Hostname: page-component-cd9895bd7-gxg78 Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-12-18T11:05:26.440Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Are there cognitive benefits of code-switching in bilingual children? A longitudinal study

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  03 June 2019

Olivia Kuzyk
Affiliation:
Concordia University
Margaret Friend
Affiliation:
San Diego State University
Vivianne Severdija
Affiliation:
Concordia University
Pascal Zesiger
Affiliation:
Université de Genève
Diane Poulin-Dubois*
Affiliation:
Concordia University
*
Address for correspondence: Diane Poulin-Dubois, E-mail: [email protected]

Abstract

The current study explored bilingual parent and child code-switching patterns over time. Concurrent and predictive models of code-switching behaviour on executive function outcomes were also examined in a sample of 29 French–English bilinguals at 36 (Wave 1) and 61 (Wave 2) months of age. We investigated whether code-switching typology in a single-language context predicted executive function performance at each wave independently, and whether growth in code-switching frequency across waves predicted executive function performance at Wave 2. At both waves, parents and children participated in two free play sessions (in English and French), followed by a battery of executive function tasks administered in the dominant language. Results indicate more frequent code-switching from the non-dominant to the dominant language in children, and that children code-switch to fill lexical gaps. Results also suggest that less frequent code-switching in a single-language context is associated with better inhibitory control skills during the preschool period.

Type
Research Article
Copyright
Copyright © Cambridge University Press 2019

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

Abutalebi, J, Della Rosa, PA, Ding, G, Weekes, B, Costa, A and Green, DW (2013) Language proficiency modulates the engagement of cognitive control areas in multilinguals. Cortex: A Journal Devoted To The Study Of The Nervous System And Behavior 49, 905911. doi:10.1016/j.cortex.2012.08.018CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Adesope, OO, Lavin, T, Thompson, T and Ungerleider, C (2010) A systematic review and meta-analysis of the cognitive correlates of bilingualism. Review of Educational Research 80, 207245. doi:10.3102/0034654310368803CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bail, A, Morini, G and Newman, RS (2015) Look at the gato! Code-switching in speech to toddlers. Journal of Child Language 42, 10731101, doi:10.1017/S0305000914000695CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Barac, R, Bialystok, E, Castro, DC and Sanchez, M (2014) The cognitive development of young dual language learners: A critical review. Early Research Quarterly 29, 699714. doi:10.1016/j.ecresq.2014.02.003CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Benjamini, Y and Hochberg, Y (1995) Controlling the false discovery rate: A practical and powerful approach to multiple testing. Journal of the Royal Statistical Society. Series B (Methodological) 57, 289300. doi:10.2307/2346101CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bialystok, E (2004) [Review of the book One mind, two languages: Bilingual language processing, Janet L. Nicol (Ed.)]. Language in Society, 33, 124–127. doi:10.1017/S0047404504251053CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bialystok, E (2017) The bilingual adaptation: How minds accommodate experience. Psychological Bulletin 143, 233262. doi:10.1037/bul0000099CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bialystok, E, Craik, FM, Klein, R and Viswanathan, M (2004) Bilingualism, aging, and cognitive control: Evidence from the Simon task. Psychology and Aging 19, 290303. doi:10.1037/0882-7974.19.2.290CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bialystok, E, Craik, F and Luk, G (2009) Cognitive control and lexical access in younger and older bilinguals. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition 35, 828. doi:10.1037/a0015638Google Scholar
Bialystok, E, Craik, F and Luk, G (2012) Bilingualism: Consequences for mind and brain. Trends in Cognitive Sciences 16, 240250. doi:10.1016/j.tics.2012.03.001CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bobb, SC and Wodniecka, Z (2013) Language switching in picture naming: What asymmetric switch costs (do not) tell us about inhibition in bilingual speech planning. Journal of Cognitive Psychology 25, 568585. doi:10.1080/20445911.2013.792822CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Byers-Heinlein, K (2013) Parental language mixing: Its measurement and the relation of mixed input to young bilingual children's vocabulary size. Bilingualism 16, 3248. doi:10.1017/S1366728912000120CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Carlson, SM and Meltzoff, AN (2008) Bilingual experience and executive functioning in young children. Developmental Science 11, 282298. doi:10.1111/j.1467-7687.2008.00675.xCrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Costa, A and Santesteban, M (2004) Lexical access in bilingual speech production: Evidence from language switching in highly proficient bilinguals and L2 learners. Journal of Memory and Language 50, 491511. doi:10.1016/j.jml.2004.02.002CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Crivello, C, Kuzyk, O, Rodrigues, M, Friend, M, Zesiger, P and Poulin-Dubois, D (2016) The effects of bilingual growth on toddlers’ executive function. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology 141, 121132. doi:10.1016/j.jecp.2015.08.004CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
de Bruin, A, Bak, TH and Della Sala, S (2015) Examining the effects of active versus inactive bilingualism on executive control in a carefully matched non-immigrant sample. Journal of Memory and Language 85, 1526. doi:10.1016/j.jml.2015.07.001CrossRefGoogle Scholar
DeAnda, S, Bosch, L, Poulin-Dubois, D, Zesiger, P and Friend, M (2016) The language exposure assessment tool: Quantifying language exposure in infants and children. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research 59, 111. doi:10.1044/2016_JSLHR-L-15-0234CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Declerck, M, Grainger, J, Koch, I and Philipp, AM (2017a) Is language control just a form of executive control? Evidence for overlapping processes in language switching and task switching. Journal of Memory and Language 95, 138145. doi:10.1016/j.jml.2017.03.005CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Declerck, M, Koch, I and Philipp, AM (2015) The minimum requirements of language control: Evidence from sequential predictability effects in language switching. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory and Cognition 41, 377394. doi:10.1037/xlm0000021Google ScholarPubMed
Declerck, M, Lemhöfer, K and Grainger, J (2017b) Bilingual language interference initiates error detection: Evidence from language intrusions. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition 20, 10101016. doi:10.1017/S1366728916000845CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Declerck, M and Philipp, AM (2015) A review of control processes and their locus in language switching. Psychonomic Bulletin and Review 22, 16301645. doi:10.3758/s13423-015-0836-1CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Deuchar, M and Quay, S (2000) Bilingual acquisition: Theoretical implications of a case. New York, NY: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Duñabeitia, J.A., Hernández, JA, Antón, E, Macizo, P, Estévez, A, Fuentes, LJ and Carreiras, M (2014) The inhibitory advantage in bilingual children revisited: Myth or reality? Experimental Psychology 61, 118. doi:10.1027/1618-3169/a000243CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Dunn, LM and Dunn, LM (1997) Peabody Picture Vocabulary Test. Circle Pines, MN: American Guidance Service.Google Scholar
Dunn, LM, Thériault-Whalen, CM, Dunn, LM (1993) Échelle de vocabulaire en images Peabody. Toronto, Canada: Psycan.Google Scholar
Engel de Abreu, PJ, Cruz-Santos, A, Tourinho, CJ, Martin, R and Bialystok, E (2012) Bilingualism enriches the poor: Enhanced cognitive control in low-income minority children. Psychological Science 23, 13641371. doi:10.1177/0956797612443836CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Genesee, F (2004) What do we know about bilingual education for majority language students? In Bhatia, TK and Ritchie, W (eds), Handbook of Bilingualism and Multiculturalism. Malden, MA: Blackwell, pp. 547576.Google Scholar
Genesee, F (2006) Bilingual first language acquisition in perspective. In McCardle, P and Hoff, E (eds), Childhood bilingualism: Research on infancy through school age. Clevedon, UK: Multilingual Matters, pp. 4567.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Genesee, F, Nicoladis, E and Paradis, J (1995a) Language differentiation in early bilingual development. Journal of Child Language 22, 611631. doi:10.1017/S0305000900009971CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Genesee, F, Paradis, J and Wolf, L (1995b) Lexical development in young preschool bilinguals. Unpublished paper, Psychology Department, McGill University.Google Scholar
