Hostname: page-component-cd9895bd7-mkpzs Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-12-27T05:42:44.061Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Intention and action in retirement preparation

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  30 October 2018

JOB M.T. KRIJNEN*
Affiliation:
UCLA Anderson School of Management, Los Angeles, CA, USA
MARCEL ZEELENBERG
Affiliation:
Department of Social Psychology, Tilburg University, Tilburg, The Netherlands and Department of Marketing, Vrije Universiteit Amsterdam, Amsterdam, The Netherlands
SEGER M. BREUGELMANS
Affiliation:
Department of Social Psychology, Tilburg University, Tilburg, The Netherlands
ANNA VAN DER SCHORS
Affiliation:
National Institute for Family Finance Information (Nibud), Utrecht, The Netherlands
*
*Correspondence to: UCLA Anderson School of Management, 110 Westwood Plaza, Los Angeles, CA90095, USA. Email: [email protected]

Abstract

Many people delay their preparation for retirement. Policy-makers often attempt to motivate people to take timely action by increasing the perceived importance of retirement saving, yet the effectiveness of such strategies can be doubted. We examined why a strategy of emphasizing importance may be ineffective by distinguishing between intention to prepare for retirement and action in actually taking steps toward preparation. Two surveys (n1 = 1171; n2 = 832) showed that importance and difficulty were both predictive of people's intentions to prepare for retirement, but that difficulty was a much stronger predictor of people's actual actions. Using data from an additional survey (n3 = 986), a series of follow-up tests provided further evidence that difficulty of retirement preparation is a stronger predictor of inaction than importance of retirement saving. These findings help explain why policies aimed at simplifying retirement preparation (e.g., automatic enrollment) have been more successful than policies aimed at increasing the importance of retirement saving (e.g., tax advantages).

Type
Article
Copyright
Copyright © Cambridge University Press 2018

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

Ainslie, G. (1975), ‘Specious reward: A behavioral theory of impulsiveness and impulse control’, Psychological Bulletin, 82(4): 463482.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ajzen, I. (1991), ‘The theory of planned behavior’, Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes, 50(2): 179211.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Akerlof, G. A. (1991), ‘Procrastination and obedience’, American Economic Review, 81(2): 119.Google Scholar
Ameriks, J., Caplin, A., and Leahy, J. (2003), ‘Wealth accumulation and the propensity to plan’, Quarterly Journal of Economics, 118(3): 10071047.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Antolín, P., De Serres, A., and De la Maisonneuve, C. (2004), Long-term budgetary implications of tax-favoured retirement saving plans, OECD Economic Studies No. 39.Google Scholar
Attanasio, O. P., Banks, J. and Wakefield, M. (2004), Effectiveness of tax incentives to boost (retirement) saving: Theoretical motivation and empirical evidence, The Institute for Fiscal Studies Working Paper 4.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bell, C. (2016), Survey: Most Americans have financial regrets, particularly about saving. Retrieved from http://www.bankrate.com/finance/consumer-index/financial-security-charts-0516.aspx.Google Scholar
Benartzi, S., Beshears, J., Milkman, K. L., Sunstein, C. R., Thaler, R. H., Shankar, M. and Galing, S. (2017), ‘Should governments invest more in nudging?’, Psychological Science, 28(8): 10411055.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Benartzi, S. and Thaler, R. H. (2013), ‘Behavioral economics and the retirement savings crisis’, Science, 339(6124): 11521153.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Beshears, J., Choi, J. J., Laibson, D. and Madrian, B. C. (2010), ‘The impact of employer matching on savings plan participation under automatic enrollment’, In Wise, D. A. (Ed.), Research findings in the economics of aging, (pp. 311327). Chicago, IL: University of Chicago Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Butt, A., Donald, M. S., Foster, F. D., Thorp, S. and Warren, G. J. (2018), ‘One size fits all? Tailoring retirement plan defaults’, Journal of Economic Behavior & Organization, 145: 546566.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Carroll, G. D., Choi, J. J., Laibson, D., Madrian, B. C. and Metrick, A. (2009), ‘Optimal defaults and active decisions’, Quarterly Journal of Economics, 124(4): 16391674.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Chetty, R., Friedman, J. N., Leth-Peterson, S., Nielsen, T. and Olsen, T. (2014), ‘Active vs. passive decisions and crowd-out in retirement savings accounts: Evidence from Denmark’, Quarterly Journal of Economics, 129(3): 11411219.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Choi, J. J., Laibson, D., and Madrian, B. C. (2011), ‘$100 bills on the sidewalk: Suboptimal investment in 401(k) plans’, Review of Economics and Statistics, 93(3): 748763.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Choi, J. J., Laibson, D., Madrian, B. C., and Metrick, A. (2002), ‘Defined contribution pensions: Plan rules, participant decisions and the path of least resistance’, In Poterba, J. M. (Ed.), Tax policy and the economy, Vol. 16, (pp. 67113). Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Engelhardt, G. V., and Kumar, A. (2007), ‘Employer matching and 401(k) saving: Evidence from the health and retirement study’, Journal of Public Economics, 91(10): 19201943.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Fernandes, D., Lynch, J. G. Jr, and Netemeyer, R. G. (2014), ‘Financial literacy, financial education, and downstream financial behaviors’, Management Science, 60(8): 18611883.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Fischer, C. (1999), Read this paper even later: Procrastination with time-inconsistent preferences, Resources for the Future Discussion Paper 99–20.Google Scholar
Gallup (2016), Americans' Financial Worries Edge Up in 2016. Retrieved from https://news.gallup.com/poll/191174/americans-financial-worries-edge-2016.aspx.Google Scholar
Gallup (2017a), Americans' Financial Anxieties Ease in 2017. Retrieved from https://news.gallup.com/poll/210890/americans-financial-anxieties-ease-2017.aspx.Google Scholar
Gallup (2017b), Financial Worry (Trends). Retrieved from https://news.gallup.com/poll/210893/financial-worry-trends.aspx.Google Scholar
Gollwitzer, P. M. (1999), ‘Implementation intentions: Strong effects of simple plans’, American Psychologist, 54(7): 493503.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hadar, L., Sood, S., and Fox, C. R. (2013), ‘Subjective knowledge in consumer financial decisions’, Journal of Marketing Research, 50(3): 303316.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hershfield, H. E., John, E. M. and Reiff, J. S. (2018), ‘Using vividness interventions to improve financial decision making’, Policy Insights from the Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 5(2): 209215.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Keim, D. B., and Mitchell, O. S. (2018), ‘Simplifying choices in defined contribution retirement plan design: A case study’, Journal of Pension Economics & Finance, 17(3): 363384.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kim, K. T. and Hanna, S. D. (2013), ‘Does financial sophistication matter in retirement preparedness of U.S. households? Evidence from the 2010 Survey of Consumer Finances’, Consumer Interests Annual, 59. Retrieved from https://papers.ssrn.com/sol3/papers.cfm?abstract_id=2291600.Google Scholar
