Hostname: page-component-cd9895bd7-gxg78 Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-12-26T07:03:38.499Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Associations between bilingualism and memory generalization during infancy: Does socioeconomic status matter?

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  22 May 2020

Natalie H. Brito*
Affiliation:
Department of Applied Psychology, New York University, New York, NY, 10003
Ashley Greaves
Affiliation:
Department of Applied Psychology, New York University, New York, NY, 10003
Ana Leon-Santos
Affiliation:
Department of Biobehavioral Sciences, Teachers College, Columbia University, New York, NY, 10027
William P. Fifer
Affiliation:
Department of Pediatrics, Columbia University Medical Center, New York, NY10032 Division of Developmental Neuroscience, New York State Psychiatric Institute, New York, NY10032
Kimberly G. Noble
Affiliation:
Department of Biobehavioral Sciences, Teachers College, Columbia University, New York, NY, 10027
*
Address for correspondence: Natalie Hiromi Brito, E-mail: [email protected]

Abstract

Past studies have reported memory differences between monolingual and bilingual infants (Brito & Barr, 2012; Singh, Fu, Rahman, Hameed, Sanmugam, Agarwal, Jiang, Chong, Meaney & Rifkin-Graboi, 2015). A common critique within the bilingualism literature is the absence of socioeconomic indicators and/or a lack of socioeconomic diversity among participants. Previous research has demonstrated robust bilingual differences in memory generalization from 6- to 24-months of age. The goal of the current study was to examine if these findings would replicate in a sample of 18-month-old monolingual and bilingual infants from a range of socioeconomic backgrounds (N = 92). Results indicate no differences between language groups on working memory or cued recall, but significant differences for memory generalization, with bilingual infants outperforming monolingual infants regardless of socioeconomic status (SES). These findings replicate and extend results from past studies (Brito & Barr, 2012; Brito, Sebastián-Gallés & Barr, 2015) and suggest possible differential learning patterns dependent on linguistic experience.

Type
Research Article
Copyright
Copyright © The Author(s), 2020. Published by Cambridge University Press

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

Amabile, TA, and Rovee-Collier, C (1991) Contextual Variation and Memory Retrieval at Six Months. Child Development 62, 1155.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Antovich, DM, and Graf Estes, K (2018) Learning across languages: Bilingual experience supports dual language statistical word segmentation. Developmental Science 21.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Arredondo, MM, Hu, X, Satterfield, T, and Kovelman, I (2016) Bilingualism alters children's fontal lobe functioning for attentional control. Developmental Science 20, e12377.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Barr, R, and Brito, N (2013) From Specificity to Flexibility: Early Developmental Changes in Memory Generalization. The Wiley Handbook on the Development of Children's Memory, 453479.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Barr, R, Muentener, P, Garcia, A, Fujimoto, M, and Chávez, V (2007) The effect of repetition on imitation from television during infancy. Developmental Psychobiology 49, 196207.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Barr, R, Marrott, H, and Rovee-Collier, C (2003) The role of sensory preconditioning in memory retrieval by preverbal infants. Animal Learning & Behavior 31, 111123.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bernier, A, Carlson, SM, and Whipple, N (2010) From External Regulation to Self-Regulation: Early Parenting Precursors of Young Children's Executive Functioning. Child Development 81, 326339.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bialystok, E, Craik, FI, and Luk, G (2012) Bilingualism: Consequences for mind and brain. Trends in Cognitive Sciences 16, 240250.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bialystok, E (1992) Selective attention in cognitive processing: The bilingual edge. In Advances in psychology, Volume 83. North-Holland, pp. 501513Google Scholar
Bialystok, E (2017) The bilingual adaptation: How minds accommodate experience. Psychological Bulletin 143, 233262.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Borovsky, D, and Rovee-Collier, C (1990) Contextual Constraints on Memory Retrieval at Six Months. Child Development 61, 1569.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bosch, L, and Sebastián-Gallés, N (2001) Evidence of Early Language Discrimination Abilities in Infants From Bilingual Environments. Infancy 2, 2949.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Brito, NH, Sebastián-Gallés, N, and Barr, R (2015) Differences in Language Exposure and its Effects on Memory Flexibility in Monolingual, Bilingual, and Trilingual Infants. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition 18, 670682.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Brito, N, and Barr, R (2014) Flexible memory retrieval in bilingual 6-month-old infants. Developmental Psychobiology 56, 11561163.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Brito, N, and Barr, R (2012) Influence of bilingualism on memory generalization during infancy. Developmental Science 15, 812816.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Brito, NH (2017) Influence of the Home Linguistic Environment on Early Language Development. Policy Insights from the Behavioral and Brain Sciences 4, 155162.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Brito, NH, Grenell, A, and Barr, R (2014) Specificity of the bilingual advantage for memory: Examining cued recall, generalization, and working memory in monolingual, bilingual, and trilingual toddlers. Frontiers in Psychology 5.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Busch, T, Sangen, A, Vanpoucke, F, and Wieringen, A van. (2017) Correlation and agreement between Language Environment Analysis (LENATM) and manual transcription for Dutch natural language recordings. Behavior Research Methods, 112. https://doi.org/10.3758/s13428-017-0960-0Google Scholar
Byers-Heinlein, K, and Werker, JF (2009) Monolingual, bilingual, trilingual: Infants' language experience influences the development of a word-learning heuristic. Developmental Science 12, 815823.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Canault, M, Le Normand, M-T, Foudil, S, Loundon, N, and Thai-Van, H (2015) Reliability of the Language ENvironment Analysis system (LENATM) in European French. Behavior Research Methods. https://doi.org/10.3758/s13428-015-0634-8Google Scholar
Castro-Vázquez, G (2009) Immigrant children from Latin America at Japanese schools: Homogeneity, ethnicity, gender and language in education. Journal of Research in International Education 8, 5780.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Calvo, A, and Bialystok, E (2014) Independent effects of bilingualism and socioeconomic status on language ability and executive functioning. Cognition 130, 278288.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cat, CD, Gusnanto, A, and Serratrice, L (2018) Identifying A Threshold For The Executive Function Advantage In Bilingual Children. Studies in Second Language Acquisition 40, 119151.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Christofides, LN, and Swidinsky, R (2010) The Economic Returns to the Knowledge and Use of a Second Official Language: English in Quebec and French in the Rest-of-Canada. Canadian Public Policy 36, 137158.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Clearfield, MW, and Niman, LC (2012) SES affects infant cognitive flexibility. Infant Behavior and Development 35, 2935.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Comishen, KJ, Bialystok, E, and Adler, SA (2019) The impact of bilingual environments on selective attention in infancy. Developmental Science 22.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Costa, A, and Sebastián-Gallés, N (2014) How does the bilingual experience sculpt the brain? Nature Reviews Neuroscience 15, 336345.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cummins, J (1976) The influence of bilingualism on cognitive growth: A synthesis of research findings and explanatory hypotheses. Working Papers on Bilingualism 9, 143.Google Scholar
Cummins, J (1979) Linguistic Interdependence and the Educational Development of Bilingual Children. Review of Educational Research 49, 222.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
D'Angiulli, A, Herdman, A, Stapells, D, and Hertzman, C (2008) Children's event-related potentials of auditory selective attention vary with their socioeconomic status. Neuropsychology 22( 3), 292300.Google Scholar
Eichenbaum, H (1997) Declarative Memory: Insights from Cognitive Neurobiology. Annual Review of Psychology 48, 547572.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Frankenhuis, WE, & Nettle, D (2020) The strengths of people in poverty. Current Directions in Psychological Science 29, 1621.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Fenson, L, Pethick, S, Renda, C, Cox, JL, Dale, PS, and Reznick, JS (2000) Short-form versions of the MacArthur Communicative Development Inventories. Applied Psycholinguistics 21, 95116.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Garcia, LN, Guerrero-Mosquera, C, Colomer, M, and Sebastián-Gallés, N (2018) Evoked and oscillatory EEG activity differentiates language discrimination in young monolingual and bilingual infants. Scientific Reports 8, 2770.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gervain, J, and Werker, JF (2013) Prosody cues word order in 7-month-old bilingual infants. Nature Communications 4.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gilkerson, J, Richards, JA, Warren, SF, Montgomery, JK, Greenwood, CR, Oller, DK, Hansen, JHL, and Paul, TD (2017) Mapping the Early Language Environment Using All-Day Recordings and Automated Analysis. American Journal of Speech-Language Pathology 26, 248265.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gilkerson, J, Zhang, Y, Xu, D, Richards, JA, Xu, X, Jiang, F, Harnsberger, J, and Topping, K (2015) Evaluating Language Environment Analysis system performance for Chinese: A pilot study in Shanghai. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research 58, 445452. https://doi.org/10.1044/2015_JSLHR-L-14-0014CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Greco, C, Hayne, H, and Rovee-Collier, C (1990) Roles of function, reminding, and variability in categorization by 3-month-old infants. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition 16, 617633.Google ScholarPubMed
Green, DW, and Abutalebi, J (2013) Language control in bilinguals: The adaptive control hypothesis. Journal of Cognitive Psychology 25, 515530.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hackman, DA, Gallop, R, Evans, GW, and Farah, MJ (2015) Socioeconomic status 92 and executive function: developmental trajectories and mediation. Developmental Science 18(5), 686702. https://doi.org/10.1111/desc.12246CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hartanto, A, Toh, WX, and Yang, H (2018) Bilingualism Narrows Socioeconomic Disparities in Executive Functions and Self-Regulatory Behaviors During Early Childhood: Evidence From the Early Childhood Longitudinal Study. Child Development.Google ScholarPubMed
Hayne, H (2006) Age-related changes in infant memory retrieval: Implications for knowledge transfer. In Munakata, Y & Johnson, JH (eds), Processes of change in brain and cognitive development. Attention and Performance XXI. Oxford: University Press, pp. 209231.Google Scholar
Hayne, H, Macdonald, S, and Barr, R (1997) Developmental changes in the specificity of memory over the second year of life. Infant Behavior and Development 20, 233245.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Herbert, J, Gross, J, and Hayne, H (2007) Crawling is associated with more flexible memory retrieval by 9-month-old infants. Developmental Science 10, 183189.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Herbert, J, and Hayne, H (2000) Memory retrieval by 18–30-month-olds: Age-related changes in representational flexibility. Developmental Psychology 36, 473484.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hoff-Ginsberg, E (1998) The relation of birth order and socioeconomic status to children's language experience and language development. Applied Psycholinguistics 19, 603629.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hoff, E (2006) How social contexts support and shape language development. Developmental Review 26, 5588.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hoff, E (2003) The Specificity of Environmental Influence: Socioeconomic Status Affects Early Vocabulary Development Via Maternal Speech. Child Development 74, 13681378.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Huttenlocher, J, Waterfall, H, Vasilyeva, M, Vevea, J, and Hedges, LV (2010) Sources of variability in children's language growth. Cognitive Psychology 61, 343365.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Johnson, EJ, Avineri, N, and Johnson, DC (2017) Exposing Gaps in/Between Discourses of Linguistic Deficits. International Multilingual Research Journal 11, 522.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kim, P, Evans, GW, Angstadt, M, Ho, SS, Sripada, CS, Swain, JE, Liberzon, I, and Phan, KL (2013) Effects of childhood poverty and chronic stress on emotion regulatory brain function in adulthood. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences 110, 1844218447.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kovács, AM, and Mehler, J (2009) Flexible Learning of Multiple Speech Structures in Bilingual Infants. Science 325, 611612.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Krizman, J, Skoe, E, Marian, V, and Kraus, N (2014) Bilingualism increases neural response consistency and attentional control: Evidence for sensory and cognitive coupling. Brain and language 128(1), 3440.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kuipers, J, and Thierry, G (2013) ERP-pupil size correlations reveal how bilingualism enhances cognitive flexibility. Cortex 49, 28532860.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Learmonth, AE, Lamberth, R, and Rovee-Collier, C (2004) Generalization of deferred imitation during the first year of life. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology 88, 297318.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Mattock, K, Polka, L, Rvachew, S, and Krehm, M (2010) The first steps in word learning are easier when the shoes fit: Comparing monolingual and bilingual infants. Developmental Science 13, 229243.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Morris, N, and Jones, DM (1990) Memory updating in working memory: The role of the central executive. British Journal of Psychology 81, 111121.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Morton, JB, and Harper, SN (2007) What did Simon say? Revisiting the bilingual advantage. Developmental Science 10, 719726.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Noble, KG, Engelhardt, LE, Brito, NH, Mack, LJ, Nail, EJ, Angal, J, Barr, R, Fifer, WP, and Elliott, AJ (2015a) Socioeconomic disparities in neurocognitive development in the first two years of life. Developmental Psychobiology 57, 535551.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Noble, KG, Houston, SM, Brito, NH, Bartsch, H, Kan, E, Kuperman, JM, Akshoomoff, N, Amaral, DG, Bloss, CS, Libiger, O, Schork, NJ, Murray, SS, Casey, BJ, Chang, L, Ernst, TM, Frazier, JA, Gruen, JR, Kennedy, DN, Van Zijl, P, Mostofsky, S, Kaufmann, WE, Kenet, T, Dale, AM, Jernigan, TL, and Sowell, ER (2015b) Family income, parental education and brain structure in children and adolescents. Nature Neuroscience 18, 773778.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Noble, KG, Norman, MF, and Farah, MJ (2005) Neurocognitive correlates of socioeconomic status in kindergarten children. Developmental Science 8, 7487.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Paap, KR, and Greenberg, ZI (2013) There is no coherent evidence for a bilingual advantage in executive processing. Cognitive Psychology 66, 232258.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Pae, S, Yoon, H, Seol, A, Gilkerson, J, Richards, JA, Ma, L, and Topping, K (2016) Effects of feedback on parent–child language with infants and toddlers in Korea. First Language 36, 549569. https://doi.org/10.1177/0142723716649273CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Pearson, BZ, Fernández, SC, and Oller, DK (1993) Lexical Development in Bilingual Infants and Toddlers: Comparison to Monolingual Norms. Language Learning 43, 93120.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Place, S, and Hoff, E (2011) Properties of Dual Language Exposure That Influence 2-Year-Olds’ Bilingual Proficiency. Child Development 82, 18341849.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Poulin-Dubois, D, Blaye, A, Coutya, J, and Bialystok, E (2011) The effects of bilingualism on toddlers’ executive functioning. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology 108, 567579.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Putnam, SP, and Rothbart, MK (2006) Development of Short and Very Short Forms of the Children's Behavior Questionnaire. Journal of Personality Assessment 87, 102112.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Raizada, RD, Richards, TL, Meltzoff, A, and Kuhl, PK (2008) Socioeconomic status predicts hemispheric specialisation of the left inferior frontal gyrus in young children. NeuroImage 40, 13921401.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ramírez-Esparza, N, García-Sierra, A, and Kuhl, PK (2017) Look who's talking NOW! Parentese speech, social context, and language development across time. Frontiers in psychology 8, 1008.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Romeo, RR, Segaran, J, Leonard, JA, Robinson, ST, West, MR, Mackey, AP, Yendiki, A, Rowe, ML, and Gabrieli, JD (2018) Language Exposure Relates to Structural Neural Connectivity in Childhood. The Journal of Neuroscience 38, 78707877.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rovee-Collier, C, and Dufault, D (1991) Multiple contexts and memory retrieval at three months. Developmental Psychobiology 24, 3949.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rowe, ML (2008) Child-directed speech: Relation to socioeconomic status, knowledge of child development and child vocabulary skill. Journal of Child Language 35, 185205.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Sebastián-Gallés, N, Albareda-Castellot, B, Weikum, WM, and Werker, JF (2012) A Bilingual Advantage in Visual Language Discrimination in Infancy. Psychological Science 23, 994999.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Sheridan, MA, Sarsour, K, Jutte, D, D'esposito, M, and Boyce, WT (2012) The Impact of Social Disparity on Prefrontal Function in Childhood. PLoS ONE 7.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Singh, L, Fu, CS, Rahman, AA, Hameed, WB, Sanmugam, S, Agarwal, P, Jiang, B, Chong, YS, Meaney, MJ, and Rifkin-Graboi, A (2015) Back to Basics: A Bilingual Advantage in Infant Visual Habituation. Child Development 86, 294302.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Smith, EE, and Jonides, J (1998) Neuroimaging analyses of human working memory. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences 95, 12061–12068.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Spear, NE (1984) The future study of learning and memory from a psychobiological perspective. In: Sarris, V and Parducci, A (eds), Perspectives in psychological experimentation. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum, pp. 87103.Google Scholar
Stevens, C, Lauinger, B, and Neville, H (2009) Differences in the neural mechanisms of selective attention in children from different socioeconomic backgrounds: An event-related brain potential study. Developmental Science 12, 634646.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Tulving, E, and Thomson, DM (1973) Encoding specificity and retrieval processes in episodic memory. Psychological Review 80, 352373.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Weisleder, A, and Fernald, A (2013) Talking to children matters: early language experience strengthens processing and builds vocabulary. Psychological Science 24, 21432152. https://doi.org/10.1177/0956797613488145CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Werker, J (2012) Perceptual foundations of bilingual acquisition in infancy. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences 1251, 5061.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Yoshida, KA, Iversen, JR, Patel, AD, Mazuka, R, Nito, H, Gervain, J, and Werker, JF (2010) The development of perceptual grouping biases in infancy: A Japanese-English cross-linguistic study. Cognition 115, 356361.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed