Hostname: page-component-586b7cd67f-dsjbd Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-11-28T02:23:10.174Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

The Mediating Roles of Disgust Sensitivity and Danger Expectancy in Relation to Hand Washing Behaviour

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  02 December 2010

Susan J. Thorpe*
Affiliation:
University of Surrey, Guildford, UK
Julie Barnett
Affiliation:
Brunel University, Uxbridge, UK
Katy Friend
Affiliation:
University of Surrey, Guildford, UK
Kate Nottingham
Affiliation:
University of Surrey, Guildford, UK
*
Reprint requests to S. J. Thorpe, Department of Psychology, Faculty of Arts and Human Sciences, University of Surrey, Guildford GU2 7XH, UK. E-mail: [email protected]

Abstract

Background: Recent interest in the role of vulnerability factors in obsessional washing has suggested that disgust sensitivity, danger expectancy and health anxiety may be of interest. Aims: This study explores the differential impact of these factors on both behavioural and cognitive measures of washing behaviour and is based on a replication of the Jones and Menzies (1997) experiment, during which participants immersed their hands in a noxious compound while rating themselves on a range of measures: the time they subsequently took to wash their hands was measured and danger expectancies were found to be the best predictor of this. Method: The present study added measures of disgust sensitivity and health anxiety to this experimental methodology while removing factors they found to be of little import to compulsive washing. Thirty non-clinical participants took part. Results: Results confirmed that disgust sensitivity was related to the behavioural measure of washing time, but that this relationship was almost entirely mediated by the danger expectancy concerning judgements of severity of consequent disease. However, a different pattern emerged when the outcome measure was questionnaire based: danger expectancy was not at all related to this. Disgust sensitivity mediated the relationship between health anxiety and scores on a questionnaire measure of washing compulsions. Interestingly, these scores were not related to the behavioural measure of washing time. Conclusions: The implications of these relationships to the further development of subtypes of Obsessive Compulsive Disorder (OCD) are discussed.

Type
Research Article
Copyright
Copyright © British Association for Behavioural and Cognitive Psychotherapies 2010

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

Baron, R. and Kenny, D. (1986). The moderator-mediator variable distinction in social psychological research: conceptual, strategic, and statistical considerations. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 51, 11731182.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Beck, A. and Steer, R. (1990). Manual for the Beck Anxiety Inventory. San Antonio, TX: Psychological Corporation.Google Scholar
Charash, M. and McKay, D. (2002). Attention bias for disgust. Journal of Anxiety Disorders, 16, 529541.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cisler, J. M., Olatunji, B. O., Sawchuk, C. N. and Lohr, J. M. (2008). Specificity of emotional maintenance processes among contamination fears and blood-injection-injury fears. Journal of Anxiety Disorders, 22, 915923.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Clark, D. (2005). Lumping versus splitting: a commentary on subtyping in OCD. Behavior Therapy, 36, 401404.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Coles, M., Frost, R., Heimberg, R. and Rhéaume, J. (2003). “Not just right experiences”: perfectionism, obsessive-compulsive features and general psychopathology. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 41, 681700.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cougle, J., Wolitzky-Taylor, K., Lee, H. and Telch, M. (2007). Mechanisms of change in ERP treatment of compulsive hand washing: does primary threat make a difference? Behaviour Research and Therapy, 45, 14491459.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Fairbrother, N., Newth, S. and Rachman, S. (2005). Mental pollution: feelings of dirtiness without physical contact. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 43, 121130.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Foa, E., Huppert, J., Leiberg, S., Langner, R., Kichic, R., Hajcak, G. and Salkovskis, P. (2002). The Obsessive-Compulsive Inventory: development and validation of a short version. Psychological Assessment, 14, 485496.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Frost, R. and Steketee, G. (1997). Perfectionism in obsessive-compulsive disorder patients. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 35, 291296.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gibbs, N. (1996). Nonclinical populations in research on obsessive-compulsive disorder: a critical review. Clinical Psychology Review, 16, 729773.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Haidt, J., McCauley, C. and Rozin, P. (1994). Individual differences in sensitivity to disgust: a scale sampling seven domains of disgust elicitors. Personality and Individual Differences, 16, 701713.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Jones, M. K. and Menzies, R. G. (1997). The cognitive mediation of obsessive-compulsive handwashing. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 35, 843850.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
MacKinnon, D. and Fairchild, A. (2009). Current directions in mediation analysis. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 18, 16.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Mancini, F., Gragnani, A. and D'Olimpio, F. (2001). The connection between disgust and obsessions and compulsions in a non-clinical sample. Personality and Individual Differences, 31, 11731180.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Mataix-Cols, D. (2006). Deconstructing obsessive-compulsive disorder: a multidimensional perspective. Current Opinion in Psychiatry, 19, 84.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Mataiz-Cols, D., Rosario-Campos, M. and Leckman, J. (2005). A multidimensional model of obsessive-compulsive disorder. American Journal of Psychiatry, 162, 228.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Menzies, R. G., Harris, L. M., Cumming, S. R. and Einstein, D. A. (2000). The relationship between inflated personal responsibility and exaggerated danger expectancies in obsessive-compulsive concerns. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 38, 10291037.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Moulding, R., Kyrios, M. and Doron, G. (2007). Obsessive-compulsive behaviours in specific situations: the relative influence of appraisals of control, responsibility and threat. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 45, 16931702.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Muris, P., Merckelbach, H., Nederkoorn, S., Rassin, E., Candel, I. and Horselenberg, R. (2000). Disgust and psychopathological symptoms in a nonclinical sample. Personality and Individual Differences, 29, 11631167.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Nunnally, J. (1978). Psychometric Theory. New York: McGraw-Hill.Google Scholar
Olatunji, B. O. (2009). Incremental specificity of disgust propensity and sensitivity in the prediction of health anxiety dimensions. Journal of Behavior Therapy and Experimental Psychiatry, 40, 230239.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Olatunji, B. O., Abramowitz, J. S., Williams, N. L., Connolly, K. M. and Lohr, J. M. (2007). Scrupulosity and obsessive-compulsive symptoms: confirmatory factor analysis and validity of the Penn Inventory of Scrupulosity. Journal of Anxiety Disorders, 21, 771787.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Olatunji, B. O., Cisler, J. M., Deacon, B. J., Connolly, K. and Lohr, J. M. (2007). The Disgust Propensity and Sensitivity Scale-Revised: psychometric properties and specificity in relation to anxiety disorder symptoms. Journal of Anxiety Disorders, 21, 918930.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Olatunji, B. O., Forsyth, J. P. and Cherian, A. (2007). Evaluative differential conditioning of disgust: a sticky form of relational learning that is resistant to extinction. Journal of Anxiety Disorders, 21, 820834.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Olatunji, B. O., Lohr, J. M., Sawchuk, C. N. and Tolin, D. F. (2007). Multimodal assessment of disgust in contamination-related obsessive-compulsive disorder. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 45, 263276.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Olatunji, B. O., Sawchuk, C. N., Lohr, J. M. and de Jong, P. J. (2004). Disgust domains in the prediction of contamination fear. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 42, 93104.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Pilowsky, I. (1967). Dimensions of hypochondriasis. The British Journal of Psychiatry, 113, 89.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rachman, S. (1974). Primary obsessional slowness. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 12, 918.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rachman, S. (1993). Obsessions, responsibility and guilt. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 31, 249278.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rachman, S. (2007). Unwanted intrusive images in obsessive compulsive disorders. Journal of Behavior Therapy and Experimental Psychiatry, 38, 402410.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rachman, S. and Hodgson, R. (1980). Obsessions and Compulsions: New York: Prentice Hall.Google Scholar
Radomsky, A. and Taylor, S. (2005). Subtyping OCD: prospects and problems. Behavior Therapy, 36, 371379.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Reiss, S. (1991). Expectancy model of fear, anxiety, and panic. Clinical Psychology Review, 11, 141153.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Reiss, S. and McNally, R. J. (1985). Expectancy model of fear. Theoretical Issues in Behavior Therapy, 107–121.Google Scholar
Reiss, S., Peterson, R. A., Gursky, D. M. and McNally, R. J. (1986). Anxiety sensitivity, anxiety frequency and the prediction of fearfulness. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 24, 18.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rhéaume, J., Ladouceur, R., Freeston, M. and Letarte, H. (1995). Inflated responsibility in obsessive compulsive disorder: validation of an operational definition. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 33, 159169.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Salkovskis, P. and Forrester, E. (2002). Responsibility. In Frost, R. O. and Steketee, G. (Eds.), Cognitive Approaches to Obsessions and Compulsions: theory, assessment, and treatment (pp. 4561). Amsterdam: Pergamon/Elsevier.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Salkovskis, P., Shafran, R., Rachman, S. and Freeston, M. (1999). Multiple pathways to inflated responsibility beliefs in obsessional problems: possible origins and implications for therapy and research. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 37, 10551072.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Salkovskis, P., Wroe, A., Gledhill, A., Morrison, N., Forrester, E., Richards, C., Reynolds, M. and Thorpe, S. (2000). Responsibility attitudes and interpretations are characteristic of obsessive compulsive disorder. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 38, 347372.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Sobel, M. (1982). Asymptotic confidence intervals for indirect effects in structural equation models. Sociological Methodology, 13, 290312.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Sookman, D., Abramowitz, J., Calamari, J., Wilhelm, S. and McKay, D. (2005). Subtypes of obsessive-compulsive disorder: implications for specialized cognitive behavior therapy. Behavior Therapy, 36, 393400.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Tallis, F. (1996). Compulsive washing in the absence of phobic and illness anxiety. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 34, 361362.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Taylor, S. and Cox, B. (1998a). Anxiety sensitivity: multiple dimensions and hierarchic structure. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 36, 3751.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Taylor, S. and Cox, B. (1998b). An expanded anxiety sensitivity index evidence for a hierarchic structure in a clinical sample. Journal of Anxiety Disorders, 12, 463483.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Thorpe, S. J., Patel, S. P. and Simonds, L. M. (2003). The relationship between disgust sensitivity, anxiety and obsessions. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 41, 13971409.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Thorpe, S. J. and Salkovskis, P. M. (1998). Studies on the role of disgust in the acquisition and maintenance of specific phobias. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 36, 877893.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Tolin, D. F., Woods, C. M. and Abramowitz, J. S. (2006). Disgust sensitivity and obsessive-compulsive symptoms in a non-clinical sample. Journal of Behavior Therapy and Experimental Psychiatry, 37, 3040.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Van Overveld, W., de Jong, P., Peters, M., Cavanagh, K. and Davey, G. (2006). Disgust propensity and disgust sensitivity: separate constructs that are differentially related to specific fears. Personality and Individual Differences, 41, 12411252.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Williams, S. and Watson, N. (1985). Perceived danger and perceived self-efficacy as cognitive mediators of acrophobic behavior. Behavior Therapy, 16, 136146.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Woody, S. and Tolin, D. (2002). The relationship between disgust sensitivity and avoidant behavior: studies of clinical and nonclinical samples. Journal of Anxiety Disorders, 16, 543559.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Submit a response

Comments

No Comments have been published for this article.