Hostname: page-component-78c5997874-94fs2 Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-11-19T12:36:45.729Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Beyond global differences between monolingual and bilingual children on the nonword repetition task: retention skills for phonemes’ identity and serial order

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  25 May 2017

KIRSTEN SCHRAEYEN*
Affiliation:
Thomas More University College, Department of Speech Language Therapy and Audiology, Research group Language and Learning, Antwerp, Belgium University of Leuven, Parenting and Special Education Research Unit, Faculty of Psychology and Educational Sciences, Leuven, Belgium University of Antwerp, Department of Linguistics, Faculty of Arts, Antwerp, Belgium
WIM VAN DER ELST
Affiliation:
Hasselt University, Institute for Biostatistics and Statstical Bioinformatics, Hasselt, Belgium
ASTRID GEUDENS
Affiliation:
Thomas More University College, Department of Education & Department of Speech language Therapy and Audiology, Research group Language and Reading Education, Antwerp, Belgium
POL GHESQUIÈRE
Affiliation:
University of Leuven, Parenting and Special Education Research Unit, Faculty of Psychology and Educational Sciences, Leuven, Belgium
DOMINIEK SANDRA
Affiliation:
University of Antwerp, Department of Linguistics, Faculty of Arts, Antwerp, Belgium
*
Address for correspondence: Kirsten Schraeyen, Molenstraat 8, 2018 Antwerp, Belgium[email protected]

Abstract

This study compared NRT-performance in monolingual Dutch and bilingual Turkish–Dutch third-graders using a Dutch Nonword Repetition Task (NRT). Several novel response analyses at the phoneme level were applied to further understand the earlier reported overall accuracy differences in NRT-performance between bilinguals and monolinguals. Analyses in which the retention of phonemes and the retention of their serial order were disentangled revealed that monolinguals outperform bilinguals with respect to the retention of the phonemes themselves. However, both groups did not differ in their retention of the serial order of correctly recalled phonemes. Furthermore, this study confirms that expressive vocabulary skills do affect overall NRT-performance. The results are discussed in light of current short-term memory (STM) models and the role of long-term phonological knowledge in NRT tasks.

