Hostname: page-component-cd9895bd7-lnqnp Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-12-28T03:38:13.295Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Effect of bilingualism on infants’ cognitive flexibility

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  17 December 2021

Diane Poulin-Dubois*
Affiliation:
Department of Psychology, Concordia University, Psychology, Montréal, QC, Canada.
Cassandra Neumann
Affiliation:
Department of Psychology, Concordia University, Psychology, Montréal, QC, Canada.
Sandra Masoud
Affiliation:
Department of Psychology, Concordia University, Psychology, Montréal, QC, Canada.
Adina Gazith
Affiliation:
Department of Psychology, Concordia University, Psychology, Montréal, QC, Canada.
*
Address for correspondence: Diane Poulin-Dubois, Department of Psychology, Concordia University, 7141 Sherbrooke Street West, Montréal, Québec, Canada, H4B 1R6 [email protected]

Abstract

Research suggests that bilinguals often outperform monolinguals on tasks that tap into executive functions, such as those requiring conflict resolution and cognitive flexibility. Recently, better attentional control has been detected in infants as young as 6 months, thereby providing a possible basis for a cognitive benefit before language production. The goal of the present study was to examine if cognitive flexibility is more advanced in bilingual infants. A detour reaching task assessing conflict resolution, a delayed response task assessing shifting, and a multiple location task assessing maintaining, were administered to 17-month-old infants. The main findings revealed that being bilingual did not improve performance on any of the executive function tasks. Furthermore, current exposure to a second language or language proficiency did not impact executive functioning. We conclude that a bilingual advantage in cognitive flexibility may not be present before children have enough experience in code switching.

Type
Research Article
Copyright
Copyright © The Author(s), 2021. Published by Cambridge University Press