Goldsmith, SF and Morton, JB (2018) Time to disengage from the bilingual advantage hypothesis. Cognition 170, 328329. doi:10.1016/j.cognition.2017.09.011CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gollan, TH and Ferreira, VS (2009) Should I stay or should I switch? A cost–benefit analysis of voluntary language switching in young and aging bilinguals. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition 35, 640665. doi:10.1037/a0014981Google ScholarPubMed
Green, DW (1998) Mental control of the bilingual lexico-semantic system. Language and Cognition 1, 6781. doi:101017S1366728998000133CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Green, DW and Abutalebi, J (2013) Language control in bilinguals: The adaptive control hypothesis. Journal of Cognitive Psychology 25, 515530. doi:10.1080/20445911.2013.796377Google ScholarPubMed
Green, DW and Wei, L (2014) A control process model of code-switching. Language, Cognition and Neuroscience 29, 499511. doi:10.1080/23273798.2014.882515CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gross, DW and Kaushanskaya, M (2015) Voluntary language switching in English-Spanish bilingual children. Journal of Cognitive Psychology 27, 9921013. doi:10.1080/20445911.2015.1074242CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hartanto, A and Yang, H (2016) Disparate bilingual experiences modulate task-switching advantages: A diffusion-model analysis of the effects of interactional context on switch costs. Cognition 150, 1019. doi:10.1016/j.cognition.2016.01.016CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hernández, M, Costa, A, Fuentes, LJ, Vivas, AB and Sebastián-Gallés, N (2010) The impact of bilingualism on the executive control and orienting networks of attention. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition 13, 315325. doi:10.1017/S1366728909990010CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Jackson, GM, Swainson, R, Cunnington, R and Jackson, SR (2001) ERP correlates of executive control during repeated language switching. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition 4, 169178. doi:10.1017/S1366728901000268CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Jia, G, Kohnert, K, Collado, J and Aquino-Garcia, E (2006) Action naming in Spanish and English by sequential bilingual children and adolescents. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research 49, 588602. doi:10.1044/1092-4388(2006/042)CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kohnert, K (2002) Picture naming in early sequential bilinguals: A 1-year follow-up. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research 45, 759771. doi:10.1044/1092-4388(2002/061)CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kohnert, K, Bates, E and Hernandez, AE (1999) Balancing bilinguals: Lexical-semantic production and cognitive processing in children learning Spanish and English. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research 42, 14001413. doi:10.1044/1092-4388(2002/027)CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kovács, ÁM and Mehler, J (2009) Cognitive gains in 7-month-old bilingual infants. Proceedings Of The National Academy Of Sciences Of The United States Of America 106, 65566560. doi:10.1073/pnas.0811323106CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Linck, JA, Kroll, JF and Sunderman, G (2009) Losing access to the native language while immersed in a second language evidence for the role of inhibition in second-language learning. Psychological Science 20, 15071515. doi:10.1111/j.1467-9280.2009.02480.xCrossRefGoogle Scholar
