Hostname: page-component-cd9895bd7-dk4vv Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-12-26T07:48:28.877Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

The contributions of proficiency and semantics to the bilingual sentence superiority effect

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  28 November 2022

Portia N. Washington
Affiliation:
Department of Psychology, University of North Carolina Greensboro, Greensboro, NC, USA
Robert W. Wiley*
Affiliation:
Department of Psychology, University of North Carolina Greensboro, Greensboro, NC, USA
*
Address for correspondence: Robert W. Wiley, Department of Psychology University of North Carolina Greensboro 296 Eberhart Building PO Box 26170 | Greensboro, NC 27402-6170 E-mail: [email protected]
Rights & Permissions [Opens in a new window]

Abstract

A long-standing question about bilingualism concerns which representations are shared across languages. Recent work has revealed a bilingual Sentence Superiority Effect (SSE) among French–English bilinguals reading mixed-language sentences: identification of target words is more accurate in syntactically grammatical than ungrammatical sentences. While this ability to connect words across the two languages has been attributed to a rapid parsing of shared syntactic representations, outstanding questions remain about the role of semantics. Here, we replicate the SSE in Spanish–English bilinguals (e.g., better identification of vacío in “my vaso is vacío” [my glass is empty] than “is vaso my vacío” [is glass my empty]). Importantly, we report evidence that semantics do contribute to word identification, but significantly less than syntax and only in the context of syntactically grammatical sentences. Moreover, the effect is moderated by language proficiency, further constraining the conditions under which shared cross-linguistic representations are rapidly accessed in the bilingual mind.

Type
Research Article
Creative Commons
Creative Common License - CCCreative Common License - BY
This is an Open Access article, distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution licence (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/), which permits unrestricted re-use, distribution and reproduction, provided the original article is properly cited.
Copyright
Copyright © The Author(s), 2022. Published by Cambridge University Press

Introduction

Bilingual research has been propelled by questions such as whether bilinguals have separate, language-specific systems, and the extent to which they are capable of processing cross-linguistic information in parallel. The separate syntax account states that bilinguals rely on distinct, language-specific properties during language processing. Behavioral and fMRI data from selective impairment and differential recovery of one language in bilingual aphasic participants has been interpreted as evidence in favor of separate processing (Pearce, Reference Pearce2005). More recently, a stronger case has been made for the shared syntax account that states bilinguals are able to access language information cross-linguistically. Evidence from cross-linguistic priming (e.g., Hartsuiker, Pickering & Veltkamp, Reference Hartsuiker, Pickering and Veltkamp2004; Hsin, Legendre & Omaki, Reference Hsin, Legendre and Omaki2013), post-cued report paradigms (e.g., Declerck, Wen, Snell, Meade & Grainger, Reference Declerck, Wen, Snell, Meade and Grainger2020), eye tracking and fMRI (Marian, Spivey & Hirsch, Reference Marian, Spivey and Hirsch2003) has all supported the shared syntax account in both sentence production and comprehension.

For example, Hartsuiker et al. (Reference Hartsuiker, Pickering and Veltkamp2004) examined the effects of crosslinguistic priming on Spanish–English bilinguals using a picture description task. Prior to this study, work done on shared or separate systems was primarily concerned with lexical or semantic contexts. The participants were asked to describe pictures to a confederate who read from a script (in English) to prime the participants to use either an active or passive phrase in Spanish. The tendency of the participants to use the same type of phrase as the confederate was interpreted as evidence for shared syntax. This seminal paper explains the shared syntax account by claiming that the grammatical hierarchy consists of the lemmas for Spanish–English equivalents attaching to the same category node (e.g., verb, noun), then forming combinatorial nodes to represent sentence structures (e.g., active or passive).

This study also raised the relevance of language proficiency; specifically, the authors emphasized that the bilinguals in their study were moderately-to-highly proficient and lived in a second language-dominant culture, arguing that “at least for these speakers, the advantage of parsimoniously storing a syntactic rule only once outweighs the disadvantage of having to consider alternatives in another language” (Hartsuiker et al., Reference Hartsuiker, Pickering and Veltkamp2004, p. 412). This implies that shared syntax may depend at least in part on the proficiency and linguistic environment in which the bilinguals find themselves. Moreover, others have stressed that bilingual status is not a binary variable (De Bruin, Reference De Bruin2019), and a recent meta-analysis of the “bilingual advantage” (Lowe, Cho, Goldsmith & Morton, Reference Lowe, Cho, Goldsmith and Morton2021) argues that proficiency is an underappreciated consideration in bilingual research. It is therefore important to consider that shared syntax may similarly be a question of degree rather than an absolute.

The bilingual sentence superiority effect

The origins of the sentence superiority effect (SSE) can be traced to the seminal work of Cattel (1886), who first demonstrated the word superiority effect: participants more accurately recall briefly presented letters when they form a word as opposed to a scrambled string of letters. Extended to sentences (Baddeley, Hitch & Allen, Reference Baddeley, Hitch and Allen2009; Toyota, Reference Toyota2001), the SSE refers to a participant's ability to recall a briefly presented target word more often in the context of a grammatical sentence as opposed to an ungrammatical one. With the development of the post-cued partial report method (Reicher, Reference Reicher1969; Wheeler, Reference Wheeler1970), researchers used the Rapid Parallel Visual Presentation (RPVP) paradigm to show that whole word identification is active before individual letter identification in sentence reading (McClelland & Rumelhart, Reference McClelland and Rumelhart1981). Recent work has used the RPVP paradigm with post-cued partial report to identify a contribution of syntax to the SSE (Snell & Grainger, Reference Snell and Grainger2017), with target word identification being greater when embedded in syntactically grammatical as opposed to ungrammatical sentences. This has been interpreted as evidence for rapid parallel processing of parts-of-speech, such that the reader constructs a sentence-level frame that constrains the candidate words, thereby improving word identification. That claim is supported in part by eye-tracking methods that demonstrate multiple words are simultaneously processed in reading sufficiently for their syntactic roles to influence behavior. Specifically, Snell, Meeter, and Grainger (Reference Snell, Meeter and Grainger2017) used a gaze-contingent boundary paradigm to investigate syntactic ambiguity in parafoveal processing. Participants read sentences that were syntactically congruent, with the parafoveal preview of the target word matching the target's part-of-speech (e.g., preview “The young horse | waved over the fence” for the target “The young horse | jumped over the fence”), or incongruent, with the parafoveal preview having a different part-of-speech (e.g., “The young horse | table over the fence). Both faster response times and higher accuracy for congruent compared to incongruent sentences provides clear evidence for simultaneous integration of syntactic information from multiple words.