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

Adesope, OO, Lavin, T, Thompson, T and Ungerleider, C (2010) A systematic review and meta-analysis of the cognitive correlates of bilingualism. Review of Educational Research 80(2), 207245. https://doi.org/10.3102/0034654310368803CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Adi-Japha, E, Berberich-Artzi, J and Libnawi, A (2010) Cognitive flexibility in drawings of bilingual children. Child Development 81, 13561366. http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-8624.2010.01477.xCrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Antón, E, Duñabeitia, JA, Estévez, A, Hernández, JA, Castillo, A, Fuentes, LJ, Davidson, DJ and Carreiras, M (2014) Is there a bilingual advantage in the ANT task? Evidence from children. Frontiers in Psychology 5, 398. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2014.00398Google Scholar
Arizmendi, GD, Alt, M, Gray, S, Hogan, TP, Green, S and Cowan, N (2018) Do bilingual children have an executive function advantage? Results from inhibition, shifting, and updating tasks. Language, Speech, and Hearing Services in Schools 49(3), 356378. https://doi.org/10.1044/2018_LSHSS-17-0107CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bialystok, E (2017) The bilingual adaptation: How minds accommodate experience. Psychological Bulletin 143(3), 233262. https://doi.org/10.1037/bul0000099CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bialystok, E, Barac, R, Blaye, A and Poulin-Dubois, D (2010) Word mapping and executive functioning in young monolingual and bilingual children. Journal of Cognition and Development 11, 485508. https://doi.org/10.1080/15248372.2010.516420CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bosch, L and Ramon-Casas, M (2014) First translation equivalents in bilingual toddlers' expressive vocabulary: Does form similarity matter? International Journal of Behavioral Development 38(4), 317322. https://doi.org/10.1177/0165025414532559CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bright, P and Filippi, R (2019) Perspectives on the ‘Bilingual Advantage’: Challenges and opportunities. Frontiers Research Topics. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2019.01346CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Brito, N and Barr, R (2014) Flexible memory retrieval in bilingual 6-month-old infants. Developmental psychobiology 56(5), 11561163. https://doi.org/10.1002/dev.21188CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Brito, NH and Barr, R (2012) Influence of bilingualism on memory generalization during infancy. Developmental science 15(6), 812816. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-7687.2012.1184.xCrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Brito, NH, Greaves, A, Leon-Santos, A, Fifer, WP and Noble, KG (2020) Associations between bilingualism and memory generalization during infancy: Does socioeconomic status matter? Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 110. https://doi.org/10.1017/S1366728920000334Google ScholarPubMed
Brito, NH, Grenell, A and Barr, R (2014) Specificity of the bilingual advantage for memory: Examining cued recall, generalization, and working memory in monolingual, bilingual, and trilingual toddlers. Frontiers in psychology 5, 1369. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2014.01369CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Carlson, SM and Meltzoff, AN (2008) Bilingual experience and executive functioning in young children. Developmental Science 11(2), 282–98. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-7687.2008.00675.xCrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Comishen, KJ, Bialystok, E and Adler, SA (2019) The impact of bilingual environments on selective attention in infancy. Developmental Science 22(4), 111. https://doi.org/10.1111/desc.12797CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Costa, A, Hernández, M, Costa-Faidella, J and Sebastián-Gallés, N (2009) On the bilingual advantage in conflict processing: Now you see it, now you don't. Cognition 113, 135149. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cognition.2009.08.001CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Crivello, C, Kuzyk, O, Rodrigues, M, Friend, M, Zesiger, P and Poulin-Dubois, D (2016) The effects of bilingual growth on toddlers’ executive function. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology 141, 121132. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.jecp.2015.08.004CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
David, A and Wei, L (2008) Individual differences in the lexical development of french–english bilingual children. International Journal of Bilingual Education and Bilingualism 11(5), 598618. https://doi.org/10.2167/beb478.0CrossRefGoogle Scholar
DeAnda, S, Bosch, L, Poulin-Dubois, D, Zesiger, P and Friend, M (2016) The language exposure assessment tool: Quantifying language exposure in infants and children. Journal of speech, language, and hearing research: JSLHR 59(6), 13461356. https://doi.org/10.1044/2016_JSLHR-L-15-0234CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Devine, RT, Ribner, A and Hughes, C (2019) Measuring and predicting individual differences in executive functions at 14 Months: A longitudinal study. Child Development 90, 618636. https://doi.org/10.1111/cdev.13217CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Diamond, A (1985) Development of the ability to use recall to guide action, as indicated by infants’ performance on AB. Child Development 56(4), 868883. PMID: 4042750.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
D'Souza, D, Bradt, D, Haensel, JX and D'Souza, H (2020) Is mere exposure enough? The effects of bilingual environments on infant cognitive development. Royal Society Open Science 7(2), https://doi.org/10.1098/rsos.180191.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Duñabeitia, JA, Hernández, JA, Antón, E, Macizo, P, Estévez, A, Fuentez, LJ and Carreiras, M (2014) The inhibitory advantage in bilingual children revisited: Myth or reality? Experimental Psychology 61(3), 234251. https://doi.org/10.1027/1618-3169/a000243CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Fenson, L, Pethick, S, Renda, C, Cox, JL, Dale, PS and Reznick, JS (2000) Short-form versions of the MacArthur Communicative Development Inventories. Applied Psycholinguistics 21(1), 95116. https://doi.org/10.1017/s0142716400001053CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Fuhs, MW and Day, JD (2011) Verbal ability and executive functioning development in preschoolers at head start. Developmental Psychology 47, 404416. Doi: https://doi.org/10.1037/a0021065CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gaertner, BM, Spinrad, TL and Eisenberg, N (2008), Focused attention in toddlers: Measurement, stability, and relations to negative emotion and parenting. Infant and Child Development 17, 339363. https://doi.org/10.1002/icd.580CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Garon, N, Smith, IM and Bryson, SE (2013) A novel executive function battery for preschoolers: Sensitivity to age differences. Child Neuropsychology 20(6), 713736. https://doi.org/10.1080/09297049.2013.857650CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gathercole, VCM, Thomas, EM, Kennedy, I, Prys, C, Young, N, Guasch, NV, Roberts, EJ, Hughes, EK and Jones, L (2014) Does language dominance affect cognitive performance in bilinguals? Lifespan evidence from preschoolers through older adults on card sorting, Simon, and metalinguistic tasks. Frontiers in Psychology 5(11). https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2014.00011CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gervain, J and Werker, JF (2013) Prosody cues word order in 7-month-old bilingual infants. Nature Communications 4, 1490. https://doi.org/10.1038/ncomms2430CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Goldsmith, SF and Morton, JB (2018) Sequential congruency effects in monolingual and bilingual adults: A failure to replicate Grundy et al. (2017). Frontiers in Psychology 9. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2018.02476CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Green, DW and Abutalebi, J (2013) Language control in bilinguals: The adaptive control hypothesis. Journal of cognitive psychology 25(5), 515530. https://doi.org/10.1080/20445911.2013.796377CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Grosjean, F (2012) Bilingual: Life and reality. Cambridge, MA: Harvard.Google Scholar
Hendry, A and Holmboe, K (2020, April 16). Early Executive Functions Questionnaire. https://doi.org/10.31234/osf.io/rhzkqCrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hendry, A, Jones, EJH and Charman, T (2016) Executive function in the first three years of life: Precursors, predictors and patterns. Developmental Review 42, 133. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.dr.2016.06.005CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ibanez-Lillo, A, Pons, F, Costa, A and Sebastian-Galles, N (2010) Inhibitory control in 8-month-old monolingual and bilingual infants: Evidence from an anticipatory eye movement task. In Poster presented at the 22nd Biennial International Conference on Infant Studies, Baltimore, MD.Google Scholar
IBM Corp. Released 2020. IBM SPSS Statistics for Windows, Version 27.0. Armonk, NY: IBM CorpGoogle Scholar
Kalashnikova, M, Pejovic, J and Carreiras, M (2020) The effects of bilingualism on attentional processes in the first year of life. Developmental Science 24(2). https://doi.org/10.1111/desc.13011Google ScholarPubMed
Kovács, AM and Mehler, J (2009) Cognitive gains in 7-month-old bilingual infants. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America 106, 65566560. http://dx.doi.org/10.1073/pnas.0811323106CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kroll, JF, Dussias, PE, Bogulski, CA and Kroff, JRV (2012) Juggling two languages in one mind: What bilinguals tell us about language processing and its consequences for cognition. In Ross, B (ed.), The psychology of learning and motivation, pp. 229262.Google Scholar
Kuhn, LJ, Willoughby, MT, Wilbourn, MP, Vernon-Feagans, L and Blair, CB (2014) Early communicative gestures prospectively predict language development and executive function in early childhood. Child Development 85, 18981914. https://doi.org/10.1111/cdev.12249Google ScholarPubMed
Legacy, J, Reider, J, Crivello, C, Kuzyk, O, Friend, M, Zesiger, P and Poulin-Dubois, D (2017) Dog or chien? Translation equivalents in the receptive and expressive vocabularies of young french-english bilinguals. Journal of Child Language 44(4), 881904. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0305000916000295CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lehtonen, M, Soveri, A, Laine, A, Järvenpää, J, De Bruin, A and Antfolk, J (2018) Is bilingualism associated with enhanced executive functioning in adults? A meta-analytic review. Psychological Bulletin 144, 394425. https://doi.org/10.1037/bul0000142CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lowe, CJ, Cho, I, Goldsmith, SF and Morton, JB (2021) The bilingual advantage in children's executive functioning is not related to language status: A meta-analytic review. Psychological science 32(7), 11151146. https://doi.org/10.1177/0956797621993108CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lukasik, KM, Lehtonen, M, Soveri, A, Waris, O, Jylkkä, J, Laine, M and Dritschel, B (2018) Bilingualism and working memory performance: evidence from a large-scale online study. Plos One 13(11), 0205916. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0205916CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Mangold, P. (2010) Interact user guide, V. 8.0. ff. Arnstorf: Mangold International.Google Scholar
McGuigan, N and Nunez, M (2006) Executive functioning by 18–24-month-old children: Effects of inhibition, working memory demands and narrative in a novel detour-reaching task. Infant and Child Development 15, 519542. https://doi.org/10.1002/icd.477CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Molnar, M, Pejovic, J, Yee, E and Carreiras, M (2014) Cognitive gains in Basque-Spanish bilingual infants? Poster Presented at the International Conference on Infant Studies, Berlin, Germany.Google Scholar
Nichols, ES, Wild, CJ, Stojanoski, B, Battista, ME and Owen, AM (2020) Bilingualism affords no general cognitive advantages: A population study of executive function in 11,000 People. Psychological Science 31(5), 548567. https://doi.org/10.1177/0956797620903113CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Peredo, TN, Owen, MT, Rojas, R and Caughy, MO (2015) Child vocabulary, maternal behavior, and inhibitory control development among Spanish-speaking children. Early Education and Development 26, 749769. https://doi.org/10.1080/10409289.2015.1009319CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Piaget, J (1954) The development of object concept. (M. Cook, Trans.). In J. Piaget & M. Cook (Trans.), The construction of reality in the child (p. 3–96). Basic Books. https://doi.org/10.1037/11168-001CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Poulin-Dubois, D and Yott, J (2014) Fonctions exécutives et théorie de l'esprit chez le jeune enfant: une relation réciproque? Psychologie Française 59(1), 5969.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Poulin-Dubois, D, Blaye, A, Coutya, J and Bialystok, E (2011) The effects of bilingualism on toddlers’ executive functioning. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology 108(3), 567579. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jecp.2010.10.009CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Poulin-Dubois, D, Kuzyk, O, Legacy, J, Zesiger, P and Friend, M (2017) Translation equivalents facilitate lexical access in very young bilinguals. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition 21, 856866. https://doi.org/10.1017/S1366728917000657 https://doi.org/10.1016/j.psfr.2013.11.002CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
R Core Team (2013) R: A language and environment for statistical computing. R Foundation for Statistical Computing, Vienna, Austria. URL http://www.R-project.org/.Google Scholar
Schott, E, Kremin, LV and Byers-Heinlein, K (2019, June). Child Bi- and Multilingualism in the Home in Canada: Rates and language pairs. Poster presented at the International Symposium on Bilingualism 12, Edmonton, Canada.Google Scholar
Singh, L, Fu, CSL, Rahman, AA, Hameed, WB, Sanmugam, S, Agarwal, P, Jiang, B, Chong, YS, Meaney, MJ, Rifkin-Graboi, A, & GUSTO Research Team. (2015) Back to basics: a bilingual advantage in infant visual habituation. Child Development 86(1), 294302. https://doi.org/10.1111/cdev.12271CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Trudeau, N, Frank, I and Poulin-Dubois, D (1999) Une adaptation en français Québécois du MacArthur Communicative Development Inventory. La revue d'orthophonie et d'audiologie 23, 6173.Google Scholar
Tsui, A and Fennell, C (2019, May 18). Do bilingual infants possess enhanced cognitive skills? https://doi.org/10.31234/osf.io/dcn7yCrossRefGoogle Scholar
Vallotton, C and Ayoub, C (2011) Use your words: The role of language in the development of toddlers’ self-regulation. Early Childhood Research Quarterly 26, 169181. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.ecresq.2010.09.002CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
van den Noort, M, Struys, E, Bosch, P, Jaswetz, L, Perriard, B, Yeo, S, Barisch, P, Vermeire, K, Lee, SH and Lim, S (2019) Does the Bilingual Advantage in cognitive control exist and if so, what are its modulating factors? A Systematic Review. Behavioral Sciences 9(3), 27. https://doi.org/10.3390/bs9030027CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Yott, J and Poulin-Dubois, D (2012) Breaking the rules: Do infants have a true understanding of false belief? British Journal of Developmental Psychology 30(1), 156171. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.2044-835X.2011.02060.xCrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Zelazo, PD, Reznick, JS and Spinazolla, J (1998) Representational flexibility and response control in a multistep search task. Developmental Psychology 34(2), 203214. https://doi.org/10.1037//0012-1649.34.2.203CrossRefGoogle Scholar