Litcofsky, KA and Van Hell, JG (2017) Switching direction affects switching costs: Behavioural, ERP and time-frequency analyses of intra-sentential codeswitching. Neuropsychologia 97, 112139. doi:10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2017.02.002CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Luk, G and Bialystok, E (2013) Bilingualism is not a categorical variable: Interaction between language proficiency and usage. Journal of Cognitive Psychology 25, 605621. doi:10.1080/20445911.2013.795574CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Luk, G, Green, DW, Abutalebi, J and Grady, C (2011) Cognitive control for language switching in bilinguals: A quantitative meta-analysis of functional neuroimaging studies. Language and cognitive processes 27(10), 14791488. doi:10.1080/01690965.2011.613209CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Martin-Rhee, M and Bialystok, E (2008) The development of two types of inhibitory control in monolingual and bilingual children. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition 11, 8193. doi:10.1017/S1366728907003227CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Meuter, RFI and Allport, A (1999) Bilingual language switching in naming: Asymmetrical costs of language selection. Journal of Memory and Language 40, 2540. doi:10.1006/jmla.1998.2602CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Miller, JF, Andriacchi, K, Nockerts, A, Westerveld, M and Gillon, G (2012) Assessing language production using SALT Software. A clinician's guide to language sample analysis. New Zealand version. Australia: SALT Software LLC.Google Scholar
Miyake, A, Friedman, NP, Emerson, MJ, Witzki, AH and Howerter, A (2000) The unity and diversity of executive functions and their contributions to complex ‘frontal lobe’ tasks: A latent variable analysis. Cognitive Psychology 41, 49100. doi:10.1006/cogp.1999.0734CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Miyake, A, Friedman, NP, Rettinger, DA, Shah, P and Hegarty, M (2001) How are visuospatial working memory, executive functioning, and spatial abilities related? A latent-variable analysis. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General 130, 621640. doi:10.1037/0096-3445.130.4.621CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Morales, J, Calvo, A and Bialystok, E (2013) Working memory development in monolingual and bilingual children. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology 114, 187202. doi:10.1016/j.jecp.2012.09.002CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Morton, JB and Harper, SN (2007) What did Simon say? Revisiting the bilingual advantage. Developmental Science 10, 719726. doi:10.1111/j.1467-7687.2007.00623.xCrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Myers-Scotton, C (1997) Duelling Language, grammatical structure in codeswitching. Oxford, England: Clarendon Press.Google Scholar
Nicoladis, E (1995) Code-mixing in young bilingual children (Unpublished doctoral dissertation). McGill University, Montreal, Quebec.Google Scholar
Nicoladis, E and Secco, G (2000) The role of a child's productive vocabulary in the language choice of a bilingual family. First Language 20, 328. doi:10.1177/014272370002005801CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Paap, KR and Greenberg, ZI (2013) There is no coherent evidence for a bilingual advantage in executive processing. Cognitive Psychology 66, 232258. doi:10.1016/j.cogpsych.2012.12.002CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Philipp, AM, Gade, M and Koch, I (2007) Inhibitory processes in language switching: Evidence from switching language-defined response sets. European Journal of Cognitive Psychology 19, 395416. doi:10.1080/09541440600758812CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Philipp, AM and Koch, I (2009) Inhibition in language switching: What is inhibited when switching between languages in naming tasks? Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition 35, 11871195. doi:10.1037/a0016376Google ScholarPubMed
Philipp, AM and Koch, I (2016) Action speaks louder than words, even in speaking: The influence of (no) overt speech production on language-switch costs. In Schwieter, JW (ed), Cognitive control and consequences in the multilingual mind. Philadelphia, PA: John Benjamins, pp. 127144.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Poarch, GJ and Bialystok, E (2015) Bilingualism as a model for multitasking. Developmental Review 35, 113124. doi:10.1016/j.dr.2014.12.003CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Poulin-Dubois, D, Bialystok, E, Blaye, A, Polonia, A and Yott, J (2012) Lexical access and vocabulary development in very young bilinguals. The International Journal of Bilingualism: Cross-Disciplinary, Cross-Linguistic Studies of Language Behaviour 17, 5770. doi:10.1177/1367006911431198CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Poulin-Dubois, D, Blaye, A, Coutya, J and Bialystok, E (2011) The effects of bilingualism on toddlers’ executive functioning. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology 108, 567579. doi:10.1016/j.jecp.2010.10.009CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Prior, A and Gollan, TH (2011) Good language-switchers are good task-switchers: evidence from Spanish-English and Mandarin-English bilinguals. Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society 17, 682691. doi:10.1017/S1355617711000580CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Prior, A, Goldwasser, N, Ravet-Hirsh, R and Schwartz, M (2016) Executive functions in bilingual children: Is there a role for language balance? In Schwieter, JW (ed), Cognitive control and consequences of multilingualism. Philadelphia, PA: John Benjamins, pp. 323350.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ribot, KM and Hoff, E (2014) “¿Cómo estas?” “I'm good.” Conversational code-switching is related to profiles of expressive and receptive proficiency in Spanish-English bilingual toddlers. International Journal of Behavioural Development 38, 333341. doi:10.1177/0165025414533225CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Schwieter, JW and Sunderman, G (2008) Language switching in bilingual speech production: In search of the language-specific selection mechanism. The Mental Lexicon 3, 214238. doi:10.1075/ml.3.2.06schCrossRefGoogle Scholar
Singh, L, Fu, CL, Rahman, AA, Hameed, WB, Sanmugam, S, Agarwal, P, Jiang, B, Chong, YS, Meaney, MJ and Rifkin-Graboi, A (2015) Back to basics: A bilingual advantage in infant visual habituation. Child Development 86, 294302. doi:10.1111/cdev.12271CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Singh, N and Mishra, RK (2012) Does language proficiency modulate oculomotor control? Evidence from Hindi–English bilinguals. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition 15, 771781. doi:10.1017/S1366728912000065CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Stasenko, A, Matt, GE and Gollan, TH (2017) A relative bilingual advantage in switching with preparation: Nuanced explorations of the proposed association between bilingualism and task switching. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General 146, 15271550. doi:10.1037/xge0000340CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Tare, M and Gelman, SA (2011) Bilingual parents' modeling of pragmatic language use in multiparty interactions. Applied Psycholinguistics 32, 761780. doi:10.1017/S0142716411000051CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Tarlowski, A, Wodniecka, Z and Marzecová, A (2013) Language switching in the production of phrases. Journal of Psycholinguistic Research 42, 103118. doi:10.1007/s10936-012-9203-9CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Thierry, G and Wu, YJ (2007) Brain potentials reveal unconscious translation during foreign-language comprehension. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences (PNAS) 104, 1253012535. doi:10.1073/pnas.0609927104CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
van Heuven, WJB and Dijkstra, T (2010) Language comprehension in the bilingual brain: fMRI and ERP support psycholinguistic models. Brain Research Reviews 64, 104122. doi:10.1016/j.brainresrev.2010.03.002CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
van Heuven, WJB, Dijkstra, T and Grainger, J (1998) Orthographic neighborhood effects in bilingual word recognition. Journal of Memory and Language 39, 458483. doi:10.1006/jmla.1998.2584CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Verreyt, N, Woumans, E, Vandelanotte, D, Szmalec, A and Duyck, W (2016) The influence of language-switching experience on the bilingual executive control advantage. Bilingualism 19, 181190. doi:10.1017/S1366728914000352CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Yang, H, Hartanto, A and Yang, S (2016) The complex nature of bilinguals' language usage modulates task-switching outcomes. Frontiers in Psychology 7. doi:10.3389/fpsyg.2016.00560CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Zelazo, PD, Anderson, JE, Richler, J, Wallner-Allen, K, Beaumont, JL and Weintraub, S (2013) NIH toolbox cognition battery (CB): Measuring executive function and attention. Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development 78, 1633. doi:10.1111/mono.12032CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Zelazo, PD, Muller, U, Frye, D, Marcovitch, S, Argitis, G, Boseovski, J, Chiang, JK, Hongwanishkul, D, Schuster, BV and Sutherland, A (2003) The development of executive function in early childhood. Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development 68, 78. doi:10.1111/j.1540-5834.2003.06803001.xCrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed