Hostname: page-component-78c5997874-dh8gc Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-11-15T17:16:41.290Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Targeting beliefs and behaviours in misophonia: a case series from a UK specialist psychology service

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  19 October 2023

Jane Gregory*
Affiliation:
Department of Experimental Psychology, University of Oxford, Oxford, UK Oxford Health NHS Foundation Trust, Oxford, UK South London and Maudsley NHS Foundation Trust, UK
Tom Graham
Affiliation:
Oxford Health NHS Foundation Trust, Oxford, UK South London and Maudsley NHS Foundation Trust, UK
Brett Hayes
Affiliation:
South London and Maudsley NHS Foundation Trust, UK Salomons Institute for Applied Psychology, Canterbury Christ Church University
*
Corresponding author: Jane Gregory; Email: [email protected]
Rights & Permissions [Opens in a new window]

Abstract

Background:

Misophonia, a disorder of decreased sound tolerance, can cause significant distress and impairment. Cognitive behavioural therapy (CBT) may be helpful for improving symptoms of misophonia, but the key mechanisms of the disorder are not yet known.

Aims:

This case series aimed to evaluate individual, formulation-driven CBT for patients with misophonia in a UK psychology service.

Method:

A service evaluation of one-to-one therapy for patients with misophonia (n=19) was conducted in a specialist psychology service. Patients completed an average of 13 hours of therapy with a focus on the meaning applied to their reactions to sounds and associated behaviours. Primary outcome measures were the Misophonia Questionnaire (MQ) and the Amsterdam Misophonia Scale (A-MISO-S). Repeated measures t-tests were used to compare scores from pre-treatment to follow-up, and reliable and clinically significant change on the MQ was calculated.

Results:

Scores significantly improved on both misophonia measures, with an average of 38% change on the MQ and 40% change on the A-MISO-S. From pre-treatment to follow-up, 78% of patients showed reliable improvement on the MQ and 61% made clinically significant change.

Conclusions:

Limitations included a lack of control group, small sample size, and the use of an outcome measure that had not been thoroughly validated for a treatment-seeking sample. These results suggest that one-to-one, formulation-driven CBT for misophonia is worth exploring further using experimental design. Potential mechanisms to explore further include feared consequences of escalating reactions, the role of safety-seeking behaviours and the impact of early memories associated with reactions to sounds.

Type
Main
Creative Commons
Creative Common License - CCCreative Common License - BY
This is an Open Access article, distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution licence (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/), which permits unrestricted re-use, distribution and reproduction, provided the original article is properly cited.
Copyright
© The Author(s), 2023. Published by Cambridge University Press on behalf of British Association for Behavioural and Cognitive Psychotherapies

Introduction

Misophonia is a disorder characterised by a decreased tolerance to certain sounds, typically repetitive, everyday sounds that most people would find neutral or mildly aversive (Jastreboff and Jastreboff, Reference Jastreboff and Jastreboff2002; Swedo et al., Reference Swedo, Baguley, Denys, Dixon, Erfanian, Fioretti, Jastreboff, Kumar, Rosenthal, Rouw, Schiller, Simner, Storch, Taylor, Werff, Altimus and Raver2022). Individuals with misophonia report significant distress and impairment, including problems with work, study and relationships (Edelstein et al., Reference Edelstein, Brang, Rouw and Ramachandran2013; Rouw and Erfanian, Reference Rouw and Erfanian2018). Misophonia is also associated with increased symptoms of depression and anxiety (Rosenthal et al., Reference Rosenthal, McMahon, Greenleaf, Cassiello-Robbins, Guetta, Trumbull, Anand, Frazer-Abel and Kelley2022; Vitoratou et al., Reference Vitoratou, Uglik-Marucha, Hayes and Gregory2021).

There is currently no consensus on the best treatment approach to support individuals with misophonia (Swedo et al., Reference Swedo, Baguley, Denys, Dixon, Erfanian, Fioretti, Jastreboff, Kumar, Rosenthal, Rouw, Schiller, Simner, Storch, Taylor, Werff, Altimus and Raver2022). Developing and refining treatments requires understanding the key mechanisms contributing to the onset and maintenance of the disorder (Clark, Reference Clark2004); these mechanisms have not yet been established for misophonia. However, there have been a range of cognitive and behavioural features reported by individuals with misophonia, suggesting that one of the lines of pursuit for treatment could be the development of a theoretical cognitive model which could then be systematically tested.

Cognitive features reported by individuals with misophonia include feeling like they might panic or explode (Singer, Reference Singer2018; Vitoratou et al., Reference Vitoratou, Uglik-Marucha, Hayes and Gregory2021), lose control (Hocaoglu, Reference Hocaoglu2018; Jager et al., Reference Jager, de Koning, Bost, Denys and Vulink2020a; Vitoratou et al., Reference Vitoratou, Uglik-Marucha, Hayes and Gregory2021) and not be able to cope (Schneider and Arch, Reference Schneider and Arch2017) if they are unable to get away from certain sounds. They report negative appraisals, including judgement towards the person making the sound (Edelstein et al., Reference Edelstein, Brang, Rouw and Ramachandran2013; Reid et al., Reference Reid, Guzick, Gernand and Olsen2016; Vitoratou et al., Reference Vitoratou, Uglik-Marucha, Hayes and Gregory2021), a perception of oneself as being an unlikeable or angry person underneath for reacting to sounds (Vitoratou et al., Reference Vitoratou, Uglik-Marucha, Hayes and Gregory2021), and a feeling of failure and future hopelessness as a result of the condition (Edelstein et al., Reference Edelstein, Brang, Rouw and Ramachandran2013; Rouw and Erfanian, Reference Rouw and Erfanian2018; Vitoratou et al., Reference Vitoratou, Uglik-Marucha, Hayes and Gregory2021).

Behaviours reported by individuals with misophonia include blocking sounds (Alekri and Al Saif, Reference Alekri and Al Saif2019), avoidance (Alekri and Al Saif, Reference Alekri and Al Saif2019; Johnson et al., Reference Johnson, Webber, Wu, Lewin, Murphy and Storch2013; Rouw and Erfanian, Reference Rouw and Erfanian2018; Schneider and Arch, Reference Schneider and Arch2017), post-event rumination (Muller et al., Reference Muller, Khemlani-Patel and Neziroglu2018) and verbal aggression (Hocaoglu, Reference Hocaoglu2018; Reid et al., Reference Reid, Guzick, Gernand and Olsen2016). However, in the absence of experimental research or detailed case reports about the context, function and consequences of an individual’s behaviour, it is difficult to distinguish between adaptive coping strategies that improve symptoms and functioning, and safety-seeking behaviours that maintain beliefs about threat or violation (Thwaites and Freeston, Reference Thwaites and Freeston2005).

