Hostname: page-component-586b7cd67f-2brh9 Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-11-27T20:06:00.577Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Learning the Futility of the Thought Suppression Enterprise in Normal Experience and in Obsessive Compulsive Disorder

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  26 October 2009

Sadia Najmi*
Affiliation:
University of California, San Diego/San Diego State University, USA
Hannah Reese
Affiliation:
Harvard University, Cambridge, USA
Sabine Wilhelm
Affiliation:
Massachusetts General Hospital and Harvard Medical School, USA
Jeanne Fama
Affiliation:
Massachusetts General Hospital and Harvard Medical School, USA
Celeste Beck
Affiliation:
Harvard University, Cambridge, USA
Daniel M. Wegner
Affiliation:
Harvard University, Cambridge, USA
*
Reprint requests to Sadia Najmi, Joint Doctoral Program at University of California, San Diego/San Diego State University, Center for Understanding and Treating Anxiety, 6386 Alvarado Ct., Suite 301, San Diego, CA 92120, USA. E-mail: [email protected]

Abstract

Background: The belief that we can control our thoughts is not inevitably adaptive, particularly when it fuels mental control activities that have ironic unintended consequences. The conviction that the mind can and should be controlled can prompt people to suppress unwanted thoughts, and so can set the stage for the intrusive return of those very thoughts. An important question is whether or not these beliefs about the control of thoughts can be reduced experimentally. One possibility is that behavioral experiments aimed at revealing the ironic return of suppressed thoughts might create a lesson that could reduce unrealistic beliefs about the control of thoughts. Aims: The present research assessed the influence of the thought suppression demonstration on beliefs about the control of thoughts in a non-clinical sample, and among individuals with obsessive-compulsive disorder (OCD). Method: In Study 1, we assessed the effect of the thought suppression demonstration on beliefs about the control of thoughts among low and high obsessive individuals in the non-clinical population (N = 62). In Study 2, we conducted a similar study with individuals with OCD (N = 29). Results: Results suggest that high obsessive individuals in the non-clinical population are able to learn the futility of suppression through the thought suppression demonstration and to alter their faulty beliefs about the control of thoughts; however, for individuals with OCD, the demonstration may be insufficient for altering underlying beliefs. Conclusions: For individuals with OCD, the connection between suppressing a neutral thought in the suppression demonstration and suppressing a personally relevant obsession may need to be stated explicitly in order to affect their obsessive beliefs.

Type
Research Article
Copyright
Copyright © British Association for Behavioural and Cognitive Psychotherapies 2009

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

Abramowitz, J. S., Tolin, D. F. and Street, G. P. (2001). Paradoxical effects of thought suppression: a meta-analysis of controlled studies. Clinical Psychology Review, 21, 683703.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Baer, L. (2001). The Imp of the Mind: exploring the silent epidemic of obsessive bad thoughts. New York: Dutton/Penguin Books.Google Scholar
Clark, D. A. (2001). Unwanted mental intrusions in clinical disorders: an introduction. Journal of Cognitive Psychotherapy: An International Quarterly, 16, 161178.Google Scholar
Clark, D. A. (2005). Intrusive Thoughts in Clinical Disorders: theory, research, and treatment. New York: Guilford Press.Google Scholar
Dalgleish, T. and Yiend, J. (2006). The effects of suppressing a negative autobiographical memory on concurrent intrusions and subsequent autobiographical recall in dysphoria. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 115, 467473.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
First, M. B., Spitzer, R. L., Gibbon, M. and Williams, J. B. W. (1995). Structured Clinical Interview for DSM-IV Axis I Disorders.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Flavell, J. H. (1979). Metacognition and cognitive monitoring: a new area of cognitive developmental inquiry. American Psychologist, 34, 906911.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Goodman, W. K., Price, L. H., Rasmussen, S. A., Mazure, C., Fleischmann, R. L., Hill, C. L., Heninger, G. R. and Charney, D. S. (1989a). The Yale-Brown Obsessive Compulsive Scale: II. development, use, and reliability. Archives of General Psychiatry, 46, 10061011.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Goodman, W. K., Price, L. H., Rasmussen, S. A., Mazure, C., Delgado, P., Heninger, G. R. and Charney, D. S. (1989b). The Yale-Brown Obsessive Compulsive Scale: II. validity. Archives of General Psychiatry, 46, 10121016.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Guthrie, R. and Bryant, R. A. (2000). Attempting suppression of traumatic memories over extended periods in acute stress disorder. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 38, 899907.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Harvey, A. G. and Bryant, R. A. (1998). The effect of attempted thought suppression in acute stress disorder. Behavior Research and Therapy, 36, 583590.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Janeck, A. S. and Calamari, J. E. (1999). Thought suppression in obsessive-compulsive disorder. Cognitive Therapy and Research, 23, 497509.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kelly, A. E. and Kahn, J. H. (1994). Effects of suppression of personal intrusive thoughts. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 66, 9981006.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Najmi, S., Riemann, B. and Wegner, D. M. (in press). Managing unwanted intrusive thoughts in obsessive-compulsive disorder: relative effectiveness of suppression, focused distraction, and acceptance. Behaviour Research and Therapy.Google Scholar
Najmi, S. and Wegner, D. M. (2008). Thought suppression and psychopathology. In Elliot, A. J. (Ed.), Handbook of Approach and Avoidance Motivation (pp. 447459). New York: Psychology Press.Google Scholar
McNally, R. J. and Ricciardi, J. N. (1996). Suppression of negative and neutral thoughts. Behavioural and Cognitive Psychotherapy, 24, 1725.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Obsessive Compulsive Cognitions Working Group (1997). Cognitive assessment of obsessive-compulsive disorder. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 35, 667681.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Obsessive Compulsive Cognitions Working Group (2001). Development and initial validation of the obsessive beliefs questionnaire and the interpretation of intrusions inventory. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 39, 9871006.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Obsessive Compulsive Cognitions Working Group (2003). Psychometric validation of the Obsessive Beliefs Questionnaire and the Interpretation of Intrusions Inventory: Part I. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 41, 863878.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Purdon, C. L. and Clark, D. A. (1993). Obsessive intrusive thoughts in nonclinical subjects: Part I. content and relation with depressive, anxious and obsessional symptoms. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 31, 713720.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Purdon, C. L. and Clark, D. A. (2000). White bears and other elusive intrusions: assessing the relevance of thought suppression for obsessional phenomena. Behavior Modification, 24, 425453.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Purdon, C. L. and Clark, D. A. (2001). Suppression of obsession-like thoughts in nonclinical individuals: impact on thought frequency, appraisal and mood state. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 39, 11631181.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Purdon, C. L., Rowa, K. and Antony, M. M. (2005). Thought suppression and its effects on thought frequency, appraisal and mood state in individuals with obsessive-compulsive disorder. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 43, 93108.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Rachman, S. J. (1997). A cognitive theory of obsessions. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 35, 793802.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rachman, S. J. and de Silva, P. (1978). Abnormal and normal obsessions. Research and Therapy, 16, 233248.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rassin, E. (2005). Thought Suppression. New York: Elsevier.Google Scholar
Salkovskis, P. M. (1985). Obsessional-compulsive problems: a cognitive-behavioral analysis. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 23, 571583.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Salkovskis, P. M. and Campbell, P. (1994). Thought suppression induces intrusion in naturally occurring negative intrusive thoughts. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 32, 18.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Salkovskis, P. M. and Harrison, J. (1984). Abnormal and normal obsessions: a replication. Research and Therapy, 23, 571584.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Salkovskis, P. M. and Reynolds, M. (1994). Thought suppression and smoking cessation. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 32, 193201.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Shipherd, J. C. and Beck, J. G. (1999). The effects of suppressing trauma-related thoughts on women with rape-related posttraumatic stress disorder. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 37, 99112.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Taylor, S. (1995). Assessment of obsessions and compulsions: reliability, validity, and sensitivity to treatment effects. Clinical Psychology Review, 15, 261296.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Tolin, D. F., Abramowitz, J. S., Hamlin, C., Foa, E. B. and Synodi, D. S. (2002a). Attributions for thought suppression failure in obsessive-compulsive disorder. Cognitive Therapy and Research, 26, 505517.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Tolin, D. F., Abramowitz, J. S., Przeworski, A. and Foa, E. B. (2002b). Thought suppression in obsessive-compulsive disorder. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 40, 12551274.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Trinder, H. and Salkovskis, P. M. (1994). Personally relevant intrusions outside the laboratory: long-term suppression increases intrusion. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 32, 833842.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Wegner, D. M. (1994). Ironic processes of mental control. Psychological Review, 101, 3452.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Wegner, D. M., Schneider, D. J., Carter III, S. and White, L. (1987). Paradoxical effects of thought suppression. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 53, 513.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Wenzlaff, R. M. and Bates, D. E. (1998). Unmasking a cognitive vulnerability to depression: how lapses in mental control reveal depressive thinking. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 75, 15591571.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Wenzlaff, R. M. and Eisenberg, A. R. (2001). Mental control after dysphoria: evidence of a suppressed, depressive bias. Behavior Therapy, 32, 2745.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Wenzlaff, R. M., Meir, J. and Salas, D. M. (2002). Thought suppression and memory biases during and after depressive moods. Cognition and Emotion, 16, 403422.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Wenzlaff, R. M., Wegner, D. M. and Roper, D. W. (1988). Depression and mental control: the resurgence of unwanted negative thoughts. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 55, 882892.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Wenzlaff, R. M. and Wegner, D. M. (2000). Thought suppression. In Fiske, S. T. (Ed.), Annual Review of Psychology: Vol. 51 (pp. 5991). Palo Alto, CA: Annual Reviews.Google Scholar
Wilhelm, S. and Steketee, G. (2006). Cognitive Therapy for Obsessive-Compulsive Disorder: a guide for professionals. Oakland, CA: New Harbinger Press.Google Scholar
Wilhelm, S., Steketee, G., Reilly-Harrington, N., Deckersbach, T., Buhlmann, U. and Baer, L. (2005). Effectiveness of cognitive therapy for obsessive-compulsive disorder: an open trial. Journal of Cognitive Psychotherapy, 19, 173179.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Submit a response

Comments

No Comments have been published for this article.