Hostname: page-component-586b7cd67f-tf8b9 Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-11-28T04:35:45.087Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Bilingual versus monolingual infants’ novel word-action mapping before and after first-word production: Influence of developing noun-dominance on perceptual narrowing

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  13 February 2019

Lakshmi Gogate*
Affiliation:
University of Missouri Columbia
Madhavilatha Maganti
Affiliation:
Ashoka University
*
Author for correspondence: Lakshmi Gogate, E-mail: [email protected]

Abstract

This experimental study examined bilingual (English and another noun-dominant language) and monolingual (English) preverbal (10.5-month-old) and postverbal (12.5-month-old) infants’ word-action mapping. Sixteen infants in each group were habituated to dynamic video-displays of novel word-action pairings during infant-controlled habituation. They received two words, /wem/ and /bæf/, spoken synchronously with an adult shaking or looming an object, and were tested with switched versus same word-action pairings. Results revealed that for the preverbal bilingual infants, word-action mapping is intensified relative to postverbal bilingual infants. For the postverbal bilingual infants, word-action mapping is attenuated and inversely correlated with noun learning. No such differences were observed in the monolingual infants. These findings illustrate a perceptual protraction prior to word production, and accelerated perceptual narrowing to nouns after word production in bilingual infants learning two noun-dominant languages.