This research has been extended to bilingual contexts, wherein a rapid parsing of mixed-language sentences allows bilingual participants to report a target word in syntactically grammatical sentences more accurately than ungrammatical sentences, first observed in Declerck et al. (Reference Declerck, Wen, Snell, Meade and Grainger2020) in French–English bilinguals. The authors interpreted their results as evidence of cross-linguistic processing of syntactic properties in parallel, which is in support of the shared syntax hypothesis. Effectively ruling out the role of guessing while using post-cued partial report with RPVP, Wen, Snell, and Grainger (Reference Wen, Snell and Grainger2019) presented ERP evidence showing that sentence structure has a significant influence during the early stages of sentence comprehension (within 300 ms), a finding which has been replicated in the context of a grammaticality judgment task (Wen, Mirault & Grainger, Reference Wen, Mirault and Grainger2021). A question remains about the extent to which this effect can be explained by syntax alone, and in particular parts-of-speech. Previous research observing the SSE acknowledges that there may be at least some contribution of semantics because of the likelihood of syntactically grammatical sentences also being semantically interpretable (e.g., Snell & Grainger, Reference Snell and Grainger2017). Recently, Massol, Mirault, and Grainger (Reference Massol, Mirault and Grainger2021) examined the effect of incorporating semantically uninterpretable sentences to explore the contribution of semantics in the SSE for monolingual French speakers. Although they identified a significant effect of both syntactic grammaticality and semantic interpretability, the SSE effect arising from semantics was much smaller than that arising from syntax. It is currently unknown whether this pattern is also true of bilingual processing of mixed-language sentences.

The current study addresses the following outstanding questions: 1) does the bilingual SSE, first reported in Declerck et al. (Reference Declerck, Wen, Snell, Meade and Grainger2020), replicate in Spanish–English/English–Spanish bilinguals? 2) how does language proficiency moderate the bilingual SSE? and 3) how does semantic interpretability contribute to cross-linguistic processing of syntax in mixed-language sentences? To answer these questions, we first adapted the stimuli and RPVP paradigm used in Declerck et al. (Reference Declerck, Wen, Snell, Meade and Grainger2020) to replicate the bilingual SSE in a different population of bilinguals (Figure 1). We then expanded the stimuli to include semantically uninterpretable sentences, by replacing a single (non-target) word in each sentence while keeping all other factors in the sentence constant (language, parts-of-speech, word lengths, and the target word and its position in the sentence). To anticipate our results, we replicated the bilingual SSE in Spanish–English/English–Spanish bilinguals, demonstrating that this effect of (un)grammatical word order in mixed-language sentences is not specific to French–English, and furthermore we found that this effect is moderated by proficiency. Moreover, we report a significant interaction between syntax and semantics, such that semantic intepretability only moderates the bilingual SSE when the word order is grammatical in both Spanish and English (but not when the word order is ungrammatical in both languages). These results provide novel evidence generally in line with recent work (e.g., Snell & Grainger, Reference Snell and Grainger2017; Wen et al., Reference Wen, Snell and Grainger2019, Reference Wen, Mirault and Grainger2021), supporting the hypothesis that it is primarily information about parts-of-speech that contribute to rapid identification of the words within grammatical sentences. The notable exception is that the role of semantics was found to differ in the bilingual SSE compared to the monolingual SSE, as reported in Massol et al. (Reference Massol, Mirault and Grainger2021) – a possible indication that integration of semantic information differs in the case of cross-linguistic processing. We return to these points in the discussion.

Figure 1. Example of the types of sentences used in the experiments. The target word (vacío) maintains its position in the sentence, while the non-target word subjected to semantic manipulation (vaso/odio) maintains its position, part-of-speech, and word length. GI = Grammatical Syntax, Interpretable Semantics; GU = Grammatical Syntax, Uninterpretable Semantics; UI = Ungrammatical Syntax, Interpretable Semantics; UU = Ungrammatical Syntax, Uninterpretable Semantics.

Methods & materials

Participants

For Experiment 1, data were collected from 57 individuals with 9 excluded from the analysesFootnote 1. This sample size was based on previous work (e.g., Declerck et al., Reference Declerck, Wen, Snell, Meade and Grainger2020 indicated that as few as N = 24 was sufficient to detect the SSE with the same procedure and number of items). For Experiment 2, data were collected from 153 individuals with 18 excluded from analysesFootnote 2, with no overlap in participants between the two experiments. This sample size was based on a sensitivity analysis (conducted with the R package simr, version 1.0.5; Green & MacLeod, Reference Green and MacLeod2016) that indicated N = 148 participants would be sufficient to detect an effect size as small as a log odds ratio = 0.04 for the interaction between syntactic and semantic grammaticality, with power = 0.8. Participants were recruited from Amazon Mechanical Turk (MTurk) using CloudResearch (Litman & Robinson, Reference Litman and Robinson2020), which is linked to MTurk and provided additional data collection control (i.e., to target participants who speak both English and Spanish). They provided informed consent according to IRB protocols and were compensated $5.00. In addition to professing bilingual status, prior to the main task, the participants completed a version of the Lextale-Esp measure of Spanish proficiency (Izura, Cuetos & Brysbaert, Reference Izura, Cuetos and Brysbaert2014), adapted by the authors for the online software Psytoolkit (Stoet, Reference Stoet2010, Reference Stoet2017). This measure was used to assess the Spanish proficiency of each participant and allowed for investigating an interaction between the bilingual SSE and a range of Spanish proficiency. English proficiency was not assessed, based on the assumption that the participant recruitment pool would provide only highly proficient speakers of English. This assumption was supported empirically by participants’ responses to a language background questionnaire: 96% of the participants reported using English on a daily basis versus just 36% for Spanish, and the mean age of acquisition for English was 1.67 years (median = 0, i.e., from birth), versus 5.21 years for Spanish (median = 1). Tables 1 and 3 summarize the language background responses respectively for Experiment 1 and 2.

Table 1. Descriptive statistics for the participants included in Experiment 1. AoA = Age of acquisition (years). Comm. = Communication (1 = daily, 2 = most days, 3 = occasionally, 4 = rarely).

Stimuli

Experiment 1

The stimuli from Declerck et al. (Reference Declerck, Wen, Snell, Meade and Grainger2020) were translated to fit our population. We only made changes that fell within the criteria set by Declerck et al., and only when necessary based on the effects of translating the French words to Spanish (e.g., differences in which words are cognates in English). Briefly, each sentence had four words, 2 to 6 letters long, none of which were cognates. Half of the words in the sentence were in English and the other half in Spanish, the order varying randomly to prevent the participant from guessing the language of the target word. The target words were equally divided between English and Spanish. Each of the 200 grammatical sentences had an ungrammatical counterpart that consisted of the same words in a scrambled order (holding the target word in the same position). We refer hereafter to the fully grammatical sentences as GI (Grammatical Syntax, Interpretable Semantics), and to the ungrammatical counterparts with scrambled word order as UI (Ungrammatical Syntax, Interpretable Semantics).

Experiment 2

Semantically uninterpretable versions of the sentences from Experiment 1 were created by replacing a non-target word with a semantically anomalous one, maintaining the same part-of-speech, length, and position. These sentences are hereafter referred to as either GU (Grammatical Syntax, Uninterpretable Semantics) or UU (Ungrammatical Syntax, Uninterpretable Semantics). Seven sentences differed otherwise from those in Experiment 1 (three were changed and four new ones were added)Footnote 3, resulting in a final list of 204 sentences, each with four versions (Figure 2).

Figure 2. Illustration of the trial sequence for Experiments 1 and 2. In this grammatical sentence, the cued target word is “stay” and can be typed after the hash marks appear.

Interpretability Ratings of Experiment 1 and 2 Sentences

Subsequent to collecting the data for both experiments, a separate group of 80 participants was asked to rate the sentences for their interpretability. These participants were recruited in the same manner as those in Experiments 1 and 2 and were compensated $2.50 for their time. All of the GI sentences as well as their UI and GU counterparts were rated on a 5-point scale, with 1 = Uninterpretable and 5 = Interpretable. Participants rated 102 GI sentences and either 102 UI or 102 GU sentences, and were allowed to freely view the sentences for as long as they wanted (unlike the main experiments, where sentence presentations were 200 ms). Full details about this task are presented in the Supplementary Materials (Supplementary Materials, Section 1: Interpretability Ratings). Summarizing the results, the mean rating for GI sentences was 4.5 (sd = 0.30), UI sentences 2.12 (sd = 0.39), and GU sentences 2.52 (sd = 0.53). This confirmed that the manipulations of both syntax and semantics affected the interpretability of the sentences as intended. However, six of the GI sentences received a median rating below “Somewhat Interpretable”, and an additional two GU sentences failed to be rated as less interpretable than their GI counterparts. We therefore removed those items from analyses, as well as one item from Experiment 1 that was presented to participants with an error. Therefore, 193 items were included in the analyses of Experiment 1 and 196 in the analyses of Experiment 2.

The sentences used for both experiments, their interpretability ratings, and the processed data are all available on the Open Science Foundation (OSF) at: https://osf.io/k5es9/?view_only=d7104e73d7044dc397fc58d498447cab

Procedure

This study was approved by the authors’ home institution's IRB. The experiment was programmed with Psytoolkit (Stoet, Reference Stoet2017) and administered online. All participants gave informed consent prior to completing the experiment. The Lextale-Esp proficiency measure was completed first, followed by eight practice trials of the RPVP task. The main experiment consisted of 200 (Experiment 1) or 204 (Experiment 2) Spanish–English trials. Each participant saw 100 grammatical and 100 ungrammatical sentences (Experiment 1), or 51 sentences per combination of syntactic grammaticality and semantic interpretability (Experiment 2). Experiment 1 stimuli were organized into two pairs of counter-balanced lists, such that each participant saw either the grammatical or ungrammatical version of each sentence (but never both). The same was true for Experiment 2, except more lists were needed for counterbalancing due to the 2 × 2 manipulation. The order of sentences within each list was randomized.

The procedure followed that of Declerck et al. (Reference Declerck, Wen, Snell, Meade and Grainger2020): each trial started with two vertical bars at the center of the screen (500 ms), followed by four words between the bars (200 ms), after which each letter was replaced with a hashmark (Figure 2). A cue mark appeared above the target word as well as a place for participants to type their answers; the participant's screen did not change until they pressed the enter key; they were then given feedback in the form of a green (correct) or red (incorrect) circle.

Analyses

Accuracy was modeled with generalized (binomial) linear mixed-effects regression in R (R Core Team, 2022), package lme4 (v 1.1-29; Bates, Maechler, Bolker, Walker, Christensen, Singmann, Dai, Scheipl, Grothendieck, Green, Fox, Bauer & Krivitsky, Reference Bates, Maechler, Bolker, Walker, Christensen, Singmann, Dai, Scheipl, Grothendieck, Green, Fox, Bauer and Krivitsky2021). All continuous variables were mean-centered and scaled by the standard deviation. P-values for the fixed effects were obtained with the likelihood ratio test (LRT) method provided by the afex package (v. 0.27-2, Singmann, Bolker, Westfall, Aust & Ben-Shachar, Reference Singmann, Bolker, Westfall, Aust and Ben-Shachar2020). In addition, for the main analyses of Experiment 1 and 2 only, 95% confidence intervals were computed with the bootstrap method provided by lme4. The R code for these models as well as the convergence checks are available with the other files on OSF.

For Experiment 1, the dependent variable (correct/incorrect) on each trial was regressed on the main variables of interest: Syntactic Grammaticality (coded +1 for GI sentences and −1 UI sentences), Proficiency (Lextale-Esp score), the interaction of Syntactic Grammaticality X Proficiency, and the following control variables: Age (in years), Trial Number, Target Word Position (treated as a categorical variable with 4 levels), and Target Word Frequency. In addition to these fixed effects, random effects were included both by-participant and by-item: random intercepts and random slopes for Syntactic Grammaticality.

For Experiment 2, there was also Semantic Interpretability (coded +1 for GI and UI sentences and −1 for GU and UU sentences) plus its interaction with Syntactic Grammaticality (coded +1 for GI and GU sentences and −1 for UI and UU sentences), both as fixed and random slopes, to test whether semantics moderate the effect of syntactic grammaticality (or vice versa). Finally, the interaction between Proficiency and Semantic Interpretability was also included in Experiment 2, alongside the additional control variable of Non-Target Word Frequency (for the semantically-manipulated wordFootnote 4). Word frequencies were obtained from SUBTLEX-ESP (Cuetos, Glez-Nosti, Barbón & Brysbaert, Reference Cuetos, Glez-Nosti, Barbón and Brysbaert2011) and SUBTLEX-US (Brysbaert & New, Reference Brysbaert and New2009) respectively for Spanish and English words.

A note about model (non-)convergence

The mixed-effects models of both Experiments 1 and 2 encountered a model convergence warning, relating specifically to the estimation of the correlation between the random slope for Syntactic Grammaticality and/or Semantic Interpretability by-participants, on the one hand, and the random intercept by-participants on the other. We followed the suggestions outlined in Brauer and Curtin (Reference Brauer and Curtin2018) and Bates et al. (Reference Bates, Maechler, Bolker, Walker, Christensen, Singmann, Dai, Scheipl, Grothendieck, Green, Fox, Bauer and Krivitsky2021) to investigate this issue. We determined that the warning was due to too little variance remaining in the effect of grammaticality across participants, once also controlling for the interaction between grammaticality and proficiency. Nonetheless, we determined via the allFit function in the lme4 package that the fixed-effects estimates were unaffected by this issue (i.e., all optimizers estimated the same coefficients for the fixed effects to at least the third decimal, and dropping the by-participants random slope while maintaining the by-item random slope similarly had a negligeable effect on the estimates). We therefore report the results of the models despite the convergence “warnings”, consistent with the recommendations of the authors of the lme4 package (Bates et al., Reference Bates, Maechler, Bolker, Walker, Christensen, Singmann, Dai, Scheipl, Grothendieck, Green, Fox, Bauer and Krivitsky2021), and the fact that the inferences drawn in this manuscript are based only on the fixed-effects estimates (see Brauer & Curtin, Reference Brauer and Curtin2018; Singmann & Kellen, Reference Singmann, Kellen, Spieler and Schumacher2019, for more support and discussion of this strategy).

Results

The final sample of 48 participants included 12 compound bilinguals (learned both Spanish and English from birth), 19 English–Spanish bilinguals (English from birth and Spanish on average from age 11, sd = 5 years), 12 Spanish–English bilinguals (Spanish from birth and English on average from age 6, sd = 2 years), and 5 Other (some language besides English or Spanish from birth; English on average from age 4, sd = 4 years, and Spanish also from age 4, sd = 4 years). The average score on the Spanish proficiency test, Lextale-Esp, was 0.69, sd = 0.15. The participants were asked how frequently they communicate in each language (1 = daily, 2 = most days, 3 = occasionally, 4 = rarely); for English, the average was 1.04, sd = 0.2, and for Spanish, the average was 1.88, sd = 0.8. This information is summarized in Table 1.

Experiment 1

The raw data, aggregated by participant, are depicted in Figure 3 (see Supplementary Materials Appendix, Figure A1, for the data aggregated by item). The fixed effects from the regression model are summarized in Table 2 (the full model including the random effects is reported in the Appendix, Table A1). Depicted as raw averages by participant (Figure 3 top), the main effect of Syntactic Grammaticality was significant, estimated to be a log odds ratio (OR) = 0.19, 95% CI [0.09, 0.28] (likelihood ratio test, LRT, p-value < 0.001). The estimated marginal means were obtained with the R package emmeans (version 1.5.5-1; Lenth, 2021), which revealed that accuracy was 8.4% lower for target word identification in syntactically ungrammatical compared to grammatical sentences (71.2% versus 62.8%); this is comparable to the effect size reported for French–English bilinguals in Declerck et al. (Reference Declerck, Wen, Snell, Meade and Grainger2020) of 7.3%. The SSE was significantly moderated by Spanish Proficiency (Figure 4 bottom), estimated log OR = 0.06, 95% CI [0.01, 0.12] (p ≈ 0.017). This can be interpreted as a larger effect of proficiency on identifying targets in grammatical sentences compared to ungrammatical ones. Follow-up comparisons indicated a significant effect of Spanish Proficiency for grammatical sentences (log OR = 0.37, p ≈ 0.017), but only a marginal effect for ungrammatical sentences (log OR = 0.27, p ≈ 0.059).

Figure 3. Results of Experiment 1. Top panel: raw accuracy, by participants, for sentences with grammatical syntax (G) versus ungrammatical syntax (U). The box plots depict the median and interquartile ranges, with each dot representing a single participant. Bottom panel: the sentence superiority effect (SSE), depicted as mean accuracy on grammatical - ungrammatical sentences (y-axis), is moderated by Spanish proficiency (x-axis), with a larger SSE for higher-proficiency versus lower-proficiency individuals. The line depicts the positive linear trend, with each dot representing a single participant. The gray region reflects standard error of the mean.

Figure 4. Results of Experiment 2. Top panel: GI = Grammatical Syntax, Interpretable Semantics; UI = Ungrammatical Syntax, Interpretable Semantics; GU = Grammatical Syntax, Uninterpretable Semantics; UU = Ungrammatical Syntax, Uninterpretable Semantics. The box plots depict the median and interquartile ranges, with each dot representing a single participant. Bottom panel: the sentence superiority effect (SSE), depicted for Syntax as mean accuracy on GI/GU-UI/UU sentences (circles/solid line), and for Semantics as mean accuracy on GI/UI-GU/UU sentences (triangles/dashed line). The effect of Syntax is moderated by Spanish proficiency (x-axis), with a larger SSE for higher-proficiency versus lower-proficiency individuals, as shown in the positive linear trend (solid line). There is no such moderation for the effect of Semantics (dashed line). The gray region reflects standard error of the mean.

Table 2. Summary of the mixed-effects model for Experiment 1. The primary variables of interest are highlighted in gray. Log OR = log odds ratio. Confidence intervals obtained by bootstrap method and p-values reflect likelihood ratio tests (LRT). Further details about the model are presented in the Appendix, Table A1. Predictors significant at p < 0.05 in bold.

To address the possibility that the effect of Grammaticality was driven not by cross-linguistic processes, but rather by rapid translation of the mixed-language sentences into monolinguals ones (e.g., “she está with ellas” → “she is with them”), an analysis was conducted by coding each error as a translation-equivalent (e.g., target word = “guantes”, response = “gloves”) or not. On average, just 3.2% of participants’ errors were translation-equivalents (range across participants: 0%−16.2%, sd = 2.8%)Footnote 5. A generalized linear-mixed effects model was used to analyze just the incorrect trials, with identical structure to the one reported for the main analyses, and the dependent variable indicating translation-equivalent errors (1) versus all other errors (0). Neither Syntactic Grammaticality (log OR = 0.04, p ≈ 0.95), Spanish Proficiency (log OR = −0.09, p ≈ 0.65), nor their interaction (log OR = −0.02, p ≈ 0.87) were significantly associated with rates of translation-equivalent errors. These results are consistent with findings reported by Declerck et al. (Reference Declerck, Wen, Snell, Meade and Grainger2020) that just 1.7% of errors were translation-equivalents, although in that study a statistical analysis was not conducted. The only significant predictors of translation-equivalent errors were Trial Order (log OR = −0.41, p ≈ 0.01), indicating that participants made fewer of these errors later in the experiment, and Target Frequency (log OR = 1.01, p ≈ 0.001), indicating that higher frequency words were more prone to this type of error. This model is reported in detail in the Supplementary Materials Appendix (Table A2).

Experiment 2

The final sample of 135 participants included 42 compound bilinguals (learned both Spanish and English from birth), 51 English–Spanish bilinguals (English from birth and Spanish on average from age 12, sd = 7 years), 23 Spanish–English bilinguals (Spanish from birth and English on average from age 6, sd = 5 years), and 19 Other (some language besides English or Spanish from birth; English on average from age 6, sd = 4 years, and Spanish from age 6, sd = 6 years). The average score on the Spanish proficiency test, Lextale-Esp, was 0.67, sd = 0.13. The participants were asked how frequently they communicate in each language (1 = daily, 2 = most days, 3 = occasionally, 4 = rarely); for English, the average was 1.04, sd = 0.2, and for Spanish, the average was 2.01, sd = 1.0. This information is summarized in Table 3.

Table 3. Descriptive statistics for the participants included in Experiment 2. AoA = Age of acquisition (years). Comm. = Communication (1 = daily, 2 = most days, 3 = occasionally, 4 = rarely).

The raw data are depicted in Figure 4 (aggregated by participant; see Supplementary Materials Appendix, Figure A2 for aggregation by item), and the fixed effects from the regression model are summarized in Table 4 (the full model including the random effects is reported in the Supplementary Materials Appendix, Table A3). As in Experiment 1, the main effect of Syntactic Grammaticality was significant, log OR = 0.15, 95% CI [0.08, 0.22] (p < 0.001), The interaction of Syntactic Grammaticality X Proficiency was also significant, log OR = 0.041, 95% CI [0.01, 0.09] (p ≈ 0.004), indicating a larger effect of grammaticality for more-proficient participants. There was no significant main effect of Semantic Interpretability, log OR = 0.02, 95% CI [−0.03, 0.07]; similarly, the interaction of Semantic Interpretability X Proficiency was not significant, log OR = 0.002, 95% CI [−0.03, 0.03] (p ≈ 0.894).

Table 4. Summary of the mixed-effects model for Experiment 2. The primary variables of interest are highlighted in gray. Log OR = log odds ratio. Confidence intervals obtained by bootstrap method and p-values reflect likelihood ratio tests (LRT). Further details about the model are presented in the Appendix, Table A3. Predictors significant at p < 0.05 in bold.

However, there was a significant interaction of Syntactic Grammaticality X Semantic Interpretability, log OR = 0.03, 95% CI [0.003, 0,06] (p ≈ 0.032). The estimated marginal means reveal the following pattern of results: for sentences with interpretable semantics, the effect of Syntactic Grammaticality was significant, estimated as 9.0% lower accuracy for ungrammatical sentences, p < 0.001 (Figure 4 top, GI versus UI). For sentences with uninterpretable semantics, the effect of Syntactic Grammaticality was significantly smaller (p ≈ 0.032), but still itself significant, estimated as 5.8%, p ≈ 0.002 (Figure 4 top, GU versus UU). The results can also be interpreted this way: for sentences with grammatical syntax, the effect of Semantic Interpretability was marginally significant, estimated as 2.7% lower accuracy for uninterpretable sentences, p ≈ 0.055 (Figure 4 top, GI versus GU). For sentences with ungrammatical syntax, there was a nonsignificant trend in the opposite direction, 0.05% percent higher accuracy for uninterpretable sentences (p ≈ 0.76).

Exploratory analyses of Experiments 1 and 2

Two sets of exploratory analyses were conducted on the data from both experiments – full details are reported in the Supplementary Materials available on OSF. Briefly, we assessed whether the results differed between English and Spanish target words (Supplementary Materials Section 2: Analyses of Target Language), and, for sentences with Spanish target words, whether those with diacritic marks differed from those without (Section 3: Analyses of Diacritic Marks). For each of these sets of analyses, we controlled for a familywise error rate of testing each hypothesis twice (i.e., in Experiments 1 and 2) with alpha of 0.05/2 = 0.25 (see, e.g., Rubin, Reference Rubin2017).

Target Language

Declerck et al. (Reference Declerck, Wen, Snell, Meade and Grainger2020) demonstrated that the SSE effect in French–English bilinguals was independent of the target language, i.e., there was no significant difference between the magnitude of the SSE assessed by French target words compared to English target words. Similarly, our analyses revealed no significant main effect of Target Language nor an interaction with Syntactic Grammaticality in Experiment, although the trend was for better identification of English words. For Experiment 2, the main effect was significant, with better identification of English target words than Spanish, but there was no interaction with either Syntactic Grammaticality or Semantic Interpretability. The general tendency for better identification of English words plausibly is related to higher proficiency with English relative to Spanish (which may also explain why the numerical trend in Declerk et al. favored French over English, as those participants were L1 French, L2 English); because we did not measure English proficiency, we cannot directly assess this possibility.

Diacritic Marks

The final set of exploratory analyses addressed the possibility that diacritic marks, which were present only on (some of) the Spanish words, could contribute to word identification, due to their visual saliency (e.g., Perea, Baciero & Marcet, Reference Perea, Baciero and Marcet2021). We investigated this possibility by re-analyzing the results just for the sentences with a Spanish target word, comparing those with and without diacritic marks (e.g., í, ñ, ó). In total there were nine such targets (less than 5% of all trials). Numerically, words with diacritics were more accurately identified than those without, but this was not significant. Moreover, the diacritic marks did not significantly interact with Spanish Proficiency, the Syntactic Grammaticality effect, or the Semantic Interpretability effect, or Spanish Proficiency.

Discussion

The present study was designed to address three questions, motivated by the consensus of recent work observing an SSE in both monolinguals and bilinguals and related to the claim that the sentence superiority effect (SSE) is driven by parts-of-speech. First, we aimed to understand how this effect interacts with proficiency, and to replicate the effect in a population of individuals who speak both Spanish and English, as previously it has been demonstrated only in highly proficient French–English bilinguals. Experiment 1 did replicate the bilingual SSE, in a heterogenous population of individuals who speak both Spanish and English. Furthermore, we found the effect to be moderated by the participants’ Spanish proficiency, with the largest effect observed in the most proficient participants. In fact, while there was a clear and significant advantage of having higher proficiency when identifying words in grammatical sentences (p ≈ 0.02), there was relatively weak evidence for an advantage when identifying words in ungrammatical sentences (p ≈ 0.06). This, coupled with the fact that the interaction between proficiency and grammaticality remained when controlling for word frequency, strongly supports the hypothesis that the SSE is driven by expert linguistic knowledge of sentence structures, and not simply greater familiarity with individual words (or domain general processes that might be deployed even by individuals with little or no proficiency).

Experiment 2 expanded the stimuli to include manipulations of semantic interpretability. Our experimental design allowed us to isolate the contribution of semantics from syntactic grammaticality, as well as to test for interactions between each of these with language proficiency. We again replicated the effect of grammaticality as well as the interaction with proficiency. However, syntactic grammaticality and semantic interpretability showed different patterns of results. Our manipulation of the semantic interpretability of a non-target word (e.g., “my vaso is vacío” versus “my odio is vacío”, target word “vacío”) had only a modest effect on identification of the target word in sentences with grammatical syntax (2.7% lower accuracy, marginally significant at p ≈ 0.055). Crucially, however, that small effect was itself significantly different (p ≈ 0.032) in comparison to what we observed in sentences with ungrammatical syntax, where there was a numeric trend in the opposite direction (0.5% higher accuracy) that was nowhere near statistical significance (p ≈ 0.76). In addition, a separate group of participants corroborated that our manipulation of the non-target words did indeed render the sentences less interpretable, rating them on average 2 points less interpretable on a 5-point scale (see Supplementary Materials Section 1: Interpretability Ratings). In sum, the effect of semantics was smaller than that of syntax, was only present in the context of sentences with grammatical word order, and was not moderated by language proficiency.

These results are in line with other recent findings and are interpreted to be in support of the shared syntax account. By observing a significant difference in the post-cued partial report between syntactically grammatical and ungrammatical mixed-language sentences, it can be concluded that the participants completed a rapid parse of words in a sentence, cross-linguistically, in parallel. The fact that semantics were found to have a significant effect on the bilingual SSE indicates that parts-of speech do not fully account for the effect, meaning that the rapid parse of sentence processing includes (at least some) information beyond parts-of-speech, as argued by Massol and colleagues (2021) in the monolingual case. In that work, the authors reported a significant effect of semantic interpretability even in syntactically ungrammatical contexts; however, we found that target words were less-well identified in semantically ungrammatical compared to grammatical sentences only when the word order was grammatical. While these two studies are the only ones to our knowledge to attempt to distinguish between syntactic and semantic contributions to the SSE, this difference in the results suggests that semantic representations may affect cross-linguistic sentence processing only after the syntactic parsing is successful (e.g., recognition of grammatical word order may be a prerequisite for further processing at the level of semantics) – whereas monolinguistic sentences may allow for a contribution of semantics even within syntactically ungrammatical contexts. This hypothesis is supported by the work of Hagoort (Reference Hagoort2003), which demonstrated an asymmetry in the (simultaneous) processing of syntactic and semantic violations, such that a “syntactic boost” is observed when a semantic violation is put in a grammatically correct sentence. The author did not observe a concomitant semantic boost, which is analogous to our findings here that the effect of semantic grammaticality is observed only when no syntactic violations are present.

Finally, the interaction of proficiency with the effect of syntactic grammaticality but not semantic interpretability further suggests that the simultaneous processing of shared, cross-linguistic syntactic representations requires considerable skill with both languages. The fact that we did not observe any interaction between proficiency and semantics is not surprising given two considerations: first, that models of bilingualism generally posit wholly shared semantic representations (e.g., BiLex – Peñaloza, Grasemann, Dekhtyar, Miikkulainen & Kiran, Reference Peñaloza, Grasemann, Dekhtyar, Miikkulainen and Kiran2019); and second, we controlled for both target and non-target word frequency statistically in the model, which contributed to accounting for differences in the participants’ knowledge of individuals Spanish words (on the assumption that lower-proficiency individuals would be especially unfamiliar with low-frequency words). Alternatively, it may be that the heterogeneity of our bilingual sample, not only in terms of their Spanish proficiency but also their linguistic background (e.g., compound bilingual, multilingual, etc.), had an impact on our results that we cannot fully assess.

In fact, the exploratory analyses of target language and diacritic marks suggest some future directions for further investigating the factors supporting parallel processing of bilingual (i.e., mixed-language) sentences. For example, the significant effects of proficiency we found suggest that future work should consider planned comparisons between subpopulations of bilinguals, and should assess the proficiency level of both L1 and L2. In addition, the analyses of diacritic marks provided some evidence of differential processing for Spanish words with compared to without accent marks, even though power to detect any such effects was low given the small number of items with accent marks. Moreover, because diacritic marks are not part of English orthography, they might have contributed to overall differences between the target languages. One future direction would assess how these visual-orthographic cues contribute to bilingual syntactic processing, especially in mixed-language sentences. For example, the role that diacritic marks play in reading has been posited to be either linguistic (e.g., Chetail & Boursain, Reference Chetail and Boursain2019) or visual-orthographic in nature (e.g., Kinoshita, Yu, Verdonschot & Norris, Reference Kinoshita, Yu, Verdonschot and Norris2021). Research with bilingual populations may provide new insights into critical questions about the status of diacritic marks, considering that their status varies across languages (e.g., lexical stress in Spanish versus vowel quality in German; Perea, Labusch & Marcet, Reference Perea, Labusch and Marcet2022;) as well as within languages (e.g., Spanish accented “él” [he] provides lexical contrast with unaccented “el” [the], whereas accented “marrón” only indicates stress).

Conclusion

Altogether, these findings have implications for future research on the parallel and integrated nature of bilingual syntax processing. First, this effect has now been observed in French, Spanish, and English, but previous studies have indicated that non-alphabetic scripts may rely more on semantics during the initial parse (Asano & Yokosawa, Reference Asano and Yokosawa2011) – as such, more populations of bilinguals should be explored, including those with non-alphabetic scripts. Indeed, one limitation of this study is that the participants were highly heterogeneous, not only in their Spanish proficiency but also in terms of which language was L1, which language is currently their dominant language, etc.; any of these variables may affect how syntactic or semantic processes contribute to comprehension of the mixed-language sentences. Second, the novel finding of null effects of semantics in the context of ungrammatical syntax (as well as the non-significant interaction between semantics and proficiency) raises a number of questions for future work, including whether the results of Massol et al. (Reference Massol, Mirault and Grainger2021) with monolingual participants differ from those reported here due to a difference in mixed-language versus monolingual sentences. Lastly, by identifying a robust effect of syntax on the bilingual SSE, future research should explore the nature of the contribution of parts-of-speech to the rapid parse and the conditions under which semantic representations do or do not contribute – for example, whether certain parts-of-speech are identified during the rapid parse more than others (i.e., nouns and verbs as opposed to articles).

Acknowledgements

This research was funded in part by a McNair Scholars Program award to the first author.

Data availability

The data that support the findings of this study, as well as the analysis codes, are openly available from the Open Science Foundation (OSF) at https://osf.io/k5es9/?view_only=d7104e73d7044dc397fc58d498447cab

Competing interests

The authors declare none.

Supplementary Material

For supplementary material accompanying this paper, visit https://doi.org/10.1017/S1366728922000748

Footnotes

1 2 for reporting a diagnosis of dyslexia, 1 for not completing the Spanish proficiency task, and 6 for not completing the experimental RPVP task.

2 2 for reporting a diagnosis of dyslexia, 2 for not completing the Spanish proficiency task, and 14 for not completing the experimental RPVP task.

3 For example, the sentence in Experiment 1 “she está with ellas” (she is with them), target word ellas, has no possible semantically ungrammatical version that maintains the parts-of-speech and word length of a non-target word (e.g., any other pronoun besides “she” would either remain semantically grammatical or render the sentence syntactically ungrammatical).

4 This was done to control for the fact that the mean frequency of the non-target words in the Semantically Interpretable sentences (706 appearances per million) was significantly higher than their counterparts in the Semantically Uninterpretable sentences (237 per million), t(203) = 5.26, p < 0.001.

5 Fully 34% of the translation-equivalent errors were for the pair is/es, which happen to be both orthographically and phonologically very similar in English/Spanish. The rate was almost identical in Experiment 2: 35%.

References

Asano, M and Yokosawa, K (2011). Rapid extraction of gist from visual text and its influence on word recognition. The Journal of General Psychology 138, 127154.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Baddeley, AD, Hitch, GJ and Allen, RJ (2009). Working memory and binding in sentence recall. Journal of Memory and Language 61, 438456.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bates, D, Maechler, M, Bolker, B, Walker, S, Christensen, RHB, Singmann, H, Dai, B, Scheipl, F, Grothendieck, G, Green, P, Fox, J, Bauer, A and Krivitsky, PN (2021). Package ‘lme4’. CRAN. https://cran.r-project.org/web/packages/lme4/lme4.pdfGoogle Scholar
Brauer, M and Curtin, JJ (2018). Linear mixed-effects models and the analysis of nonindependent data: A unified framework to analyze categorical and continuous independent variables that vary within-subjects and/or within-items. Psychological Methods 23, 389411.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Brysbaert, M and New, B (2009). Moving beyond Kučera and Francis: A critical evaluation of current word frequency norms and the introduction of a new and improved word frequency measure for American English. Behavior Research Methods 41, 977990.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Chetail, F and Boursain, E (2019). Shared or separated representations for letters with diacritics? Psychonomic Bulletin & Review 26, 347352.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cuetos, F, Glez-Nosti, M, Barbón, A and Brysbaert, M (2011). SUBTLEX-ESP: Spanish word frequencies based on film subtitles. Psicológica 32, 133143.Google Scholar
De Bruin, A (2019). Not all bilinguals are the same: A call for more detailed assessments and descriptions of bilingual experiences. Behavioral Sciences 9, 33.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Declerck, M, Wen, Y, Snell, J, Meade, G and Grainger, J (2020). Unified syntax in the bilingual mind. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review 27, 149154.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Green, P and MacLeod, CJ (2016). SIMR : an R package for power analysis of generalized linear mixed models by simulation. Methods in Ecology and Evolution 7, 493498.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hagoort, P (2003). Interplay between syntax and semantics during sentence comprehension: ERP effects of combining syntactic and semantic violations. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience 15, 883899.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hartsuiker, RJ, Pickering, MJ and Veltkamp, E (2004). Is Syntax Separate or Shared Between Languages? Psychological Science 15, 409414.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hsin, L, Legendre, G and Omaki, A (2013). Priming cross-linguistic interference in Spanish–English bilingual children. In Proceedings of the 37th annual Boston university conference on language development. Somerville, MA: Cascadilla Press, pp. 165–77.Google Scholar
Izura, C, Cuetos, F and Brysbaert, M (2014). Lextale-Esp: A test to rapidly and efficiently assess the Spanish vocabulary size. Psicológica 35, 4966.Google Scholar
Kinoshita, S, Yu, L, Verdonschot, RG and Norris, D (2021). Letter identity and visual similarity in the processing of diacritic letters. Memory & Cognition 49, 815825.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Litman, L and Robinson, J (2020). Conducting online research on Amazon Mechanical Turk and beyond. Sage Publications.Google Scholar
Lowe, CJ, Cho, I, Goldsmith, SF and Morton, JB (2021). The Bilingual Advantage in Children's Executive Functioning Is Not Related to Language Status: A Meta-Analytic Review. Psychological science 32, 11151146.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Marian, V, Spivey, M and Hirsch, J (2003). Shared and separate systems in bilingual language processing: Converging evidence from eyetracking and brain imaging. Brain and Language 86, 7082.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Massol, S, Mirault, J and Grainger, J (2021). The contribution of semantics to the sentence superiority effect. Scientific Reports 11.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
McClelland, JL and Rumelhart, DE (1981). An interactive activation model of context effects in letter perception: Part 1. An account of basic findings. Psychological Review, 88, 375407.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Pearce, JM (2005). A Note on Aphasia in Bilingual Patients: Pitres’ and Ribot's Laws European Neurology 54, 127131.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Peñaloza, C, Grasemann, U, Dekhtyar, M, Miikkulainen, R and Kiran, S (2019). BiLex: A computational approach to the effects of age of acquisition and language exposure on bilingual lexical access. Brain and Language 195.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Perea, M, Baciero, A and Marcet, A (2021). Does a mark make a difference? Visual similarity effects with accented vowels. Psychological Research 85, 22792290.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Perea, M, Labusch, M and Marcet, A (2022). How are words with diacritical vowels represented in the mental lexicon? Evidence from Spanish and German. Language, Cognition and Neuroscience 37, 457468.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
R Core Team (2022). R: A language and environment for statistical computing. R Foundation for Statistical Computing, Vienna, Austria. https://www.R-project.org/Google Scholar
Reicher, GM (1969). Perceptual recognition as a function of meaningfulness of stimulus material. Journal of experimental psychology 81, 275.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Rubin, M (2017). Do p Values Lose Their Meaning in Exploratory Analyses? It Depends How You Define the Familywise Error Rate. Review of General Psychology 21, 269275.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Singmann, H and Kellen, D (2019). An Introduction to Mixed Models for Experimental Psychology. In Spieler, D & Schumacher, E (Eds.), New Methods in Cognitive Psychology (1st ed.). Routledge, pp. 431.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Singmann, H, Bolker, B, Westfall, J, Aust, F and Ben-Shachar, MS (2020). afex: Analysis of 644 Factorial Experiments. R package version 0.27-2. https://CRAN.R-project.org/package=afexGoogle Scholar
Snell, J and Grainger, J (2017). The sentence superiority effect revisited. Cognition 168, 217221.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Snell, J, Meeter, M and Grainger, J (2017). Evidence for simultaneous syntactic processing of multiple words during reading. PLOS ONE 12.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Stoet, G (2010). PsyToolkit - A software package for programming psychological experiments using Linux. Behavior Research Methods 42, 10961104.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Stoet, G (2017). PsyToolkit: A novel web-based method for running online questionnaires and reaction-time experiments. Teaching of Psychology 44, 2431.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Toyota, H (2001). Changes in the constraints of semantic and syntactic congruity on memory across three age groups. Perceptual and Motor Skills 92, 691698.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Wen, Y, Snell, J and Grainger, J (2019). Parallel, cascaded, interactive processing of words during sentence reading. Cognition 189, 221226.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Wen, Y, Mirault, J and Grainger, J (2021). Fast syntax in the brain: Electrophysiological evidence from the rapid parallel visual presentation paradigm (RPVP). Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition 47, 99112.Google ScholarPubMed
Wheeler, DD (1970). Processes in word recognition. Cognitive Psychology 1, 5985.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Figure 0

Figure 1. Example of the types of sentences used in the experiments. The target word (vacío) maintains its position in the sentence, while the non-target word subjected to semantic manipulation (vaso/odio) maintains its position, part-of-speech, and word length. GI = Grammatical Syntax, Interpretable Semantics; GU = Grammatical Syntax, Uninterpretable Semantics; UI = Ungrammatical Syntax, Interpretable Semantics; UU = Ungrammatical Syntax, Uninterpretable Semantics.

Figure 1

Table 1. Descriptive statistics for the participants included in Experiment 1. AoA = Age of acquisition (years). Comm. = Communication (1 = daily, 2 = most days, 3 = occasionally, 4 = rarely).

Figure 2

Figure 2. Illustration of the trial sequence for Experiments 1 and 2. In this grammatical sentence, the cued target word is “stay” and can be typed after the hash marks appear.

Figure 3

Figure 3. Results of Experiment 1. Top panel: raw accuracy, by participants, for sentences with grammatical syntax (G) versus ungrammatical syntax (U). The box plots depict the median and interquartile ranges, with each dot representing a single participant. Bottom panel: the sentence superiority effect (SSE), depicted as mean accuracy on grammatical - ungrammatical sentences (y-axis), is moderated by Spanish proficiency (x-axis), with a larger SSE for higher-proficiency versus lower-proficiency individuals. The line depicts the positive linear trend, with each dot representing a single participant. The gray region reflects standard error of the mean.

Figure 4

Figure 4. Results of Experiment 2. Top panel: GI = Grammatical Syntax, Interpretable Semantics; UI = Ungrammatical Syntax, Interpretable Semantics; GU = Grammatical Syntax, Uninterpretable Semantics; UU = Ungrammatical Syntax, Uninterpretable Semantics. The box plots depict the median and interquartile ranges, with each dot representing a single participant. Bottom panel: the sentence superiority effect (SSE), depicted for Syntax as mean accuracy on GI/GU-UI/UU sentences (circles/solid line), and for Semantics as mean accuracy on GI/UI-GU/UU sentences (triangles/dashed line). The effect of Syntax is moderated by Spanish proficiency (x-axis), with a larger SSE for higher-proficiency versus lower-proficiency individuals, as shown in the positive linear trend (solid line). There is no such moderation for the effect of Semantics (dashed line). The gray region reflects standard error of the mean.

Figure 5

Table 2. Summary of the mixed-effects model for Experiment 1. The primary variables of interest are highlighted in gray. Log OR = log odds ratio. Confidence intervals obtained by bootstrap method and p-values reflect likelihood ratio tests (LRT). Further details about the model are presented in the Appendix, Table A1. Predictors significant at p < 0.05 in bold.

Figure 6

Table 3. Descriptive statistics for the participants included in Experiment 2. AoA = Age of acquisition (years). Comm. = Communication (1 = daily, 2 = most days, 3 = occasionally, 4 = rarely).

Figure 7

Table 4. Summary of the mixed-effects model for Experiment 2. The primary variables of interest are highlighted in gray. Log OR = log odds ratio. Confidence intervals obtained by bootstrap method and p-values reflect likelihood ratio tests (LRT). Further details about the model are presented in the Appendix, Table A3. Predictors significant at p < 0.05 in bold.

Supplementary material: PDF

Washington and Wiley supplementary material

Washington and Wiley supplementary material
Download Washington and Wiley supplementary material(PDF)
PDF 343.3 KB