No CrossRef data available.
Article contents
Real people, ordinary language, and natural measurement
Published online by Cambridge University Press: 04 February 2010
Abstract
An abstract is not available for this content so a preview has been provided. Please use the Get access link above for information on how to access this content.
- Type
- Open Peer Commentary
- Information
- Copyright
- Copyright © Cambridge University Press 1984
References
Anderson, J. A. (1973) A theory for the recognition of items from short memorized lists. Psychological Review 80:417–38. [RDL]CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Baer, D. M. (1982) Some recommendations for a modest reduction in the rate of current recommendations for an immodest increase in the rate of exclusive usages of rate as a dependent measure. Paper presented at the meeting of the Association for Behavior Analysis, Milwaukee. [SMD]Google Scholar
Bealer, G. (1978) An inconsistency in functionalism. Synthèse 38:332–72. [RJN]CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Blough, D. S. (1963) Interresponse time as a function of continuous variables: A new method and some data. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior 6:237–46. [SR]CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Blough, D. S. (1972) Recognition by the pigeon of stimuli varying in two dimensions. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior 18:345–67. [SR]CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Blough, D. S. (1978) Reaction times of pigeons on a wavelength discrimination task. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior 30:163–67. [SR]CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Blough, D. S. (1982) Pigeon perception of letters of the alphabet. Science 218:397–98. [SR]CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Breland, K. & Breland, M. (1961) The misbehavior of organisms. American Psychologist 16:681–84. [RM]CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bunge, M. (1980) The mind-body problem: A psychological approach. Pergamon Press. [MNR]Google Scholar
Bush, R. R. (1960) A survey of mathematical learning theory. In: Developments in mathematical psychology, ed. Luce, R. D., Bush, R. R. & Licklider, J. C. R.. Free Press. [KMS]Google Scholar
Changeux, J.-P. & Danchin, A. (1976) Selective stabilization of developing synapses as a mechanism for the specification of neuronal networks. Nature 264:705–12. [MNR]CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Chomsky, N. (1959) A review of B. F. Skinner's Verbal behavior. Language 35:26–58. [RM, MNR, CPS, rBFS, ES]CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Craig, W. (1953) On axiomalizability within a system. Journal of Symbolic Logic 18:30–32. [ES]CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Craig, W. (1956) Replacement of auxiliary expressions. Philosophical Review 65:38–55. [ES]Google Scholar
Deitz, S. M. (1978) Current status of applied behavior analysis: Science versus technology. American Psychologist 33:805–14. [SMD]CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Deitz, S. M. (1982) Defining applied behavior analysis: An historical analogy. Behavior Analyst 5:53–64. [SMD]CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Deitz, S. M. & Arrington, R. L. (1983) Factors confusing language use in the analysis of behavior. 11:117–32. Behaviorism. [SMD]Google Scholar
Epstein, R., Lanza, R. P. & Skinner, B. F. (1980) Symbolic communication between two pigeons (Columba Livia domestica). Science 207:543–45. [CPS]CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Fodor, J. A. (1981) The mind-body problem. Scientific American 244:114–23. [SMD]CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gibbon, J. (1977) Scalar expectancy theory and Weber's law in animal timing. Psychological Review 84:279–325. [CPS]CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gudmondsson, K. (1983) The emergence of B. F. Skinner's theory of operant behaviour. Ph.D. thesis, University of Western Ontario. [JMN]Google Scholar
Gustavson, C. R., Garcia, J., Hankins, W. G. & Rusiniak, K. W. (1974) Coyote predation control by aversive conditioning. Science 184:581–83. [SR]CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Harzem, P. & Miles, T. R. (1978) Conceptual issues in operant psychology. Wiley. [SMD]Google Scholar
Hempel, C. G. (1980) The logical analysis of psychology. In: Readings in philosophy of psychology, vol. 1, ed. Block, N.. Harvard University Press. [RM]Google Scholar
Hooke, R. (1980) Getting people to use statistics properly. American Statistician 34:39–42. [LW]CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hulse, S. H. & O'Leary, D. K. (1982) Serial pattern learning: Teaching an alphabet to rats. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Animal Behavior Processes 8:260–73. [CPS]Google Scholar
Meck, W. H. (1983) Selective adjustment of the speed of internal clock and memory processes. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Animal Behavior Processes 9:171–201. [SR]Google ScholarPubMed
Mehler, J. (1974) Connaître par desapprentissage. In: L'unité de l'homme, ed. Morin, E. & Piatelli-Palmarini, . Le Seuil. [MNR]Google Scholar
Morse, W. H. & Skinner, B. F. (1958) Some factors involved in the stimulus control of operant behavior. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior 1:103–7. [rBFS]CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Mowrer, O. H. & Jones, H. M. (1943) Extinction and behavior variability as functions of effortfulness of task. Journal of Experimental Psychology 33:369–86. [tcBFS]CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Nelson, R. J. (1969) Behaviorism is false. Journal of Philosophy 66:417–51. [RJN]CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Neyman, J. & Pearson, E. S. (1967) Joint statistical papers. Cambridge University Press. [FHCM]CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Popper, K. (1969) Science: Conjectures and refutations. In: Conjectures and refutations. Basic Books. [ES]Google Scholar