Some of these features of misophonia have been targeted with cognitive behavioural therapy (CBT) and there is emerging evidence that CBT may be helpful for improving the symptoms and impact of misophonia. Case studies have described a range of CBT strategies used for misophonia, including challenging beliefs and assumptions (Bernstein et al., Reference Bernstein, Angell and Dehle2013; Muller et al., Reference Muller, Khemlani-Patel and Neziroglu2018), cognitive restructuring (McGuire et al., Reference McGuire, Wu and Storch2015; Reid et al., Reference Reid, Guzick, Gernand and Olsen2016; Singer, Reference Singer2018), exposure-based exercises (Bernstein et al., Reference Bernstein, Angell and Dehle2013; McGuire et al., Reference McGuire, Wu and Storch2015; Muller et al., Reference Muller, Khemlani-Patel and Neziroglu2018; Reid et al., Reference Reid, Guzick, Gernand and Olsen2016; Singer, Reference Singer2018) and attention training (Bernstein et al., Reference Bernstein, Angell and Dehle2013). Strategies geared towards non-judgemental acceptance of thoughts and feelings about sounds were also reported (Kamody and del Conte, Reference Kamody and del Conte2017; Schneider and Arch, Reference Schneider and Arch2017).

An open trial of 90 patients found that misophonia symptoms improved following group CBT (Schröder et al., Reference Schröder, Vulink, van Loon and Denys2017). The interventions in this study included attention training, counterconditioning by pairing ‘trigger’ sounds with positive pictures or videos, stimulus manipulation intended to gain a sense of control over sounds, and relaxation techniques. Participants were provided with seven or eight sessions of 4 hours in a group setting with a therapist and a co-therapist. Significant improvement was seen in 48% of participants, with improvement defined as a 30% or more reduction on the Amsterdam Misophonia Scale (A–MISO-S), a measure designed by the research team, and a final score on the Clinical Global Impression-Improvement Scale (CGI-I) of very much or much improved.

The open trial was followed by a randomised controlled trial of group CBT for misophonia (Jager et al., Reference Jager, de Koning, Bost, Denys and Vulink2020a). The content for this trial was similar to the open trial, with the addition of ‘re-evaluating (eating) norms and stress reduction’, and the treatment was offered over seven sessions of 3 hours plus a 1.5-hour follow-up. Reduction in scores on the Amsterdam Misophonia Scale-Revised (AMISOS-R) was significantly greater in the CBT group compared with the waitlist control, and 37% of the intention-to-treat sample reported very much or much improved on the CGI-I, compared with 0% in the waitlist control group.

The group CBT trials reported above used a modular approach, in that a set of modules were delivered to all participants. This kind of approach can be useful for treating single disorders and in randomised trials to ensure consistency in the delivery of the intervention. However, it may be less helpful when treating patients with multiple disorders (Persons et al., Reference Persons, Roberts, Zalecki and Brechwald2006) and inefficient for patients with disorders for which there is no empirical evidence for key mechanisms likely to be maintaining the problem (Clark, Reference Clark2004). Considering the high co-morbidity of psychiatric disorders with misophonia (Rosenthal et al., Reference Rosenthal, McMahon, Greenleaf, Cassiello-Robbins, Guetta, Trumbull, Anand, Frazer-Abel and Kelley2022) and the limited empirical evidence for key mechanisms maintaining misophonic distress, it may be more appropriate to target transdiagnostic mechanisms (Rosenthal et al., Reference Rosenthal, Shan and Trumbull2023).

One way of targeting the mechanisms specific to an individual patient is to use a case formulation-driven approach (also known as case conceptualisation; Kuyken et al., Reference Kuyken, Padesky and Dudley2011), where the clinician and patient develop hypotheses about the mechanisms maintaining the individual’s problem and use evidence-based strategies to target those mechanisms. This approach allows the clinician and patient to evaluate the therapy as they go and revise the formulation and treatment accordingly (Persons, Reference Persons2005). Observations from formulation-driven clinical practice can then contribute to the development of a theoretical model for testing in experimental studies.

For newly characterised conditions such as misophonia, case series are an important step for demonstrating proof (or disproof) of concept and developing hypotheses to be tested in future research (Kempen, Reference Kempen2011). In a retrospective service evaluation, we aimed to assess whether symptoms of misophonia reduced following a course of formulation-driven CBT in a consecutive series of cases. We aimed to test whether there was reliable improvement and clinically significant change from pre-treatment to follow-up. A secondary aim was to evaluate whether self-reported symptoms of anxiety and depression decreased following a course of misophonia-focused CBT in these individuals. Given the association between misophonia and symptoms of anxiety and depression, and the transdiagnostic approach of formulation-driven CBT, it was hypothesised that there would be a reduction in symptoms of misophonia, depression and anxiety.

Method

This retrospective service evaluation included 19 patients from a National Health Service (NHS) specialist psychology service in the United Kingdom, for which misophonia was identified as the main problem contributing to their psychological distress. Referrals included those made directly to the service as well as referrals seen within the specialist service on behalf of local primary care psychology teams. Demographic information is summarised in Table 1.

Table 1. Summary of patient demographic information

Patients were included if they met the following criteria: (a) identified as having clinically significant misophonia at assessment and this was the main issue of focus for therapy; (b) at least 17 years old; (c) not actively suicidal; and (d) not receiving any concurrent psychological therapy. There were no exclusion criteria based on the presence of co-occurring conditions, but it was required that targeting misophonia was their focus for treatment.

Data for these 19 patients were collected over a 2.5-year period, starting with the service’s first patient seen specifically for help with misophonia. In this time frame, we received 22 referrals where misophonia was mentioned in the referral. For two of these referrals, misophonia was not identified as the main presenting problem and so these two patients were not offered treatment for misophonia after their initial assessment. One patient postponed treatment due to personal circumstances that meant they were unable to attend appointments at the clinic, for reasons unrelated to the treatment. All other patients who received any amount of therapy for misophonia (n=19) in this time frame were included in the evaluation. There were no other referrals for misophonia during the 2.5-year period.

Measures

The primary outcome measures were the Misophonia Questionnaire (MQ; Wu et al., Reference Wu, Lewin, Murphy and Storch2014) and the Amsterdam Misophonia Scale (A-MISO-S; Schröder et al., Reference Schröder, Vulink and Denys2013), which were the two main published scales for misophonia at the time this group of patients were seen. The MQ (Wu et al., Reference Wu, Lewin, Murphy and Storch2014) consists of three parts, the 7-item Misophonia Symptom Scale (Cronbach’s α=.86 in the author’s initial validation of the scale; Wu et al., Reference Wu, Lewin, Murphy and Storch2014) and the 10-item Misophonia Emotions and Behaviours Scale (α=.86), both rated on a 5-point scale from 0 (not at all true/never) to 4 (always true/always) and the single item Misophonia Severity Scale, rated from 1 (minimal) to 15 (very severe). The first two sections are combined to create the MQ total (α=.89), with a score ranging from 0 to 68. The A-MISO-S (Schröder et al., Reference Schröder, Vulink and Denys2013) consists of six items, also rated from 0–4, with a total possible score of 24 (psychometric properties not available). Full psychometric analyses have not been published for either scale, and the limitations of these measures have been described elsewhere (see, for example, Rosenthal et al., Reference Rosenthal, Anand, Cassiello-Robbins, Williams, Guetta, Trumbull and Kelley2021).