Type
Research Article
Copyright
Copyright © Cambridge University Press 2017 

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

Bates, D., & Maechler, M. (2009). lme4: Linear mixed-effects models using S4 classes [Computer software manual]. Available: http://CRAN.R-project.org/package=lme4.Google Scholar
Boets, B. (2006). Early literacy development in children at risk for dyslexia. A longitudinal study of the general magnocellular theory (Unpublished doctoral dissertation). University of Leuven, Belgium.Google Scholar
Burgess, N., & Hitch, G. J. (1999). Memory for serial order: a network model of the phonological loop and its timing. Psychological Review, 106, 551581.Google Scholar
Burgess, N. & Hitch, G.J. (2006). A revised model of short-term memory and long-term learning of verbal sequences. Journal of Memory and Language, 55, 627652.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Cheung, H. (1996). Nonword span as a unique predictor of second language vocabulary learning. Developmental Psychology, 32, 867873.Google Scholar
Clark, H. H. (1973). The language-as-fixed-effect fallacy: A critique of language statistics in psychological research. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 12, 335359.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Coady, J.A., & Evans, J.L. (2008). Uses and interpretations of non-word repetition tasks in children with and without specific language impairments (SLI). International Journal of Language and Communicative Disorders, 43, 140.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cohen, J., Cohen, P., West, S. G., & Aiken, L. S. (2003). Applied multiple regression/correlation analysis for the behavioural sciences, 3rd edition. Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.Google Scholar
Conti-Ramsden, G., & Durkin, K. (2007). Phonological short-term memory, language and literacy: developmental relationships in early adolescence in young people with SLI. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 48, 147156.Google Scholar
Cumming, N., Page, M., & Norris, D. (2003). Testing a positional model of the Hebb effect. Memory, 11, 4363.Google Scholar
Edwards, J., Beckman, M.E., & Munson, B. (2004). The interaction between vocabulary size and phonotactic probability effects on children's production accuracy and fluency in nonword repetition. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 47, 421436.Google Scholar
Gathercole, S. E. & Baddeley, A. D. (1990). Phonological memory deficits in language disordered children: Is there a causal connection? Journal of Memory & Language, 29, 336360.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gathercole, S. E., Pickering, S. J., Hall, M., & Peaker, S. J. (2001). Dissociable lexical and phonological influences on serial recognition and serial recall. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 45A, 130.Google Scholar
Gathercole, S. E. (2006). Complexities and constraints in nonword repetition and word learning. Applied Psycholinguistics, 27, 599613.Google Scholar
Girbau, D., & Schwartz, R. G. (2008). Phonological working memory in Spanish-English bilingual children with and without Specific Language Impairment. Journal of Communication Disorders, 41, 124145.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gupta, P. (2003). Examining the relationship between word learning, nonword repetition, and immediate serial recall in adults. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 56A, 12131236.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gupta, P., Lipinski, J., Abbs, B., & Lin, P-H. (2005). Serial Position Effects in Nonword Repetition. Journal of Memory and Language, 53, 141162.Google Scholar
Gupta, P. (2009). A computational model of nonword repetition, immediate serial recall, and nonword learning. In Thorn, A. & Page, M. (eds.), Interactions between short-term and long-term memory in the verbal domain, pp. 108135. Hove, England: Psychology Press.Google Scholar
Gutiérrez‐Clellen, V., & Simon‐Cereijido, G. (2010). Using Nonword Repetition Tasks for the Identification of Language Impairment in Spanish‐English‐Speaking Children: Does the Language of Assessment Matter? Learning Disabilities Research & Practice, 25, 4858.Google Scholar
Hachmann, W., Bogaerts, L., Szmalec, A., Woumans, E., Duyck, W., & Job, R. (2014). Short-term memory for order but not for item information is impaired in developmental dyslexia. Annals of dyslexia, 64, 121136.Google Scholar
Hitch, G.J., Fastame, M.C., & Flude, B. (2005). How is the serial order of a verbal sequence coded? Some comparisons between models. Memory, 3-4, 247258.Google Scholar
Hulme, C., Maughan, S., & Brown, G. D. (1991). Memory for familiar and unfamiliar words: Evidence for a long-term memory contribution to short-term memory span. Journal of Memory and Language, 30, 685701.Google Scholar
Kohnert, K., Windsor, J., & Yim, D. (2006). Do language-processing tasks separate children with language impairment from typical bilinguals? Learning Disabilities Research & Practice, 21, 1929.Google Scholar
Lanfranchi, S., & Swanson, H.L. (2005). Short-term memory and working memory in children as a function of language-specific knowledge in English and Spanish. Learning and Individual Differences, 15, 299319.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lee, S.A.S., & Gorman, B.K. (2012). Nonword repetition performance and related factors in children representing four linguistic groups. International Journal of Bilingualism, 17, 479495.Google Scholar
