Hostname: page-component-cd9895bd7-dzt6s Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-12-26T20:22:01.815Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

The effect of age on language attrition: Evidence from bilingual returnees

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  21 July 2010

Cristina Flores*
Affiliation:
University of Minho, Portugal
*
Address for correspondence: Departamento de Estudos Germanísticos e Eslavos, Instituto de Letras e Ciências Humanas, Universidade do Minho, Campus de Gualtar, 4710-057 Braga, Portugal[email protected]

Abstract

The present study investigates the syntactic competence of bilingual Portuguese–German returnees who have lost regular contact with their L2 (German). The main criterion which distinguishes the participants is the age of input loss. This allows their division into two main groups: speakers who lost German input during early childhood (between ages seven and ten) and speakers who were eleven or older when they moved away from the German environment. Focusing on verb placement in main and embedded clauses, the available data show strong evidence of the existence of a stabilization phase following the acquisition period. The speakers who lost L2 input earlier than age eleven show significantly more syntactic deficits than the other speakers. However, the observed attrition effects seem to be the result of insufficient L2 activation, rather than the expression of undergoing competence loss.

Type
Research Article
Copyright
Copyright © Cambridge University Press 2010

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

Footnotes

*

I would like to thank Pilar Barbosa, Jürgen Meisel and Erwin Koller for their insightful comments on my research. My thanks also go to FCT, the Portuguese Council for Research, which supported the study (grant POCI/LIN/59780/2004). I am also indebted to Andreia Rauber for advice on statistics and to the anonymous reviewers for their valuable comments.

References

de Bot, K., Gommans, P., & Rossing, C. (1991). L1 loss in an L2 environment: Dutch immigrants in France. In Seliger, H. W. & Vago, R. M. (eds.), First language attrition, pp. 8798. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.10.1017/CBO9780511620720.006Google Scholar
Bylund, E. (2009). Maturational constraints and first language attrition. Language Learning, 59 (3), 687715.10.1111/j.1467-9922.2009.00521.xGoogle Scholar
Bylund, E., Abrahamsson, N., & Hyltenstam, K. (in press). The role of language aptitude in first language attrition: The case of prepubescent attriters. Applied Linguistics.Google Scholar
Clahsen, H. (1990). Constraints on parameter setting. A grammatical analysis of some acquisition stages in German child language. Language Acquisition, 1, 361391.10.1207/s15327817la0104_3Google Scholar
Clahsen, H., & Muysken, P. (1986). The availability of universal grammar to adult and child learners – a study of the acquisition of German word order. Second Language Research, 2 (2), 93119.Google Scholar
Clahsen, H., Weyerts, H., Penke, M., Münte, T. F., & Heinze, H.-J. (2002). Word order in sentence processing: An experimental study of verb placement in German. Journal of Psycholinguistic Research 31, 223280.Google Scholar
Green, D. W. (1986). Control, activation and resource: A framework and a model for the control of speech in bilinguals. Brain and Language, 27, 210223.10.1016/0093-934X(86)90016-7Google Scholar
Gürel, A. (2004). Selectivity in L2-induced L1 attrition: A psycholinguistic account. Journal of Neurolinguistics, 17 (1), 5378.10.1016/S0911-6044(03)00054-XGoogle Scholar
Håkansson, G. (1995). Syntax and morphology in language attrition: A study of five bilingual expatriate Swedes. International Journal of Applied Linguistics, 5 (2), 153171.10.1111/j.1473-4192.1995.tb00078.xGoogle Scholar
Håkansson, G., & Nettelbladt, U. (1993). Developmental sequences in L1 (normal and impaired) and L2 acquisition of Swedish. International Journal of Applied Linguistics, 3, 131157.10.1111/j.1473-4192.1993.tb00047.xGoogle Scholar
Hakuta, K., & d'Andrea, D. (1992). Some properties of bilingual maintenance and loss in Mexican background high-school students. Applied Linguistics, 13 (1), 7299.Google Scholar
Hyltenstam, K., & Abrahamsson, N. (2003). Maturational constraints in second language acquisition. In Doughty, C. & Long, M. (eds.), Handbook of second language acquisition, pp. 539588. Oxford: Blackwell.Google Scholar
Kaufman, D. (2001). Tales of L1 attrition – evidence from pre-puberty children. In Ammerlan, T., Hulsen, M., Strating, H. & Yamur, K. (eds.), Sociolinguistic and psycholinguistic perspectives on maintenance and loss of minority languages, pp. 185202. Münster: Waxmann.Google Scholar
Kaufman, D., & Aronoff, M. (1991). Morphological disintegration and reconstruction in first language attrition. In Seliger, H. W. & Vago, R. M. (eds.), First language attrition, pp. 175188. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.10.1017/CBO9780511620720.012Google Scholar
Köpke, B. (2004). Neurolinguistic aspects of attrition. Journal of Neurolinguistics, 17 (1), 330.10.1016/S0911-6044(03)00051-4Google Scholar
Köpke, B., & Schmid, M. (2004). Language attrition: The next phase. In Schmid, M., Köpke, B., Keijzer, M. & Weilemar, L. (eds.), First language attrition: Interdisciplinary perspectives on methodological issues, pp. 147. Amsterdam: John Benjamins.Google Scholar
Kuhberg, H. (1992). Longitudinal L2-attrition versus L2-acquisition, in three Turkish children – empirical findings. Second Language Research, 8 (2), 138154.Google Scholar
Meisel, J. M. (2007) Child second language acquisition or successive first language acquisition? Arbeiten zur Mehrsprachigkeit. Working Papers in Multilingualism. Folge B, 80, 3364.Google Scholar
Montrul, S. (2004). Subject and object expression in Spanish heritage speakers: A case of morpho-syntactic convergence, Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 7 (2), 125142.10.1017/S1366728904001464Google Scholar
Pallier, C., Dehaene, S., Poline, J.-B., LeBihan, D., Argenti, A.-M., Dupoux, E., & Mehler, J. (2003). Brain imaging of language plasticity in adopted adults: Can a second language replace the first? Cerebral Cortex, 13, 155161.10.1093/cercor/13.2.155Google Scholar
Paradis, M. (2004). A neurolinguistic theory of bilingualism. Amsterdam/Philadelphia: Benjamins.10.1075/sibil.18Google Scholar
Paradis, M. (2007). L1 attrition features predicted by a neurolinguistic theory of bilingualism. In Köpke, B., Schmid, M., Keijzer, M. & Dosert, S. (eds.), Language attrition. Theoretical perspectives, pp. 121133. Amsterdam: John Benjamins.10.1075/sibil.33.09parGoogle Scholar
Platzack, C. (1996). The initial hypothesis of syntax: A minimalist perspective on language acquisition and attrition. In Clahsen, H. (ed.), Generative perspectives on language acquisition, pp. 369414. Amsterdam: John Benjamins.10.1075/lald.14.15plaGoogle Scholar
Rothweiler, M. (2006). The acquisition of V2 and subordinate clauses in early successive acquisition of German. In Lleó, C. (ed.), Interfaces in multilingualism, pp. 91113. Amsterdam: John Benjamins.10.1075/hsm.4.05rotGoogle Scholar
Schmid, M. (2002). First language attrition, use, and maintenance. The case of German Jews in anglophone countries. Amsterdam: John Benjamins.10.1075/sibil.24Google Scholar
Seliger, H. W. (1989). Deterioration and creativity in childhood bilingualism. In Hylstenstam, K. & Obler, L. K. (eds.), Bilingualism across the lifespan, pp. 173184. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.10.1017/CBO9780511611780.011Google Scholar
Sorace, A. (2004). Native language attrition and developmental instability at the syntax–discourse interface: Data, interpretations and methods. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 7 (2), 143145.10.1017/S1366728904001543Google Scholar
Thoma, D., & Tracy, R. (2006). Deutsch als frühe Zweitsprache: zweite Erstsprache? In Ahrenholz, B. (ed.), Kinder mit Migrationshintergrund. Spracherwerb und Förder-möglichkeiten, pp. 5879. Freiburg: Fillbach.Google Scholar
Tomiyama, M. (2000). Child second language attrition: A longitudinal case study. Applied Linguistics, 21 (3), 304332.10.1093/applin/21.3.304Google Scholar
Tsimpli, I., Sorace, A., Heycock, C., & Filiaci, F. (2004). First language attrition and syntactic subjects: A study of Greek and Italian near-native speakers of English, International Journal of Bilingualism, 8 (3), 257277.10.1177/13670069040080030601Google Scholar
Ventureyra, V., & Pallier, C. (2004). In search of the lost language: The case of adopted Koreans in France. In Schmid, M., Köpke, B., Keijzer, M. & Weilemar, L. (eds.), First language attrition: Interdisciplinary perspectives on methodological issues, pp. 207221. Amsterdam: John Benjamins.10.1075/sibil.28.12venGoogle Scholar