Secondary outcome measures were the Patient Health Questionnaire-9 (PHQ-9; Kroenke et al., Reference Kroenke, Spitzer and Williams2001), measuring depression symptoms using a 4-point scale (total score range of 0 to 27; α=.89; clinical cut-off >9), and the General Anxiety Disorder-7 (GAD-7; Spitzer et al., Reference Spitzer, Kroenke, Williams and Löwe2006) measuring anxiety symptoms, also on a 4-point scale (total score ranging from 0 to 21; α=.92; clinical cut-off >7).

Procedure

Assessment

The diagnosis of misophonia and suitability for CBT was confirmed by either the first or second author for all patients, based on their initial assessment appointment. At the time we started seeing patients with misophonia, the only published diagnostic criteria available were those proposed by Schröder et al. (Reference Schröder, Vulink and Denys2013), which included: the presence of an aversive reaction to specific sounds produced by a human being, which starts with irritation or disgust and progresses to anger, with a sense of loss of control; recognition by the person that the reaction is out of proportion to the stimulus; avoidance or enduring sounds with distress; causing significant distress or impairment, and not better explained by another disorder. We adapted these criteria based on our clinical observations and emerging research, including: that trigger sounds were not necessarily caused by other humans; that reactions did not necessarily start with irritation or disgust; that there was some experience of emotional, behavioural or physical dysregulation, but that anger did not need to be present; and that individuals may engage in safety-seeking behaviours other than avoidance to be able to encounter sounds. We also required that the symptoms had been problematic for at least one month, to allow for the possibility that temporary changes in responsivity to sounds could occur following changes in environment (e.g. construction sounds outside the home) or after an illness, injury or traumatic event. If symptoms started after such a change, but persisted for more than one month, then they would meet criteria for diagnosis. As is standard procedure for the service, all patients completed the primary and secondary outcome measures at the assessment, before each treatment appointment, and at follow-up.

Intervention

All patients received one-to-one, formulation-driven cognitive behavioural therapy, aiming to reduce the distress and functional impact caused by misophonia. Potential mechanisms were identified from the individual’s formulation, and interventions were selected to target those theorised mechanisms. For most patients, the bulk of therapy was delivered as weekly 1-hour sessions, but treatment delivery was also adapted to meet the needs of the patient, the therapy and the service. For example, some sessions involving processing of early memories were longer, some patients completed an intensive or semi-intensive format with fewer sessions of 2–4 hours each, and some had longer breaks between sessions to put things from therapy into practice or to meet other demands on their time. Variations in the total hours of treatment were the result of individual needs of the patient and the decision to end therapy was decided by the therapist and patient together. Information about the treatment format is summarised in Table 2.

Table 2. Summary of information about treatment format

All patients were seen by either the first or second author as their primary therapist, who both have professional doctorates in psychology and are accredited CBT therapists in the UK. Some sessions were observed by assistant psychologists or students on placement, who often participated in behavioural experiments. One patient had a second clinical psychologist join as co-therapist, which was done as part of that therapist’s training in treating patients with misophonia and has been written up separately as a case study (Gregory and Foster, Reference Gregory and Foster2023).

In the first 2–3 hours of treatment, patients completed an individualised formulation of their difficulties. This was usually done by bringing to the surface a recent memory of being triggered by a sound, ideally one that felt typical of them and their misophonia. They were encouraged to tune into their bodies and the ‘felt sense’ of what was happening (Butler et al., Reference Butler, Fennell and Hackmann2010; Gendlin, Reference Gendlin1996), that is, what felt true to them in the moment, even if it did not appear as a verbal thought at the time. Our initial framework for mapping out the problem was Salkovskis’s ‘vicious flower’, drawing from its adaptation for transdiagnostic use for medically unexplained symptoms (Salkovskis et al., Reference Salkovskis, Gregory, Sedgwick-Taylor, White, Opher and Ólafsdóttir2016). This model proposes that the intensity of reactions in the problem situation is influenced by misinterpretation of bodily sensations (or sensory information, in the case of misophonia), feared consequences and perceived capacity to cope. It poses that to treat the problem, we need to understand the maintenance processes causing those problem situations to persist over time. We identified some recurring themes during the case series that enabled us to adapt that model for misophonia (see Fig. 1 for an example formulation, not based on any particular patient from the series).

Figure 1. Example of an individualised case formulation of misophonia, based on themes emerging from the case series but not pertaining to a specific patient.

Each patient developed competing explanations for their problem, using the ‘Theory A/Theory B’ technique frequently used as part of formulation-driven CBT for obsessive compulsive disorder (OCD; Bream et al., Reference Bream, Challacombe, Palmer and Salkovskis2017). This strategy provides alternative explanations for what keeps an individual’s problems going and identifies the related action that would be required for each possible explanation. These theories are then tested with a series of experiments and evidence is gathered to help the patient decide which theory is the best fit for their problem. From our patients’ individual formulations, it emerged that many people already had two competing theories, sometimes simultaneously feeling like their difficulties were caused by: (A) a problem with other people; or (B) a problem with me. We introduced a third explanation, that their difficulties were the result of (C) a problem with sound sensitivity that had spiralled. Theory C included an understanding that there is wide natural variation in sensory responsivity, and that the distress and impairment associated with misophonia was influenced by a range of biological, experiential, systemic, emotional, behavioural and environmental factors.

Based on their individualised formulation, transdiagnostic CBT techniques were used to target the individual’s theorised maintenance cycles, including behavioural experiments, imagery rescripting, exposure-based experiments using a belief disconfirmation (Salkovskis et al., Reference Salkovskis, Hackmann, Wells, Gelder and Clark2007) or an expectancy violation (Craske et al., Reference Craske, Treanor, Conway, Zbozinek and Vervliet2014) approach, attention training, behavioural activation, video feedback, interoceptive exposure, problem solving, and testing the value of coping strategies.

Behavioural experiments were used to identify, test and change the proposed maintenance cycles (Bennett-Levy et al., Reference Bennett-Levy, Westbrook, Fennell, Cooper, Rouf and Hackmann2016). Experiments targeted both misophonia-specific beliefs (e.g. ‘If someone makes a sound they know I don’t like, they must not care about me’) and more general beliefs affecting their misophonia (e.g. ‘If I show my emotions, I will be rejected’). These could be beliefs that felt true in the moment, but did not necessarily feel true at other times (e.g. ‘They’re making that sound deliberately to hurt me’) and beliefs that were more stable across situations (e.g. ‘Having judgemental thoughts about loved ones makes you a bad person’). Behavioural experiments were also used to help distinguish adaptive coping strategies from safety-seeking behaviours, by identifying and testing the function of behaviours, identifying unintended consequences, and testing out new behaviours for comparison. Table 3 shows some examples of the types of experiments used.

Table 3. Examples of behavioural experiments for misophonia

Imagery rescripting (ImRs) was used for processing early memories associated with their misophonia. This technique is increasingly being integrated into CBT and included in intervention studies for a range of disorders (Arntz, Reference Arntz2012). We used the protocol described by Arntz and Jacob (Reference Arntz and Jacob2012), using an affect bridge to connect the present experience to a memory (usually in childhood) associated with the current emotion, then rescripting the memory based on the previously unmet needs of the younger self. The function of ImRs is not to ‘rewrite’ the memory, but to update the meaning associated with the memory. For misophonia, this included things like validating and comforting the child’s emotions, explaining the limitations of the other person present in the memory, or telling the child, ‘You are not crazy, you have something called misophonia’.

In the second half of therapy, patients completed experiments related to novel interaction with sounds and their reactions to sounds. These experiments involved some exposure to sounds, but not with the purpose of habituating to sounds. The patients decided when they were ready to try these experiments and each one was planned together in line with their formulation. The goals of experiments engaging with sounds were to create new associations with sounds (Craske et al., Reference Craske, Treanor, Conway, Zbozinek and Vervliet2014; Frank and McKay, Reference Frank and McKay2019), thus reducing the intensity of the ‘felt sense’ feeling of threat and violation, to test feared consequences and beliefs about capacity to cope, and to test out and compare coping strategies. The experiments typically involved short exposure to sounds at a time (20–30 seconds), followed by rating the sense of threat of violation, then repeating the experiment several times. The experiment was terminated if it caused more distress than ‘real life’ exposure to sounds, or if there was no change after two to three repetitions.

Sound-based experiments included moving closer to sounds to test predicted outcomes, engaging with sounds by labelling the acoustic features of the sounds, ‘inviting’ more sounds to shift beliefs about violation and control, and playing with sounds electronically or by using a pretend remote control on the person making sounds. We used humour and imagery to shift the perception of threat and violation, adapting strategies described by Frank and McKay (Reference Frank and McKay2019). For example, one person imagined wind-up chattering teeth toys during meals, another pictured rain on a roof while listening to keyboard sounds, another put on a brief comedy sketch in session, portraying what they imagined the person making a recurring sound must be doing to be able to create such a noise. Some patients conducted orchestras of mouth sounds and held competitions for who could make the loudest sounds. Table 3 includes some other examples of experiments involving sounds to test a particular theory or compare coping strategies.

A range of other strategies were used depending on the individual formulation. For example, one person identified that job dissatisfaction had coincided with worsening misophonia symptoms, for which we used problem-solving strategies. We used behavioural activation, attention training and grounding techniques for individuals who were ruminating on the problem or took a long time to recover after a trigger event. For those struggling with anger about circumstances outside of their control, we used techniques weighing up the pros and cons of holding onto anger and writing anger letters. Skills in communication, assertiveness, tolerance and setting boundaries were provided for patients whose misophonia was connected to challenges in relationships.

Finally, patients wrote up a therapy blueprint. This included a description of changes made in therapy, ideas for maintaining progress, and a plan for dealing with distress and set-backs. This plan also included an understanding of their sensory sensitivity and the healthy coping strategies and accommodations that they would continue to use, when required for focus, relaxation and enjoyment of activities.

Statistical analyses

Repeated measures t-tests were used to compare mean scores at pre-treatment and follow-up on primary and secondary measures. ‘Pre’ scores refer to the first time they completed the outcome measures for our service, which may have been before or after their assessment, and always prior to commencing active treatment.

To reduce the number of comparisons tested, we did not calculate differences between start and end of treatment, only between pre-treatment and follow-up. We made a Bonferroni adjustment to account for multiple comparisons, with statistical significance determined at p<.008. Where there was missing data on any measure for either the pre-treatment or the follow-up, patient data was not included in any of the calculations or comparisons for that particular measure, but was retained in calculations for the other measures. For example, if a patient had missing follow-up data for the GAD-7 but complete data for other measures, their scores were not included in calculating means for GAD-7 at pre-treatment or follow-up, but were included in all other analyses.

The Leeds Reliable Change Indicator (Morley and Dowzer, Reference Morley and Dowzer2014) was used to calculate reliable change and clinically significant change, and to graph reliable change. The MQ was the best validated misophonia scale available, for which clinical and comparison norms had been published (Wu et al., Reference Wu, Lewin, Murphy and Storch2014), at the time of collecting data. Therefore, we selected this measure to calculate the number of patients who showed reliable change and clinically significant change (Jacobson and Truax, Reference Jacobson and Truax1991) from baseline to follow-up. We selected criterion b for clinically significant change, which is one of the two options recommended when both clinical and comparison norms are available (Jacobson and Truax, Reference Jacobson and Truax1991). This criterion states that change is clinically significant when the individual has showed reliable change and their final scores fall within the range of the ‘functional’ population (i.e. within two standard deviations of the mean of the comparison group). We chose this over criterion c, which is when a patient moves closer to the mean of the comparison group than to the mean of the clinical group by the end of therapy. The rationale for this choice was that the mean score for our patients (i.e. treatment-seeking) at baseline was over 30% higher than the mean for the ‘clinical’ group for the study in which clinical and comparison norms were reported (from participants in a university sample scoring above cut-off on a single item rating severity, which has not been psychometrically evaluated). This difference in means suggested that our patient group experienced more severe symptoms compared with the group for which ‘clinical’ norms were available. We decided that our patients’ scores at the end of treatment in relation to the comparison group norms would therefore be a better criterion for clinically significant change.

Results

There was missing data for one patient on the MQ at pre-treatment, and for three patients on the GAD-7 and PHQ-9 at follow-up. Therefore, their data were not included in any analyses using those specific measures but were included in analyses for other measures.

Comparisons on misophonia measures

For both misophonia measures, t-tests revealed a significant difference in score from pre-treatment to final follow-up (Table 4) with large effect sizes (Cohen’s d>.08). There was a 38% change on the MQ total score. For the two subscales of the MQ, there was a 21% change on the Symptoms Scale and 51% change on the Emotions and Behaviour Scale. There was a 40% change on the A-MISO-S.

Table 4. Comparisons between pre-treatment and follow-up measures of misophonia symptoms

MQ, Misophonia Questionnaire; MSYS, Misophonia Symptom Scale (MQ subscale 1); MEBS, Misophonia Emotions and Beliefs Scale (MQ subscale 2); A-MISO-S, Amsterdam Misophonia Scale. A Bonferroni adjustment was applied for multiple comparisons and all results were significant at p<.008.

Reliable and clinically significant change on the Misophonia Questionnaire

From pre-treatment to follow-up, 14 out of 18 patients (78%) had reliably improved on the MQ, and four had not made reliable change. There was no reliable deterioration found. Eleven patients (61%) made clinically significant change. The average score on the MQ before treatment was 42.39 (SD=9.39) and the average score at follow-up was 26.44 (SD=11.18). Patient scores on the MQ can be seen in Fig. 2, with pre-treatment scores on the x-axis and follow-up scores on the y-axis. Patient markers within the tramlines indicate no reliable change, and those below and to the right of the tramlines indicate reliable change.

Figure 2. Graph of reliable change index for the Misophonia Questionnaire from pre-treatment to follow-up.

Comparisons on depression and anxiety measures

Mean scores for symptoms of depression and anxiety were significantly reduced from pre-treatment to follow-up (Table 5), with large effect sizes (Cohen’s d>.08).

Table 5. Comparisons between pre-treatment and follow-up measures of anxiety and depression

PHQ-9, Patient Health Questionnaire-9, measuring symptoms of depression in the past two weeks; GAD-7, Generalised Anxiety Questionnaire-7, measuring symptoms of anxiety in the past two weeks. A Bonferroni adjustment was applied for multiple comparisons and both results were significant at p<.008.

Mean scores for depression symptoms on the PHQ-9 moved from the mild symptom range (score range 5–9; Kroenke et al., Reference Kroenke, Spitzer and Williams2001) to non-clinical (<5). For anxiety symptoms, mean GAD-7 scores reduced from the clinical range to the non-clinical range (with a clinical cut-off score of >7; Spitzer et al., Reference Spitzer, Kroenke, Williams and Löwe2006).

Discussion

This case series aimed at evaluating whether one-to-one, formulation-driven CBT significantly reduced symptoms of misophonia in patients at a specialist psychology service. Close to 80% of patients showed reliable improvement and more than half showed clinically significant change in misophonia symptoms. Symptoms of depression and anxiety also significantly reduced.

This study adds to the emerging literature on psychological treatments for improving symptoms of misophonia, which has consisted mostly of descriptive case studies of individual CBT (Bernstein et al., Reference Bernstein, Angell and Dehle2013; McGuire et al., Reference McGuire, Wu and Storch2015; Muller et al., Reference Muller, Khemlani-Patel and Neziroglu2018) and evaluations of group CBT in an open trial (Schröder et al., Reference Schröder, Vulink, van Loon and Denys2017) and a randomised controlled trial of group treatment (Jager et al., Reference Jager, Vulink, Bergfeld, Loon and Denys2020b). While misophonia is not classified as a psychiatric disorder (Swedo et al., Reference Swedo, Baguley, Denys, Dixon, Erfanian, Fioretti, Jastreboff, Kumar, Rosenthal, Rouw, Schiller, Simner, Storch, Taylor, Werff, Altimus and Raver2022), there appear to be psychological aspects to distress in misophonia, and CBT shows promise as an intervention.

Comparisons between our results and previous trials must be made cautiously due to the differences in aims, methodology, measurement tools and criteria used for clinical improvement. We noted that from pre-treatment to follow-up, our group had a mean score change on the A–MISO-S of –6.1 points, with an average symptom improvement of 40%. In the open trial of group CBT, Schröder et al. (Reference Schröder, Vulink, van Loon and Denys2017), results showed a mean score change of –4.5 points pre- to post-treatment, with an average symptom change of 33%. It was interesting to note that in both our evaluation and the open trial, the average final score on the A-MISO-S was 9.1. Schröder et al. (Reference Schröder, Vulink and Denys2013) found that higher initial scores on the A-MISO-S was predictive of greater symptom change, so it is possible that our slightly increased symptom change was influenced by the higher average score at baseline (15.1 in our evaluation compared with 13.6 in the open trial). One important difference between our case series and the open trial was the total number of hours of treatment. Our patients had an average of 13 hours of treatment, compared with 28–32 hours of group treatment in the open trial. Considering we do not yet understand the key mechanisms of misophonia, it makes sense that individual therapy would be more efficient in terms of patient time, as the formulation-driven approach allows for targeting the maintenance cycles identified for that individual, without needing to spend time on interventions that may apply to other group members.

The relative change in scores between the two subscales of the MQ was an interesting finding. Most of the change in MQ scores can be accounted for by change on the Misophonia Emotions and Beliefs Scale, which measures how respondents react to sounds. There was very little change on the Misophonia Symptom Scale, which asks respondents to rate how sensitive they are to certain sounds, compared with other people (Wu et al., Reference Wu, Lewin, Murphy and Storch2014). During informal feedback, some of our patients told us that even when they felt that their misophonia had improved substantially, and that their reactions were less intense, they still considered themselves to be sensitive to sounds compared with other people. This highlighted the limitations of this particular tool to measure change in change in misophonia.

Subsequent to this study, the S-Five, a valid and reliable tool for measuring misophonia, has been published (Vitoratou et al., Reference Vitoratou, Uglik-Marucha, Hayes and Gregory2021). The five dimensions of the S-Five capture aspects of misophonic severity that one might expect to shift with therapy, such as appraisals, aggressive outbursts, dysregulation in response to sounds and perceived impact (Vitoratou et al., Reference Vitoratou, Uglik-Marucha, Hayes and Gregory2021). We now use the S-Five to monitor outcomes, selected over the Duke Misophonia Questionnaire (DMQ; Rosenthal et al., Reference Rosenthal, Anand, Cassiello-Robbins, Williams, Guetta, Trumbull and Kelley2021) because of the five dimensions of the S-Five, which include cognitive appraisals that might be targets for CBT (e.g. beliefs that there is something wrong with oneself because of one’s reactions to sounds, and beliefs that the source of one’s distress is the bad behaviour of others). The DMQ includes a ‘cognitive response subscale’, but the items contained within the subscale seem to reflect a verbalisation of distress or dysregulation (e.g. ‘I am helpless’ and ‘I would do anything to make it stop’) rather than an appraisal of the problem, and so may be less helpful for detecting cognition change during therapy. One patient from the present case series, whose treatment commenced after the S-Five became available, completed the S-Five at each session, and there were different points of change on the different factors for that particular patient, with scores on ‘internalising appraisals’ (attributing blame to oneself) changing after the initial assessment, and ‘emotional threat’ (a sense of, or prediction of, emotional dysregulation) scores changing after active treatment commenced (Gregory and Foster, Reference Gregory and Foster2023).

Misophonia-specific interventions were those that included interacting with sounds and reactions in novel ways without using their usual coping strategies, to create new associations with sounds and test feared consequences. For some patients this involved addressing a specific, verbalised belief (e.g. ‘If I have to keep listening to this disgusting noise, I will throw up’); for others it was simplified by rating their felt sense of threat and/or violation in the moment. The experiments were typically brief and repeated several times, with changes observed in the ratings of threat/violation between repetitions. Several patients gave positive feedback on the value of these experiments, saying their reactions felt fundamentally different afterwards (e.g. from intense violation to mild irritation), which could indicate that inhibitory learning had taken place (Craske et al., Reference Craske, Treanor, Conway, Zbozinek and Vervliet2014). Further research is needed to determine the mechanism of change happening in these experiments.