Type
Research Article
Copyright
Copyright © Cambridge University Press 2019 

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

Bahrick, LE (1994) The development of infants’ sensitivity to arbitrary intermodal relations. Ecological Psychology 6, 111123.Google Scholar
Bahrick, LE, Gogate, LJ and Ruiz, I (2002) Attention and memory for faces and actions in infancy: The salience of actions over faces in dynamic events. Child Development 73(6), 16291643.Google Scholar
Bahrick, LE and Watson, JS (1985) Detection of intermodal proprioceptive-visual contingency as a potential basis of self-perception in infancy. Developmental Psychology 21, 963973.Google Scholar
Baillergeon, R, Spelke, E and Wasserman, S (1985) Object permanence in 5-month-old infants. Cognition 20, 191208.Google Scholar
Bertenthal, B, Haith, MM and Campos, JJ (1983) The partial-lag design: A method for controlling spontaneous regression in the infant-control habituation paradigm. Infant Behavior & Development 6, 331338.Google Scholar
Bosch, L and Sebastián Gallés, N (2003) Simultaneous bilingualism and the language-specific perception of a vowel contrast in the 1st year of life. Language and Speech 46(2-3), 217243.Google Scholar
Byers-Heinlein, K and Fennell, C (2014) Perceptual narrowing in the context of increased variation: Insights from bilingual infants. Developmental Psychobiology 56(2), 274291.Google Scholar
Byers-Heinlein, K, Burns, TC and Werker, JF (2010) The roots of bilingualism in newborns. Psychological Science 21, 343348.Google Scholar
Conboy, BT and Mills, DL (2006) Two languages, one developing brain: Effects of vocabulary size on bilingual toddlers’ event-related potentials to auditory words. Developmental Science 9, F1F11.Google Scholar
Core, C, Hoff, E, Rumiche, R and Señor, M (2013) Total and conceptual vocabulary in Spanish–English bilinguals from 22 to 30 months: Implications for assessment. Journal of Speech Language Hearing Research 56(5), 16371649. doi: 10.1044/1092-4388(2013/11-0044).Google Scholar
DeAnda, S, Poulin-Dubois, D, Zesiger, P and Friend, M (2016) Lexical processing and organization in bilingual first language acquisition: Guiding future research. Psychological Bulletin 142(6), 655667. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/bul0000042Google Scholar
Dhillon, R (2010) Examining the noun bias: A structural approach. Proceedings of the 33rd Annual Penn Linguistics Colloquium, 16, http://repository.upenn.edu/pwpl/vol16/iss1/7Google Scholar
Fava, E, Hull, R and Bortfeld, H (August, 2011) Linking behavioral and neurophysiological indicators of perceptual tuning to language. Frontiers in Psychology 2(174), 314. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2011.00174Google Scholar
Fennell, C, Byers-Heinlein, K and Werker, JF (2007) Using speech sounds to guide word learning: The case of bilingual infants. Child Development 78, 1510–25.Google Scholar
Fenson, L, Dale, P, Reznick, S, Bates, E, Thal, D and Pethick, S (1994) Variability in early communicative development. Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development 59 (5 Serial No. 242).Google Scholar
Flom, R (2014) Perceptual narrowing: Retrospect and prospect. Developmental Psychobiology 56, 14421453.Google Scholar
Garcia-Sierra, A, Rivera-Gaxiola, M, Percaccio, CR, Conboy, BT, Romo, H, Klarman, L, Ortiz, S and Kuhl, PK (2011) Bilingual language learning: An ERP study relating early brain responses to speech, language input, and later word production. Journal of Phonetics 39(4), 546557.Google Scholar
Genesee, F (2003) Educating second language children: The whole child, the whole curriculum, the whole community. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Gogate, LJ (2010) Learning of syllable-object relations by preverbal infants: The role of temporal synchrony and syllable distinctiveness. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology 105, 178197.Google Scholar
Gogate, L (2016) Neural and Developmental Mechanisms for Multisensory Communication in the Early Years. Special Issue, Developmental Neuropsychology 41(5–8), 269374 ISSN: 8756-5641.Google Scholar
Gogate, LJ and Bahrick, LE (1998) Intersensory redundancy facilitates learning of arbitrary relations between vowel sounds and objects in 7-month-old infants. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology 69(2), 133149.Google Scholar
Gogate, L and Hollich, G (2016) Infants’ verb-action and noun-object mapping across sensory modalities: A neuro-developmental view. In special issue, Neural and Developmental Mechanisms for Multisensory Communication in the Early Years. Developmental Neuropsychology 41(5-8), 293307.Google Scholar
Gogate, L and Maganti, M (2016) The dynamics of infant attention: Implications for crossmodal perception and word mapping research. Child Development 87(2), 345364. Doi:10.1111/cdev.12509Google Scholar
Gogate, L and Maganti, M (2017) The origins of verb learning: Preverbal and postverbal infants’ learning of word-action relations. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research 60(12), 35383550. https://doi.org/10.1044/2017_JSLHR-L-17-0085Google Scholar
Graf Estes, K and Hay, JF (2015) Flexibility in bilingual infants' word learning. Child Development 86(5), 13711375.Google Scholar
Hadley, H, Rost, G, Fava, E and Scott, LS (2014) A mechanistic approach to cross-domain perceptual narrowing in the first year of life. Brain Sciences 4, 613634. doi:10.3390/brainsci4040613Google Scholar
Hay, JF, Graf Estes, K, Wang, T and Saffran, JR (2015) From flexibility to constraint: The contrastive use of lexical tone in early word learning. Child Development 86, 1022. doi:10.1111/cdev.12269Google Scholar
Kovacs, AM and Mehler, J (2009a) Cognitive gains in 7-month-old bilingual infants. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences 106, 65566560Google Scholar
Kovacs, AM and Mehler, J (2009b) Flexible learning of multiple speech structures in bilingual infants. Science 325, 611612.Google Scholar
Kuhl, PK, Williams, KA, Lacerda, F, Stevens, KN and Lindblom, B (1992, Jan. 31). Linguistic experience alters phonetic perception in infants by 6 months of age. Science 255(5044), 606608.Google Scholar
Lewkowicz, DJ and Ghazanfar, A (2009) The emergence of multisensory systems through perceptual narrowing. Trends in Cognitive Sciences 13(11), 470478. DOI: 10.1016/j.tics.2009.08.004Google Scholar
Liu, L and Kager, R (2016) Perception of a native vowel contrast by Dutch monolingual and bilingual infants: A bilingual perceptual lead. International Journal of Bilingualism 20(3), 335345. doi: 10.1177/1367006914566082Google Scholar
Matatyaho, DJ, Mason, Z and Gogate, LJ (2007) Word Learning by Eight-Month-Old Infants: The Role of Object Motion and Synchrony. Proceedings of the 7 th International Conference on Epigenetic Robotics: Modeling cognitive development in robotic systems. In Berthouze, L, Prince, CG, Littman, M, Kozima, H, and Balkenius, C (eds.), Lund University Cognitive Studies, 135, 201-2, Lund, Sweden.Google Scholar
Matatyaho-Bullaro, D, Gogate, L, Mason, Z, Cadavid, S and Abdel-Mottaleb, M (2014) Type of object motion facilitates word mapping by preverbal infants. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology 118, 2740.Google Scholar
Maurer, D and Werker, JF (2014) Perceptual narrowing during infancy: A comparison of language and faces. Developmental Psychobiology 56, 154178.Google Scholar
Mehler, J, Jusczyk, PW, Lambertz, G, Halsted, G, Bertoncini, J and Amiel-Tison, C (1988) A precursor of language acquisition in young infants. Cognition 29, 143178.Google Scholar
Minagawa-Kawai, Y, Mori, K, Naoi, N and Kojima, S (2007) Neural attunement processes in infants during the acquisition of a language-specific phonemic contrast. Journal of Neuroscience 27, 315321.Google Scholar
Moon, C, Panneton-Cooper, R and Fifer, WP (1993) Two-day olds prefer their native language. Infant Behaviour & Development 16, 495500.Google Scholar
Nazzi, T, Bertoncini, J and Mehler, J (1998) Language discrimination by newborns: toward an understanding of the role of rhythm. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance 24, 756766.Google Scholar
Palmer, SB, Fais, L, Golinkoff, RM and Werker, JF (2012) Perceptual narrowing of linguistic sign occurs in the 1st year of life. Child Development 83(2), 543553. DOI: 10.1111/j.1467-8624.2011.01715.xGoogle Scholar
Pascalis, O, de Haan, M and Nelson, CA (2002) Is face processing species-specific in the first year of life? Science 296, 13211323.Google Scholar
Pascalis, O, Loevenbruck, H, Quinn, PC, Kandel, S, Tanaka, JW and Lee, K (2014) On the links among face processing, language processing and narrowing during development. Child Development Perspectives 8, 6570.Google Scholar
Petitto, LA, Berens, MS, Kovelman, I, Dubins, MH, Jasinska, K and Shalinsky, M (2012) The “Perceptual Wedge Hypothesis” as the basis for bilingual babies' phonetic processing advantage: New insights from fNIRS brain imaging. Brain and Language 121(2), 130–43. doi: 10.1016/j.bandl.2011.05.003.Google Scholar
Ramırez, NF, Ramırez, RR, Clarke, M, Taulu, S and Kuhl, P (2017) Speech discrimination in 11-month-old bilingual and monolingual infants: A magnetoencephalography study. Developmental Science 20, 116. doi: 10.1111/desc.12427Google Scholar
Ramus, F, Hauser, MD, Miller, C, Morris, D and Mehler, J (2000) Language discrimination by human newborns and by cotton-top tamarin monkeys. Science 288, 349351.Google Scholar
Singh, L (2017) Bilingual infants demonstrate advantages in learning words in a third language. Child Development, First published May 27 by Wiley Library. DOI:10.1111/cdev.12852Google Scholar
Singh, L, Fu, C, Tay, WZ and Golinkoff, RM (2017) Novel word learning in bilingual and monolingual infants: Evidence for a bilingual advantage. Child Development 89(3), 193198. DOI:10.1111/cdev.12747Google Scholar
Tardif, T, Fletcher, P, Liang, W, Zhang, Z, Kaciroti, N and Marchman, VA (2008) Baby's First 10 Words. Developmental Psychology 44, 929938.Google Scholar
Vihman, MM, Thierry, G, Lum, JA, Keren-Portnoy, T and Martin, P (2007) Onset of word form recognition in English, Welsh, and English–Welsh bilingual infants. Applied Psycholinguistics 28, 475493.Google Scholar
Werker, JF (2012) Perceptual foundations of bilingual acquisition in infancy. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences 50 (1251), 5061. doi: 10.1111/j.1749-6632.2012.06484.xGoogle Scholar
Werker, JF, Fennell, CT, Corcoran, KM and Stager, CL (2002) Infants' ability to learn phonetically similar words: Effects of age and vocabulary size. Infancy 3(1), 130.Google Scholar
Werker, JF and Tees, RC (1984) Cross-language speech perception: evidence for perceptual reorganization during the first year of life. Infant Behavior & Development 7, 4963.Google Scholar

Gogate and Maganti supplementary material

Gogate and Maganti supplementary material 1

Download Gogate and Maganti supplementary material(Video)
Video 1.2 MB

Gogate and Maganti supplementary material

Gogate and Maganti supplementary material 2

Download Gogate and Maganti supplementary material(Video)
Video 5.6 MB