPatten, B, Keating, GD and Leeser, MJ (2012) Missing verbal inflections as a representational problem: Evidence from self-paced reading. Linguistic Approaches to Bilingualism, 2, 109140. https://doi.org/10.1075/lab.2.2.01patCrossRefGoogle Scholar
Veríssimo, J, Heyer, V, Jacob, G and Clahsen, H (2018) Selective Effects of Age of Acquisition on Morphological Priming: Evidence for a Sensitive Period. Language Acquisition, 25, 315326. https://doi.org/10.1080/10489223.2017.1346104CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Veríssimo, J, Verhaeghen, P, Goldman, N, Weinstein, M and Ullman, MT (under review) Evidence that aspects of attention and executive function improve in old age.Google Scholar
Wagers, MW, Lau, EF and Phillips, C (2009) Agreement attraction in comprehension: Representations and processes. Journal of Memory and Language, 61, 206237.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Warren, DE, Rubin, R, Shune, S and Duff, MC (2018) Memory and Language in Aging: How Their Shared Cognitive Processes, Neural Correlates, and Supporting Mechanisms Change with Age. In Rizzo, M, Anderson, S and Fritzsch, B (eds), The Wiley Handbook on the Aging Mind and Brain. Hoboken, NJ: Wiley Blackwell, pp. 270295.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Waters, GS and Caplan, D (2001) Age, Working Memory, and On-Line Syntactic Processing in Sentence Comprehension. Psychology and Aging, 16, 128144. https://doi.org/10.1037//0882-7974.16.1.128CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Wauters, LN, Van Bon, WHJ and Tellings, AEJM (2006) Reading comprehension of Dutch deaf children. Reading and Writing, 19, 4976. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11145-004-5894-0CrossRefGoogle Scholar
White, L, Valenzuela, E, Kozlowska-MacGregor, M and Leung, Y-KI (2004) Gender and number agreement in nonnative Spanish. Applied Psycholinguistics, 25, 105133. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0142716404001067CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Wilmoth, JR (2000) Demography of longevity: past, present, and future trends. Experimental Gerontology, 35, 11111129.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Wong, PCM, Ettlinger, M and Zheng, J (2013) Linguistic Grammar Learning and DRD2-TAQ-IA Polymorphism. PLoS ONE, 8, e64983. https://doi.org/10.1371/CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Wong, PCM, Morgan-Short, K, Ettlinger, M and Zheng, J (2012) Linking neurogenetics and individual differences in language learning: The dopamine hypothesis. Cortex, 48, 10911102. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cortex.2012.03.017CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Wong, PCM, Vuong, L and Liu, K (2017) Personalized Learning: From Neurogenetics of Behaviors to Designing Optimal Language Training. Neuropsychologia, 98, 1922000. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.physbeh.2017.03.040CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Yap, MJ, Balota, DA, Sibley, DE and Ratcliff, R (2012) Individual Differences in Visual Word Recognition: Insights From the English Lexicon Project. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 38, 5379. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0024n7Google ScholarPubMed