Imagery rescripting (ImRs) was used to process and ‘update’ early memories associated with the development of strong reactions to sounds. While trauma-informed, this technique can also target non-trauma memories that are meaningful in terms of the development of beliefs underlying the current problem (Holmes et al., Reference Holmes, Arntz and Smucker2007). While ImRs has not previously been reported in the context of misophonia, there is evidence that eye movement desensitisation and reprocessing (EMDR) therapy, which involves a similar strategy of updating memories, may help reduce symptoms of misophonia (Jager et al., Reference Jager, Vulink, de Roos and Denys2021). While the mechanisms behind ImRs are not fully understood, Kunze et al. (Reference Kunze, Arntz and Kindt2019) proposed that there may be multiple mechanisms involved, and that there are likely different mechanisms involved for ImRs techniques compared with exposure techniques.

From our clinical observations and informal feedback from patients, we noticed that there was variation in the change that resulted from ImRs. For some, it shifted the feeling that there was something seriously wrong with them for reacting that way (Theory B). For others, it helped to update beliefs from believing others don’t care (Theory A), to believing they don’t understand. For some, it was useful for distinguishing early experiences from current experiences (e.g. being able to leave the situation as an adult but not as a child; no longer being in a chaotic or abusive environment; improved skills in emotion regulation and impulse control as an adult), which strengthened their belief that distress in misophonia was compounded by their circumstances and therefore could potentially change (Theory C). Techniques to identify sensory triggers and distinguish between current (benign) circumstances and past traumatic circumstances (stimulus discrimination; Ehlers et al., Reference Ehlers, Hackmann and Michael2004) were also used to support this differentiation at a felt-sense level. It would be useful to explore the relevance of early memories in misophonia using in-depth qualitative studies and to experimentally test ImRs as an intervention.

One of our aims was to develop hypotheses about potential cognitive and behavioural mechanisms that may be key to misophonia. We noticed some themes in terms of cognitions: beliefs that the individual is flawed or damaged in some way because of the way they react to sounds; beliefs that others who make problematic sounds are uncaring, thoughtless or willingly causing harm; and fears about what might happen in the moment (e.g. losing control or being judged) or longer term (emotional, physical or social harm caused by misophonia). We theorised that behaviours were intended to minimise emotional or behavioural dysregulation, prevent specific feared consequences (e.g. hiding emotions to avoid being judged), or to communicate distress. While we were able to work with individuals to test whether coping strategies achieved their intended function and to examine unintended consequences, no clear themes emerged across the group about whether particular kinds of behaviours might play a key role in maintaining distress.

Further research is needed to understand the role of behaviour in the development and maintenance of misophonia. Avoidance behaviour is a key maintenance factor in many anxiety disorders and treatment frequently aims to reduce avoidance (Butler et al., Reference Butler, Fennell and Hackmann2010). To date, there have been no experimental or prospective studies examining whether avoidance is associated with worsening misophonia symptoms. One study (Dibb and Golding, Reference Dibb and Golding2022) found that higher avoidance of triggers was cross-sectionally associated with poorer quality of life in one domain (physical functioning) but, at 6-month follow-up, higher avoidance of triggers at baseline predicted higher quality of life in another domain (role limitations as a result of emotional problems). This suggests that it is possible that some avoidance behaviour may actually help reduce the impairment associated with misophonia. Future studies need to assess a range of misophonic behaviours and explore the impact on short- and long-term misophonia severity and impairment.

Strengths and limitations

One of the strengths of this case series is that it shows outcomes from highly individualised treatment, reflecting the ‘real world’ therapy available to these patients. It used a consecutive referral design, showing the results of all eligible patients who underwent treatment in the relevant time period, including those who did not improve. There were no drop-outs in the case series, other than one patient who postponed treatment for practical reasons, indicating a high level of acceptability for this treatment.

This was an uncontrolled case series evaluating the treatment provided in a healthcare service, rather than an experimentally designed piece of research, and therefore there are several limitations to be acknowledged. Firstly, our small sample size was predominantly female and white British, and there was no control group. Treatment was highly individualised and therefore there was no set ‘protocol’ being evaluated here. The treatment format and number of sessions were not consistent across patients. The follow-up period was not consistent, and some patients were seen for follow-up sessions, while the follow-up for others consisted only of submitting outcome measures. The results should therefore be considered exploratory and hypothesis-generating for future research.

At the time these patients were seen, the measures that were publicly available had not been subjected to rigorous psychometric evaluation and norms for a treatment-seeking population were not available for the best-validated measure. This limited our analysis to using non-clinical comparison groups for the assessment of clinically significant change. Future treatment studies should use the measurement tools for misophonia now available that are multi-dimensional and have been subjected to rigorous psychometric evaluation (e.g. Rosenthal et al., Reference Rosenthal, Anand, Cassiello-Robbins, Williams, Guetta, Trumbull and Kelley2021; Vitoratou et al., Reference Vitoratou, Uglik-Marucha, Hayes and Gregory2021; Rinaldi et al., Reference Rinaldi, Smees, Ward and Simner2022), with both clinical (Vitoratou et al., Reference Vitoratou, Uglik-Marucha, Hayes and Gregory2021) and comparison (Vitoratou et al., Reference Vitoratou, Hayes, Uglik-Marucha, Pearson, Graham and Gregory2023) norms available. Finally, while there was a significant decrease in scores for symptoms of depression and anxiety, it should be noted that at the beginning of treatment the average scores for both were in the mild range. Therefore, any hypotheses about the impact of misophonia-focused treatment on symptoms of depression and anxiety should be held lightly and tested further.

Conclusions

Case series are an important early step in the development of research hypotheses and ‘proof of concept’ for treatment of newly characterised conditions (Kempen, Reference Kempen2011). The present study demonstrates that further investigation of individual CBT as a treatment for misophonia is warranted. It also provides clinical observations that can contribute to the development of a theoretical model, which could then be tested in a series of experimental studies (Clark, Reference Clark2004).

Data availability statement

The data that support the findings of this study are available on request from the corresponding author, J.G. The data are not publicly available due to data containing information that could compromise the privacy of patients.

Acknowledgements

We would like to extend a huge thank you to all our patients, truly experts by experience, who trusted us and were willing to try hard things. Thank you also to Paul Salkovskis for his advice on data analysis and feedback on the manuscript, to Victoria Bream for feedback on design of the analysis, to Sheena Liness for clinical supervision, and to Blake Stobie and David Veale for their help and support in setting up the project.

Author contributions

Jane Gregory: Conceptualization (lead), Data curation (equal), Formal analysis (lead), Funding acquisition (lead), Investigation (equal), Methodology (lead), Project administration (lead), Writing – original draft (lead); Tom Graham: Conceptualization (supporting), Data curation (supporting), Investigation (equal), Methodology (supporting), Writing – review & editing (supporting); Brett Hayes: Data curation (equal), Methodology (supporting), Writing – review & editing (supporting).

Financial support

This research was funded by the Wellcome Trust [J.G.; grant number 102176/B/13/Z]. For the purpose of open access, the author has applied a CC-BY public copyright licence to any Author Accepted Manuscript version arising from this submission. The views expressed are those of the author(s) and not necessarily those of the NHS or the Wellcome Trust. The funders were not involved in the design or implementation of the study.

Competing interests

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Ethical standards

All authors have abided by the Ethical Principles of Psychologists and Code of Conduct as set out by the BABCP and BPS. The therapy was provided as part of usual care at the Centre for Anxiety Disorders and Trauma in South London and Maudsley NHS Foundation Trust. These patients were seen as part of routine treatment provision and this case series was approved as a service evaluation by the Southwark Information Governance team at South London and Maudsley NHS Foundation Trust.

References

Alekri, J., & Al Saif, F. (2019). Suicidal misophonia: a case report. Psychiatry and Clinical Psychopharmacology, 29, 232237. https://doi.org/10.1080/24750573.2019.1597585 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Arntz, A. (2012). Imagery rescripting as a therapeutic technique: review of clinical trials, basic studies, and research agenda. Journal of Experimental Psychopathology, 3, 189208. https://doi.org/10.5127/jep.024211 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Arntz, A., & Jacob, G. (2012). Schema Therapy in Practice: An Introductory Guide to the Schema Mode Approach. Wiley.Google Scholar
Bennett-Levy, J., Westbrook, D., Fennell, M., Cooper, M., Rouf, K., & Hackmann, A. (2016). Behavioural experiments: historical and conceptual underpinnings. Oxford Guide to Behavioural Experiments in Cognitive Therapy, 120. https://doi.org/10.1093/MED:PSYCH/9780198529163.003.0001 Google Scholar
Bernstein, R. E., Angell, K. L., & Dehle, C. M. (2013). A brief course of cognitive behavioural therapy for the treatment of misophonia: a case example. Cognitive Behaviour Therapist, 6, E10. https://doi.org/10.1017/S1754470X13000172 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bream, V., Challacombe, F., Palmer, A., & Salkovskis, P. (2017). Cognitive Behaviour Therapy for Obsessive-Compulsive Disorder. Oxford University Press. https://psycnet.apa.org/record/2017-33272-000 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Butler, G., Fennell, M., & Hackmann, A. (2010). Cognitive-Behavioral Therapy for Anxiety Disorders: Mastering Clinical Challenges (Guides to Individualized Evidence-based Treatment). Guilford Press.Google Scholar
Clark, D. M. (2004). Developing new treatments: on the interplay between theories, experimental science and clinical innovation. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 42, 10891104. https://doi.org/10.1016/J.BRAT.2004.05.002 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Craske, M. G., Treanor, M., Conway, C., Zbozinek, T., & Vervliet, B. (2014). Maximizing exposure therapy: an inhibitory learning approach. Behavior and Research Therapy, 58, 1023. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.brat.2014.04.006 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Dibb, B., & Golding, S. E. (2022). A longitudinal investigation of quality of life and negative emotions in misophonia. Frontiers in Neuroscience, 16, 900474. https://doi.org/10.3389/fnins.2022.900474 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Edelstein, M., Brang, D., Rouw, R., & Ramachandran, V. S. (2013). Misophonia: physiological investigations and case descriptions. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience. https://doi.org/10.3389/fnhum.2013.00296 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ehlers, A., Hackmann, A., & Michael, T. (2004). Intrusive re-experiencing in post-traumatic stress disorder: phenomenology, theory, and therapy. Memory, 12, 403415. https://doi.org/10.1080/09658210444000025 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Frank, B., & McKay, D. (2019). The suitability of an inhibitory learning approach in exposure when habituation fails: a clinical application to misophonia. Cognitive and Behavioral Practice, 26, 130142. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cbpra.2018.04.003 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gendlin, E. T. (1996). Focusing-Oriented Psychotherapy: A Manual of the Experiential Method. Guilford Press.Google Scholar
Gregory, J., & Foster, C. (2023). Session-by-session change in misophonia: a descriptive case study using intensive CBT. The Cognitive Behaviour Therapist, 16, E18. https://doi.org/10.1017/S1754470X23000107 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hocaoglu, C. (2018). A little known topic misophonia: two case reports. Dusunen Adam The Journal of Psychiatry and Neurological Sciences, 31, 8996. https://doi.org/10.5350/DAJPN2018310109 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Holmes, E. A., Arntz, A., & Smucker, M. R. (2007). Imagery rescripting in cognitive behaviour therapy: images, treatment techniques and outcomes. Journal of Behavior Therapy and Experimental Psychiatry, 38, 297305. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jbtep.2007.10.007 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Jacobson, N. S., & Truax, P. (1991). Clinical significance: a statistical approach to defining meaningful change in psychotherapy research. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 59, 1219. https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-006X.59.1.12 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Jager, I., de Koning, P., Bost, T., Denys, D., & Vulink, N. (2020a). Misophonia: phenomenology, comorbidity and demographics in a large sample. PLoS One, 15. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0231390 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Jager, I., Vulink, N., Bergfeld, I., Loon, A., & Denys, D. (2020b). Cognitive behavioral therapy for misophonia: a randomized clinical trial. Depression and Anxiety, 37. https://doi.org/10.1002/da.23127 Google Scholar
Jager, I., Vulink, N., de Roos, C., & Denys, D. (2021). EMDR therapy for misophonia: a pilot study of case series. European Journal of Psychotraumatology, 12. https://doi.org/10.1080/20008198.2021.1968613 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Jastreboff, M. M., & Jastreboff, P. J. (2002). Decreased sound tolerance and tinnitus retraining therapy (TRT). Australian and New Zealand Journal of Audiology, 24, 7484. https://doi.org/10.1375/audi.24.2.74.31105 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Johnson, P. L., Webber, T. A., Wu, M. S., Lewin, A. B., Murphy, T. K., & Storch, E. A. (2013). When selective audiovisual stimuli become unbearable: a case series on pediatric misophonia. Neuropsychiatry, 3, 569575. https://doi.org/10.2217/npy.13.70 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kamody, R. C., & del Conte, G. S. (2017). using dialectical behavior therapy to treat misophonia in adolescence [Letter to the Editor]. The Primary Care Companion for CNS Disorders, 19. https://doi.org/10.4088/PCC.17l02105 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kempen, J. H. (2011). Appropriate use and reporting of uncontrolled case series in the medical literature. American Journal of Ophthalmology, 151, 710. https://doi.org/10.1016/J.AJO.2010.08.047 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kroenke, K., Spitzer, R. L., & Williams, J. B. W. (2001). The PHQ-9. Journal of General Internal Medicine, 16, 606613. https://doi.org/10.1046/J.1525-1497.2001.016009606.X CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kunze, A. E., Arntz, A., & Kindt, M. (2019). Investigating the effects of imagery rescripting on emotional memory: a series of analogue studies. Journal of Experimental Psychopathology, 10. https://doi.org/10.1177/2043808719850733 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kuyken, W., Padesky, C. A., & Dudley, R. (2011). Collaborative Case Conceptualization: Working Effectively with Clients in Cognitive-Behavioral Therapy. New York: Guilford Press.Google Scholar
McGuire, J. F., Wu, M. S., & Storch, E. A. (2015). Cognitive-behavioral therapy for 2 youths with misophonia. Journal of Clinical Psychiatry, 76, 573574. https://doi.org/10.4088/JCP.14cr09343 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Morley, S., & Dowzer, C. N. (2014). The Leeds Reliable Change Indicator Simple Excel (tm) applications for the analysis of individual patient and group data Manual for the Leeds Reliable Change Indicator: Simple Excel (tm) applications for the analysis of individual patient and group data. https://dclinpsych.leeds.ac.uk/wp-content/uploads/sites/26/2018/09/Manual-for-Leeds-RCI-CSC-calculators.pdf Google Scholar
Muller, D., Khemlani-Patel, S., & Neziroglu, F. (2018). Cognitive-behavioral therapy for an adolescent female presenting with misophonia: a case example. Clinical Case Studies, 17, 249258. https://doi.org/10.1177/1534650118782650 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Persons, J. B. (2005). Empiricism, mechanism, and the practice of cognitive-behavior therapy. Behavior Therapy, 36, 107118. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0005-78940580059-0 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Persons, J. B., Roberts, N. A., Zalecki, C. A., & Brechwald, W. A. (2006). Naturalistic outcome of case formulation-driven cognitive-behavior therapy for anxious depressed outpatients. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 44, 10411051. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.brat.2005.08.005 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Reid, A. M., Guzick, A. G., Gernand, A., & Olsen, B. (2016). Intensive cognitive-behavioral therapy for comorbid misophonic and obsessive-compulsive symptoms: a systematic case study. Journal of Obsessive-Compulsive and Related Disorders, 10, 19. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jocrd.2016.04.009 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Rinaldi, L. J., Smees, R., Ward, J., & Simner, J. (2022). Poorer well-being in children with misophonia: evidence from the Sussex misophonia scale for adolescents. Frontiers in Psychology, 13, 808379.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rosenthal, M. Z., Anand, D., Cassiello-Robbins, C., Williams, Z. J., Guetta, R. E., Trumbull, J., & Kelley, L. D. (2021). Development and initial validation of the Duke Misophonia Questionnaire. Frontiers in Psychology, 12, 4197. https://doi.org/10.3389/FPSYG.2021.709928/BIBTEX CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rosenthal, M. Z., Shan, Y., & Trumbull, J. (2023). Treatment of misophonia. Advances in Psychiatry and Behavioral Health. https://doi.org/10.1016/J.YPSC.2023.03.009 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Rosenthal, M. Z., McMahon, K., Greenleaf, A. S., Cassiello-Robbins, C., Guetta, R., Trumbull, J., Anand, D., Frazer-Abel, E. S. and Kelley, L. (2022). Phenotyping misophonia: psychiatric disorders and medical health correlates. Frontiers in Psychology, 13. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2022.941898 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rouw, R., & Erfanian, M. (2018). A large-scale study of misophonia. Journal of Clinical Psychology, 74, 453479. https://doi.org/10.1002/jclp.22500 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Salkovskis, P. M., Gregory, J. D., Sedgwick-Taylor, A., White, J., Opher, S., & Ólafsdóttir, S. (2016). Extending cognitive-behavioural theory and therapy to medically unexplained symptoms and long-term physical conditions: a hybrid transdiagnostic/problem specific approach. Behaviour Change, 33, 172192. https://doi.org/10.1017/bec.2016.8 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Salkovskis, P. M., Hackmann, A., Wells, A., Gelder, M. G., & Clark, D. M. (2007). Belief disconfirmation versus habituation approaches to situational exposure in panic disorder with agoraphobia: a pilot study. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 45, 877885. https://doi.org/10.1016/J.BRAT.2006.02.008 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Schneider, R. L., & Arch, J. J. (2017). Case study: a novel application of mindfulness- and acceptance-based components to treat misophonia. Journal of Contextual Behavioral Science, 6, 221225. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jcbs.2017.04.003 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Schröder, A., Vulink, N., & Denys, D. (2013). Misophonia: diagnostic criteria for a new psychiatric disorder. PLoS One, 8. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0054706 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Schröder, A., Vulink, N., van Loon, A., & Denys, D. (2017). Cognitive behavioral therapy is effective in misophonia: an open trial. Journal of Affective Disorders, 217, 289294. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jad.2017.04.017 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Singer, D. A. (2018). The mixed methods case study of ‘Zara’: cognitive behavior therapy treatment of a college student with misophonia. Doctoral dissertation, Rutgers, The State University of New Jersey. https://doi.org/10.7282/T33J3HKH CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Spitzer, R. L., Kroenke, K., Williams, J. B. W., & Löwe, B. (2006). A brief measure for assessing generalized anxiety disorder: the GAD-7. Archives of Internal Medicine, 166, 10921097. https://doi.org/10.1001/ARCHINTE.166.10.1092 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Swedo, S. E., Baguley, D. M., Denys, D., Dixon, L. J., Erfanian, M., Fioretti, A., Jastreboff, P. J., Kumar, S., Rosenthal, M. Z., Rouw, R., Schiller, D., Simner, J., Storch, E. A., Taylor, S., Werff, K. R. V., Altimus, C. M., & Raver, S. M. (2022). Consensus definition of misophonia: a Delphi study. Frontiers in Neuroscience, 16, 224. https://doi.org/10.3389/FNINS.2022.841816/BIBTEX CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Thwaites, R., & Freeston, M. H. (2005). Safety-Seeking behaviours: fact or function? How can we clinically differentiate between safety behaviours and adaptive coping strategies across anxiety disorders? Behavioural and Cognitive Psychotherapy, 33, 177188. https://doi.org/10.1017/S1352465804001985 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Vitoratou, S., Hayes, C., Uglik-Marucha, N., Pearson, O., Graham, T., & Gregory, J. (2023). Misophonia in the UK: prevalence and norms from the S-Five in a UK representative sample. PLoS One, 18, e0282777. https://doi.org/10.1371/JOURNAL.PONE.0282777 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Vitoratou, S., Uglik-Marucha, N., Hayes, C., & Gregory, J. (2021). Listening to people with misophonia: exploring the multiple dimensions of sound intolerance using a new psychometric tool, the S-Five, in a large sample of individuals identifying with the condition. Psych, 3, 639662. https://doi.org/10.3390/PSYCH3040041 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Wu, M. S., Lewin, A. B., Murphy, T. K., & Storch, E. A. (2014). Misophonia: incidence, phenomenology, and clinical correlates in an undergraduate student sample. Journal of Clinical Psychology, 70, 9941007. https://doi.org/10.1002/jclp.22098 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Figure 0

Table 1. Summary of patient demographic information

Figure 1

Table 2. Summary of information about treatment format

Figure 2

Figure 1. Example of an individualised case formulation of misophonia, based on themes emerging from the case series but not pertaining to a specific patient.

Figure 3

Table 3. Examples of behavioural experiments for misophonia

Figure 4

Table 4. Comparisons between pre-treatment and follow-up measures of misophonia symptoms

Figure 5

Figure 2. Graph of reliable change index for the Misophonia Questionnaire from pre-treatment to follow-up.

Figure 6

Table 5. Comparisons between pre-treatment and follow-up measures of anxiety and depression

Submit a response

Comments

No Comments have been published for this article.