Prelec, D. (1982) Matching, maximizing, and the hyperbolic reinforcement feedback function. Psychological Review 89:189–230. [RDL]CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Rescorla, R. A. & Wagner, A. R. (1972) A theory of Pavlovian conditioning: Variations in the effectiveness of reinforcement and nonreinforcement. In: Classical conditioning, vol. 2, Current research and theory, ed. Black, A. H. & Prokasy, W. F.. Appleton-Century-Crofts. [SR]Google Scholar
Richelle, M. (1976) Formal analysis and functional analysis of verbal behavior: Notes on the debate between Chomsky and Skinner (transl. Foster, W. S. & Rondal, J. A.). Behaviorism 4:209–21. [MNR]Google Scholar
Roberts, S. (1981) Isolation of an internal clock. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Animal Behavior Processes 7:242–68. [SR]Google ScholarPubMed
Roberts, S. (1982) Cross-modal use of an internal clock. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Animal Behavior Processes 8:2–22. [SR]Google ScholarPubMed
Roberts, S. & Holder, M. D. (in press) What starts an internal clock? Journal of Experimental Psychology: Animal Behavior Processes. [SR]Google Scholar
Rorty, R. (1979) Philosophy and the mirror of nature. Princeton University Press. [CPS]Google Scholar
Rozeboom, W. W. (1958) “What is learned?” – an empirical enigma. Psychological Review 65:22–33. [WWR]CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Rozeboom, W. W. (1961) Ontological induction and the logical typology of scientific variables. Philosophy of Science 28:337–77. [WWR]CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Rozeboom, W. W. (1966) Scaling theory and the nature of measurement. Synthèse, 16:170–233. [WWR]CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Rozeboom, W. W. (1967) Conditioned generalization, cognitive set, and the structure of human learning. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior 6:491–500. [WWR]CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Rozeboom, W. W. (1970) The art of metascience, or, what should a psychological theory be? In: Toward unification in psychology, ed. Royce, J. R.. Toronto University Press. [WWR]Google Scholar
Rozeboom, W. W. (1972) Scientific inference: The myth and the reality. In: Science, psychology and communication, ed. Brown, S. R. & Brenner, D. J.. Teachers College Press. [WWR]Google Scholar
Rozeboom, W. W. (1973) Dispositions revisited. Philosophy of Science 40:59–74. [WWR]CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Rozeboom, W. W. (1982) Let's dump hypothetico-deductivism for the right reasons. Philosophy of Science 49:637–47. [WWR]CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Rozeboom, W. W. (1984) Dispositions do explain. In: Annals of theoretical psychology, vol. 1, ed. Royce, J. R. & Mos, L. P.. Plenum Press. [WWR]Google Scholar
Rozeboom, W. W. (forthcoming) Do mental systems have a scientific future? [WWR]Google Scholar
Schwartz, B. (1974) On going back tonature: A review of Seligman and Hager's Biological boundaries of learning. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior 21:183–98. [BM]CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Seligman, M. E. P. & Hager, J. L., eds. (1972) Biological boundaries of learning. Appleton-Century-Crofts. [BM]Google Scholar
Shimp, C. P. (1975) Perspectives on the behavioral unit: Choice behavior in animals. In: Handbook of learning and cognitive processes, ed. Estes, W. K., vol. 2. Lawrence Erlbaum Associates. [CPS]Google Scholar
Shimp, C. P. (1976) Organization in memory and behavior. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior 26:113–30. [CPS]CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Shimp, C. P. (in press, a) Relations between memory and operant behavior, according to AL. Canadian Journal of Psychology. [CPS]Google Scholar
Shimp, C. P. (in press, b) Timing, learning and forgetting. In: Timing, ed. Gibbon, J. & Allen, L.. New York Academy of Sciences. [CPS]Google Scholar
Siegel, S. (1979) The role of conditioning in drug tolerance and addietion. In: Psychopathology in animals: Research and treatment implications, ed. Keehn, J. D.. Academic Press. [SR]Google Scholar
Skinner, B. F. (1931) The concept of the reflex in the description of behavior. Journal of General Psychology 5:427–58. [JMN]CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Skinner, B. F. (1932) Drive and reflex strength, I. Journal of General Psychology 6:22–37. [rBFS]CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Skinner, B. F. (1935) The generic nature of stimulus and response. Journal of General Psychology 12:40–65. [rBFS]CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Skinner, B. F. (1957) Verbal behavior. Appleton-Century-Crofts. (MNR, rBFS, ES]CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Skinner, B. F. (1966) Operant behavior. In: Operant behavior: Areas of research and applications, ed. Honig, W. K.. Appleton-Century-Crofts. [CPS]Google Scholar
Skinner, B. F. (1969) Contingencies of reinforcement: A theoretical analysis. Prentice-Hall. [SMD, KMS]Google Scholar
Skinner, B. F. (1972) Cumulative record: A selection of papers. Appleton-Century-Crofts. [JMN]Google Scholar
Skinner, B. F. (1979) The shaping of a behaviorist: Part two of an autobiography. Alfred Knopf. [JMN]Google Scholar
Stevens, S. S. (1957) On the psychophysical law. Psychological Review 64:153–81. [JTT]CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Straub, R. O., Seidenberg, M. S., Bever, T. G. & Terrace, H. S. (1979) Serial learning in the pigeon. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior 32:137–48. [CPS]CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Suppes, P. (1975) From behaviorism to neobehaviorism. Theory and Decision 6:269–86. [RJN]CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Townsend, J. T. (1972) Some results concerning the identifiability of parallel and serial processes. Journal of Mathematical Psychology 25:168–199. [JTT]Google Scholar
Townsend, J. T. & Ashby, F. G. (1983) The stochastic modeling of elementary psychological processes. Cambridge University Press. [JTT]Google Scholar
Tukey, J. W. (1980) Teaching of statistics: We need both exploratory and confirmatory. American Statistician 34:23–25. [LW]CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Westby, G. (1966) Psychology today: Problems and directions. Bulletin of the British Psychological Society 19(65). [taBFS]Google Scholar