Krijnen, J. M. T., Zeelenberg, M. and Breugelmans, S. M. (2015), ‘Decision importance as a cue for deferral’, Judgment and Decision Making, 10(5): 407415.Google Scholar
Krijnen, J. M. T., Zeelenberg, M. and Breugelmans, S. M. (2016), Overcoming inertia in retirement saving: Why now and how?, Netspar Survey Paper 47.Google Scholar
Liberman, N., and Trope, Y. (1998), ‘The role of feasibility and desirability considerations in near and distant future decisions: A test of temporal construal theory’, Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 75(1): 518.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Liberman, N., Trope, Y., McCrea, S. M., and Sherman, S. J. (2007), ‘The effect of level of construal on the temporal distance of activity enactment’, Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 43(1): 143149.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Linzer, D. A. and Lewis, J. B. (2011), ‘poLCA: An R package for polytomous variable latent class analysis’, Journal of Statistical Software, 42(10): 129.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Loewenstein, G., and Chater, N. (2017), ‘Putting nudges in perspective’, Behavioural Public Policy, 1(1): 2653.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lusardi, A. and Mitchell, O. (2007), ‘Baby boomers retirement security: The role of planning, financial literacy and housing wealth?’, Journal of Monetary Economics, 54(1): 205224.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lusardi, A., and Mitchell, O. S. (2011), ‘Financial literacy around the world: An overview’, Journal of Pension Economics & Finance, 10(4): 497508.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Madrian, B. C., and Shea, D. F. (2001), ‘The power of suggestion: Inertia in 401(k) participation and savings behavior’, Quarterly Journal of Economics, 116(4): 11491187.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
McCrea, S. M., Liberman, N., Trope, Y., and Sherman, S. J. (2008), ‘Construal level and procrastination’, Psychological Science, 19(12): 13081314.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
McCutcheon, A. C. (1987), Latent Class Analysis, Newbury Park, CA: Sage.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Munnell, A. H. (2015), Falling short: The coming retirement crisis and what to do about it, Center for Retirement Research at Boston College Report 15–7.Google Scholar
Nibud (2015), De Nederlanders en hun pensioen: Hoe mensen financieel zijn voorbereid op hun pensioen. Retrieved from https://www.nibud.nl/wp-content/uploads/Onderzoeksrapport-Nibud-De-Nederlanders-en-hun-pensioen_okt2015.pdf.Google Scholar
OECD (2009), Pensions at a glance 2009: Retirement-income systems in OECD countries, Paris, France: OECD Publishing. Retrieved from: https://doi.org/10.1787/pension_glance-2009-en.Google Scholar
OECD (2015), Pensions at a glance 2015: OECD and G20 indicators, Paris, France: OECD Publishing. Retrieved from: http://dx.doi.org/10.1787/pension_glance-2015-en.Google Scholar
OECD (2017), Pensions at a glance 2017: OECD and G20 indicators, Paris, France: OECD Publishing. Retrieved from: http://dx.doi.org/10.1787/pension_glance-2017-en.Google Scholar
O'Donoghue, T., and Rabin, M. (1999), ‘Doing it now or later’, American Economic Review, 89(1): 103124.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
O'Donoghue, T., and Rabin, M. (2001), ‘Choice and procrastination’, Quarterly Journal of Economics, 116(1): 121160.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Rogers, T., Milkman, K. L., John, L. K., and Norton, M. I. (2015), ‘Beyond good intentions: Prompting people to make plans improves follow-through on important tasks’, Behavioral Science & Policy, 1(2): 3341.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Steel, P. (2007), ‘The nature of procrastination: a meta-analytic and theoretical review of quintessential self-regulatory failure’, Psychological Bulletin, 133(1): 6594.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Strotz, R. H. (1955), ‘Myopia and inconsistency in dynamic utility maximization’, Review of Economic Studies, 23(3): 165180.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Thaler, R. H. and Benartzi, S. (2004), ‘Save more tomorrow: Using behavioral economics to increase employee saving’, Journal of Political Economy, 112(S1): 164187.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Trope, Y., and Liberman, N. (2003), ‘Temporal construal’, Psychological Review, 110(3): 403421.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Vallacher, R. R., and Wegner, D. M. (1987), ‘What do people think they're doing? Action identification and human behavior’, Psychological Review, 94(1): 315.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Woolley, K., and Fishbach, A. (2016), ‘For the fun of it: Harnessing immediate rewards to increase persistence in long-term goals’, Journal of Consumer Research, 42(6): 952966.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Woolley, K., and Fishbach, A. (2017), ‘Immediate rewards predict adherence to long-term goals’, Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 43(2): 151162.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Supplementary material: PDF

Krijnen et al. supplementary material

Krijnen et al. supplementary material 1

Download Krijnen et al. supplementary material(PDF)
PDF 326 KB