Majerus, S., & Cowan, N. (2016). The nature of verbal short-term impairment in dyslexia: The importance of serial order. Frontiers in Psychology, 7, 18. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2016.01522 Google Scholar
Majerus, S., Poncelet, M., Van der Linden, M., & Weekes, B. (2008). Lexical learning in bilingual adults: the relative importance of short-term memory for serial order and phonological knowledge. Cognition, 107, 395419.Google Scholar
Masoura, E. & Gathercole, S. E. (1999). Phonological short-term memory and foreign vocabulary learning. International Journal of Psychology, 34, 383388.Google Scholar
McKelvie, S. J. (1987). Learning and awareness in the Hebb digits task. Journal of General Psychology, 114, 7588.Google Scholar
Messer, M.H., Leseman, P.P.M., Mayo, A.Y., & Boom, J. (2010). Phonotactic probability effects in nonword recall and its relationship with vocabulary in monolingual and bilingual preschoolers. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 105, 306323.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Metsala, J.L., & Walley, A.C. (1998). Spoken vocabulary growth and the segmental restructuring of lexical representations: Precursors to phonemic awareness and early reading ability. In Metsala, J.L. & Ehri, L.C. (Eds.), Word recognition in beginning literacy (pp. 89120). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.Google Scholar
Michas, I. C., & Henry, L. A. (1994). The link between phonological memory and vocabulary acquisition. British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 12, 147163.Google Scholar
Molenberghs, G. and Verbeke, G. (2005). Models for Discrete Longitudinal Data. New York: Springer-Verlag.Google Scholar
Munson, B., Kurtz, B.A., & Windsor, J. (2005). The Influence of Vocabulary Size, Phonotactic Probability, and Wordlikeness on Nonword Repetitions of Children with and without Language Impairments. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 48, 10331047.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Nairne, J. S., & Kelley, M. R. (2004). Separating item and order information through process dissociation. Journal of Memory and Language, 50, 113133.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Page, M.P.A., & Norris, D. (1998). The primacy model: A new model of immediate serial recall. Psychological Review, 105, 761781.Google Scholar
Poirier, M., & Saint-Aubin, J. (1996). Immediate serial recall, word frequency, item identity and item position. Canadian Journal of Experimental Psychology, 50, 408412.Google Scholar
Schraeyen, K., Geudens, A., Ghesquire, P., Van der Elst, W., & Sandra, D. (in press). Poor performance on the retention of phonemes’ serial order in short-term memory reflects young children's poor reading skills. The Mental Lexicon.Google Scholar
Semel, E., Wiig, E.H., & Secord, W.A. (2008). Clinical Evaluation of Language Fundamentals (CELF-4NL): handleiding. Amsterdam: Pearson.Google Scholar
Sorenson Duncan, T., & Paradis, J. (2016). English Language Learners' Nonword Repetition Performance: The Influence of Age, L2 Vocabulary Size, Length of L2 Exposure, and L1 Phonology. Journal of speech, language, and hearing research, 59, 3948.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Staels, E., & Van den Broeck, W. (2014). Order short-term memory is not impaired in dyslexia and does not affect orthographic learning. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience, 8, 116.Google Scholar
Summers, C., Bohman, T.M., Gillam, R.B., Pena, E.D., & Bedore, L.M. (2010). Bilingual performance on NWR in Spanish and English. International Journal of Language and Communication Disorders, 45, 480493.Google Scholar
Thordardottir, E., Kehayia, E., Lessard, N., Sutton, A. & Trudeau, N. (2010). Typical performance on tests of language knowledge and language processing of French-speaking 5-year-olds. Canadian Journal of Speech Language Pathology and Audiology, 34, 516.Google Scholar
Thordardottir, E., & Brandeker, M. (2013). The effect of bilingual exposure versus language impairment on nonword repetition and sentence imitation scores. Journal of Communication Disorders, 46, 116.Google Scholar
Thorn, A.S.C., & Gathercole, S.E. (1999). Language-specific knowledge and short-term memory in bilingual andnon-bilingual children. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 52A, 303324.Google Scholar
Topbaş, S., & Kopkalli-Yavuz, H. (2008). Reviewing sonority for word-final sonorant+obstruent consonant cluster development in Turkish. Clinical Linguistics and Phonetics, 22, 871880.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Topbaş, S., Kaçar-Kütükçü, D., Kopkalli-Yavuz, H. (2014). Performance of children on the Turkish Nonword Repetition Test: Effect of word similarity, word length, and scoring. Clinical linguistics & phonetics, 28, 602–16.Google Scholar
Windsor, J., Kohnert, K., Lobitz, K., & Pham, G. (2010). Cross-language nonword repetition by bilingual and monolingual children. American Journal of Speech-Language Pathology, 19, 298310.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed