Hostname: page-component-586b7cd67f-rcrh6 Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-11-28T01:37:14.421Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

The “putting the baby down” hypothesis: Bipedalism, babbling, and baby slings

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  14 February 2005

Dean Falk*
Affiliation:
Department of Anthropology, Florida State University, Tallahassee, FL32306-4531http://www.anthro.fsu.edu/people/faculty/falk.html

Abstract:

My responses to the observations and criticisms of 26 commentaries focus on the coregulated and affective nature of initial mother/infant interactions, the relationship between motherese and emergent linguistic skills and its implication for hominin evolution, the plausibility of the “putting the baby down” hypothesis, and details about specific neurological substrates that may have formed the basis for the evolution of prelinguistic behaviors and, eventually, protolanguage.

Type
Author's Response
Copyright
Copyright © Cambridge University Press 2004

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

Aboitiz, F. & García, R. (1997a) The anatomy of language revisited. Biological Research 30:171–83. [EG]Google Scholar
Aboitiz, F. & García, R. (1997b) The evolutionary origin of the language areas in the human brain. A neuroanatomical perspective. Brain Research Reviews 25:381–96. [FA, EG]Google Scholar
Aboitiz, F. & Montiel, J. (2003) One hundred million years of interhemispheric communication: The history of the corpus callosum. Brazilian Journal of Medical and Biological Research 36:409–20. [FA]Google Scholar
Ackerman, B. (1983) Form and function in children's understanding of ironic utterances. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology 35:487508. [MM]Google Scholar
Aiello, L. C. & Wells, J. C. K. (2002) Energetics and the evolution of the genus Homo. Annual Review of Anthropology 31:323–38. [AF]Google Scholar
Ainsworth, M., Blehar, M. C., Waters, E. & Wall, S. (1978) Patterns of attachment: A psychological study of the strange situation. Erlbaum. [aDF]Google Scholar
Aitken, K. & Trevarthen, C. (1997) Self-other organization in human psychological development. Development and Psychopathology 9:653–77. [ED]Google Scholar
Akhtar, N. & Tomasello, M. (1998) Intersubjectivity in early language learning and use. In: Intersubjective communication and emotion in early ontogeny, ed. Braten, S.. Cambridge University Press. [SB]Google Scholar
Allison, T., Puce, A. & McCarthy, G. (2000) Social perception from visual cues: Role of the STS region. Trends in Cognitive Sciences 4(2):267–78. [DD]Google Scholar
Altmann, J. (1980) Baboon mothers and infants. Harvard University Press. [aDF]Google Scholar
Andruski, J. E., Kuhl, P. K. & Hayashi, A. (1999) Point vowels in Japanese mothers’ speech to infants and adults. Journal of the Acoustical Society of America 105:1095–96. [aDF]Google Scholar
Armstrong, D. F., Stokoe, W. C. & Wilcox, S. E. (1994) Signs of the origin of syntax. Current Anthropology 35:349–68. [aDF]Google Scholar
Bachorowski, J., Smoski, M. J. & Owren, M. J. (2001) The acoustic features of human laughter. Journal of the Acoustical Society of America 110:1581–97. [aDF]Google Scholar
Baldwin, J. M. (1906) Mental development in the child and the race. Augustus Kelly. [aDF]Google Scholar
Bard, K. (1998) Social-experiential contribution to imitation and emotion in chimpanzees. In: Intersubjective communication and emotion in early ontogeny, ed. Braten, S.. Cambridge University Press. [SB]Google Scholar
Barr, R. G., Hopkins, B. & Green, J. A., eds. (2000) Crying as a sign, a signal, and a symptom. MacKeith Press. [RS]Google Scholar
Barrett, L., Dunbar, R. I. M. & Dunbar, P. (1995) Mother-infant contact as contingent behaviour in gelada baboons. Animal Behaviour 49:805–10. [aDF]Google Scholar
Bateson, M. (1979) The epigenesis of conversational interaction: A personal account of research development. In: Before speech: The beginning of interpersonal communication, ed. Bullowa, M.. Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Beck, B. B. (1975) Primate tool behavior. In: Primate socioecology and psychology, ed. Tuttle, R. H.. Mouton. [RS]Google Scholar
Beck, B. B. (1980) Animal tool behavior. Garland Press. [RS]Google Scholar
Beebe, B. (1982) Micro-timing in mother-infant communication. In: Nonverbal communication today, ed. Key, M. R., pp. 168–95. Mouton. [ED]Google Scholar
Begun, D. & Walker, A. (1993) The endocast. In: The Nariokotome Homo erectus skeleton, ed. Walker, A. & Leakey, R.. Harvard University Press. [CLB]Google Scholar
Bell, S. M. & Ainsworth, M. D. S. (1972) Infant crying and maternal responsiveness. Child Development 43:1171–90. [JLL]Google Scholar
Bermejo, M. & Omedes, A. (1999) Preliminary vocal repertoire and vocal communication of wild bonobos (Pan paniscus) at Lilungu (Democratic Republic of Congo). Folia Primatologica 70:328–57. [aDF]Google Scholar
Biben, M. (1992) Allomaternal vocal behavior in squirrel monkeys. Developmental Psychobiology 25:7992. [JDN]Google Scholar
Biben, M., Symmes, D. & Bernhards, D. (1988) Contour variables in vocal communication between squirrel monkey mothers and infants. Developmental Psychobiology 22(6):617–31. [MM]Google Scholar
Bickerton, D. (2002) Foraging versus social intelligence in the evolution of protolanguage. In: The transition to language, ed. Wray, A.. Oxford University Press. [aDF]Google Scholar
Binkofski, F., Amunts, K., Stephan, K. M., Posse, S., Schormann, T., Freund, H. J., Zilles, K. & Seitz, R. J. (2000) Broca's region subserves imagery of motion: A combined cytoarchitectonic and fMRI study. Human Brain Mapping 11:273–85. [AR]Google Scholar
Blonder, L. X., Bowers, D. & Heilman, K. M. (1991) The role of the right hemisphere in emotional communication. Brain 114:1115–27. [MM]Google Scholar
Blurton Jones, N. (1972) Comparative aspects of mother-child contact. In: Ethological studies in child behaviour, ed. Blurton Jones, N.. Cambridge University Press. [JLL]Google Scholar
Boesch, C. (1991) Teaching among wild chimpanzees. Animal Behaviour 41:530–32. [aDF]Google Scholar
Boesch, C. & Boesch-Achermann, H. (1991) Dim forest, bright chimps. Natural History 9:5057. [aDF]Google Scholar
Boesch, C., Hohmann, G. & Marchant, L. F., eds. (2002) Behavioral diversity on chimpanzees and bonobos. Cambridge University Press. [AF]Google Scholar
Bogin, B. (1999) Patterns of human growth, 2nd edition. Cambridge University Press. [JLL]Google Scholar
Bolinger, D. (1972) Intonation. Penguin. [MM]Google Scholar
Bolinger, D. (1986) Intonation and its parts: Melody in spoken English. Stanford University Press. [SW]Google Scholar
Bortz, W. M. Jr. (1985) Physical exercise as an evolutionary force. Journal of Human Evolution 14:145–55. [CLB]Google Scholar
Bowers, D., Bauer, R. M. & Heilman, K. M. (1993) The nonverbal affect lexicon: Theoretical perspectives from neuropsychological studies of affect perception. Neuropsychology 7:433–44. [MM]Google Scholar
Brace, C. L. (1963) Structural reduction in evolution. The American Naturalist 64:3949. [CLB]Google Scholar
Brace, C. L. (1995) The stages of human evolution, 5th edition. Prentice-Hall. [CLB]Google Scholar
Brace, C. L. (2000) Evolution in an anthropological view. AltaMira Press. [CLB]Google Scholar
Brace, C. L. (2000) (in press) “Neutral theory” and the dynamics of the evolution of “modern” Human morphology. Proceedings of the XV ICAES, July 612, 2003. Florence, Italy. [CLB]Google Scholar
Brace, C. L., Mahler, P. E. & Rosen, R. B. (1973) Tooth measurements and the rejection of the taxon “Homo habilis. Yearbook of Physical Anthropology 16:5068. [CLB]Google Scholar
Bramble, D. M. & Currier, D. R. (1983) Running and breathing in mammals. Science 219:251–56. [RRP]Google Scholar
Braten, S. (1974) Coding simulation circuits during symbolic interaction. Proceedings of the 7th International Congress on Cybernetics, pp. 327–36. Association Internationale de Cybernetique. [SB]Google Scholar
Braten, S. (1988) Dialogic mind: The infant and the adult in protoconversation. In: Nature, Cognition and System I, ed. Carvallo, M.. Kluwer. [SB]Google Scholar
Braten, S., ed. (1998) Infant learning by altercentric participation: the reverse of egocentric observation in autism. In: Intersubjective Communication and Emotion in Early Ontogeny. Cambridge University Press. [SB]Google Scholar
Braten, S., ed. (2000) From zoo observations of chimpanzees with offspring. Appendix in a bilingual collection: Modellmakt og altersentriske spedbarn. Essays on Dialogue in Infant and Adult. Sigma. [SB]Google Scholar
Braten, S., ed. (2001) Delaktige spedbarn og dialogpartnere. Om altersentrisk persepsjon. Det Norske Videnskaps-Akademi Arbok 2000 (Yearbook Proceedings 2000). The Norwegian Academy of Science and Letters. [SB]Google Scholar
Braten, S., ed. (2002) Altercentric perception by infants and adults in dialogue. In: Mirror neurons and the evolution of brain and language, ed. Stamenov, M. & Gallese, V.. John Benjamins. [SB]Google Scholar
Braten, S., ed. (2003a) Beteiligte Spiegelung. Alterzentrische Lernprozesse in der Kleinkindentwicklung und der Evolution. In: Subjekte und Gesellschaft, ed. Wenzel, U., Gretzinger, B. & Holz, K.. Velbruck. [SB]Google Scholar
Braten, S., ed. (2003b) Participant perception of others’ acts: Virtual otherness in infants and adults. Culture & Psychology 9(3):261–76. [SB]Google Scholar
Brown, J. (1988) Cingulate gyrus and supplementary motor correlates of vocalization in man. In: The physiological control of mammalian vocalization, ed Newman, J. D.. Plenum. [JDN]Google Scholar
Bruner, J. (1983) Child's talk: Learning to use language. Oxford University Press. [BJK]Google Scholar
Bruner, J. (1996) The Culture of eduction. Harvard University Press. [SB]Google Scholar
Burling, R. (1993) Primate calls, human language, and nonverbal communication. Current Anthropology 34:2537. [RB, aDF]Google Scholar
Burnham, D., Kitamura, C. & Vollmer-Conna, U. (2002) What's new, pussycat? On talking to babies and animals. Science 296:1435. [aDF]Google Scholar
Butterworth, G. (1997) Starting point. Natural History 5:1416. [aDF]Google Scholar
Byrne, R. W. (1998) Imitation: The contribution of priming and program-level copying. In: Intersubjective communication and emotion in early ontogeny, ed. Braten, S.. Cambridge University Press. [SB]Google Scholar
Byrne, R. W. & Russon, A. E. (1998) Learning by imitation: A hierarchical approach. Behavioral and Brain Sciences 21:667–90. [CS]Google Scholar
Callaghan, C. S. (1994) Comment on Armstrong, Stoko and Wilcox. Current Anthropology 35:359–60. [aDF, CS]Google Scholar
Cantalupo, C. & Hopkins, W. D. (2001) Asymmetric Broca's area in great apes. Nature 414:505. [rDF]Google Scholar
Caro, T. M. & Hauser, M. D. (1992) Is there teaching in nonhuman animals? Quarterly Review of Biology 67:151–74. [JLL]Google Scholar
Carton, J. S., Kessler, E. A. & Pape, C. L. (1999) Nonverbal decoding skills and relationship well-being in adults. Journal of Nonverbal Behavior 23(1):91100. [MM]Google Scholar
Chaminade, T., Meltzoff, A. N. & Decety, J. (2002) Does the end justify the means? A PET exploration of the mechanisms involved in human imitation. NeuroImage 15:318–28. [FA]Google Scholar
Cheney, D. L. & Seyfarth, R. M. (1990) How monkeys see the world. University of Chicago Press. [aDF]Google Scholar
Chomsky, N. (1972) Language and Mind (extended edition). Harcourt, Brace & Jovanovich. [CS, SW]Google Scholar
Chomsky, N. (1995) The minimalist program. MIT Press. [CS]Google Scholar
Cohn, J. F. & Tronick, E. (1987) The sequence of dyadic states at 3, 6, and 9 months. Developmental Psychology 23:6877. [ED]Google Scholar
Colombo, J., Frick, J. E. & Ryther, J. S. (1995) Infant's detection of analogs of ‘motherese’ in noise. Merrill-Palmer Quarterly 41(1):104–13. [MM]Google Scholar
Condon, W. S. & Sander, L. W. (1974) Neonate movement is synchronized with adult speech. Integrated participation and language acquisition. Science 183:99101. [SJC]Google Scholar
Cooper, R. P., Abraham, J., Berman, S. & Staska, M. (1997) The development of infants’ preference for motherese. Infant Behavior and Development 20:477–88. [aDF, MM, CS]Google Scholar
Cooper, R. P. & Aslin, R. N. (1989) “The language environment of the young infant: Implications for early perceptual development.” Canadian Journal of Psychology 43:247–65. [MM]Google Scholar
Corballis, M. C. (1992) On the evolution of language and generativity. Cognition 44:226. [AR]Google Scholar
Corballis, M. C. (1999) The gestural origins of language. American Scientist 87:3845. [aDF]Google Scholar
Corballis, M. C. (2002) From hand to mouth: The origins of language. Princeton University Press. [aDF, BJK]Google Scholar
Cowley, S. J. (1998) Of timing, turn-taking and conversations. Journal of Psycholinguistic Research 27:541–71. [SJC]Google Scholar
Cowley, S. J., Moodley, S. & Fiori-Cowley, A. (2004) Grounding signs of culture: Primary intersubjectivity in social semiosis. Mind, Culture and Activity 11(2):109–32. [SJC]Google Scholar
Crawford, C. (2002) Musings on the concept of exaptation and “creationism.” Behavioral and Brain Sciences 25:511–12. [DB]Google Scholar
Crosby, E. C., Humphrey, T. & Lauer, E. (1962) Correlative Anatomy of the Nervous System. Macmillan. [rDF]Google Scholar
Crystal, D. (1975) The English tone of voice. St. Martin's Press. [MM]Google Scholar
Darwin, C. (1859) On the origin of species by means of natural selection, or the preservation of the favoured races in the struggle for life. John Murray. [CLB]Google Scholar
Davidson, I. & Noble, W. (1989) The archaeology of perception: Traces of depiction and language. Current Anthropology 30:125–55. [aDF]Google Scholar
Davis, B. L & MacNeilage, P. F. (2000) The internal structure of the syllable: An ontogenetic perspective on origins. In: From pre-language to language, ed. Givon, T. & Malle, B.. John Benjamins. [PFM]Google Scholar
Deacon, T. W. (1989) The neural circuitry underlying primate calls and human language. Human Evolution 4:367401. [EG]Google Scholar
de Boysson-Bardies, B. (1999) How language comes to children. MIT Press. [SB]Google Scholar
de Heinzelien, J., Desmond Clark, J., White, T., Hart, W., Renne, P., WoldeGabriel, G., Beyene, Y. & Vrba, E. (1999) Environment and behavior of 2.5 million year old Bouri hominids Science 284:625–29. [AF]Google Scholar
Demuth, K. (1996) The prosodic structure of early words. In: Signal to syntax: Bootstrapping from speech to grammar in early acquisition, ed. Morgan, J. L. & Demuth, K.. Erlbaum. [aDF]Google Scholar
Dennett, D. (1987) The intentional stance. MIT Press. [SJC]Google Scholar
de Waal, F. (1997) Bonobos: The forgotten ape. University of California Press. [aDF]Google Scholar
de Waal, F. (1997) The symbolic species: The co-evolution of language and the human brain. Norton. [CLB, PB, SJC]Google Scholar
Di Pellegrino, G., Fadiga, L., Fogassi, L., Gallese, V. & Rizzolatti, G. (1992) Understanding motor events: A neurophysiological study. Experimental Brain Research 91:176–80. [SB, AR]Google Scholar
Dissanayake, E. (2000) Antecedents of the temporal arts in early mother-infant interaction. In: The origins of music, ed. Wallin, N. L., Merker, B. & Brown, S., pp. 389410. MIT Press. [ED, aDF]Google Scholar
Dissanayake, E. (2001) An ethological view of music and its relevance to music therapy. Nordic Journal of Music Therapy 10(2):159–75. [ED]Google Scholar
Donald, M. (1993) Précis of Origins of the modern mind. Behavioral and Brain Sciences 16:737–91. [aDF]Google Scholar
Dooling, D. J. (1974) Rhythm and syntax in sentence perception. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior 13:255–64. [aDF]Google Scholar
Drayna, D., Manichaikul, A., Lange, M. D., Sneider, H. & Spector, T. (2001) Genetic correlates of musical pitch recognition in humans. Science 291:1969–72. [aDF]Google Scholar
Draper, P. (1976) Social and economic constraints on child life among the !Kung. In: Kalahari hunter-gatherers: Studies of the !Kung San and their neighbors, ed. Lee, R. B. & DeVore, I.. Harvard University Press. [JLL]Google Scholar
Dunbar, R. (1997) Grooming, gossip and the evolution of language. Harvard University Press. [PB]Google Scholar
Dunbar, R. I. M. (1993) Coevolution of neocortical size, group size and language in humans. Behavioral and Brain Sciences 16:681735. [arDF]Google Scholar
Dunbar, R. I. M. (1996) Grooming, gossip and the evolution of language. Faber. [SJC]Google Scholar
Duncan, S. D. (2002) Gesture, verb aspect, and the nature of iconic imagery in natural discourse. Gesture 2(2):183206. [SW]Google Scholar
Eckerman, C. O., Oehler, J. M., Hannan, T. E. & Molitor, A. (1995) The development prior to term age of very prematurely born newborns responsiveness in en face exchanges. Infant Behavior and Development 18(3):283–97. [MM]Google Scholar
Eibl-Eibesfeldt, I. (1989) Human ethology. Aldine de Gruyter. [ED]Google Scholar
Ekman, P. (1972) Universals and cultural differences in facial expressions of emotion. In: Nebraska symposium on motivation, ed. Cole, J.. University of Nebraska Press. [SJC, DS]Google Scholar
Ekman, P. (1979) About brows: Emotional and conversational signals. In: Human ethology: Claims and limits of a new discipline, ed. von Cranach, M., Foppa, K., Lepenies, W. & Ploog, D.. Cambridge University Press. [aDF]Google Scholar
Elowson, A. M., Snowdon, C. T. & Lazaro-Perea, C. (1998) Infant “babbling” in a nonhuman primate: Complex vocal sequences with repeated call types. Behaviour 135:643–64. [JLL]Google Scholar
Enard, W., Przeworski, M., Fisher, S. E., Lai, C. S. L., Wiebe, V., Kitano, T., Monaco, A. P. & Paabo, S. (2002) Molecular evolution of FOXP2, a gene involved in speech and language. Nature 418:869–72. [aDF]Google Scholar
Falk, D. (1990) Brain evolution in Homo: The “radiator” theory. Behavioral and Brain Sciences 13:333–81. [CLB, arDF]Google Scholar
Falk, D. (1992a) Braindance. Henry Holt. [aDF]Google Scholar
Falk, D. (1992b) Evolution of the brain and cognition in hominins. The 62nd James Arthur Lecture. American Museum of Natural History. [aDF]Google Scholar
Falk, D. (1997) Brain evolution in females: An answer to Mr. Lovejoy. In: Women in human evolution, ed. Hager, L. D.. Routledge. [rDF]Google Scholar
Falk, D. (1998) Hominin brain evolution: Looks can be deceiving. Science 280:1714. [aDF]Google Scholar
Falk, D. (2000a) Hominid brain evolution and the origins of music. In: The origins of music, ed. Wallin, N. L., Merker, B. & Brown, S.. MIT Press. [rDF]Google Scholar
Falk, D. (2000b) Primate diversity. W. W. Norton. [arDF]Google Scholar
Falk, D. (2001) The evolution of sex differences in primate brains. In: Evolutionary anatomy of the primate cerebral cortex, ed. Falk, D. & Gibson, K. R.. Cambridge University Press. [rDF]Google Scholar
Falk, D. (2004a) Braindance, revised and expanded. University Press of Florida. [rDF]Google Scholar
Falk, D. (2004b) Prelinguistic evolution in hominin mothers and babies: For cryin’ out loud! (Commentary on target article by Joseph Soltis). Behavioral and Brain Sciences 27(4):443–90. [rDF]Google Scholar
Falk, D. & Dudek, B. (1993) Mosaic evolution of the neocortex. Behavioral and Brain Sciences 16:701702. [rDF]Google Scholar
Falk, D., Froese, N., Sade, D. S. & Dudek, B. C. (1999) Sex differences in brain/ body relationships of rhesus monkeys and humans. Journal of Human Evolution 36:233–38. [rDF]Google Scholar
Falk, D., Redmond, J. C. Jr., Guyer, J., Conroy, G. G., Recheis, W., Weber, G. W. & Seidler, H. (2000) Early hominin brain evolution: A new look at old endocasts. Journal of Human Evolution 38:695717. [arDF]Google Scholar
Fee, J. E. (1997) The prosodic framework for language learning. Topics in Language Disorders 17:5362. [aDF]Google Scholar
Ferguson, C. A. (1964) Baby talk in six languages. In: American Anthropologist: Transcultural Studies in Cognition, vol. 66, ed. Romney, A. K. & D’Andrade, R. G., pp. 103–14. American Anthropological Association. [HB, PFM, MM]Google Scholar
Ferguson, C. A. (1977) Baby talk as a simplified register. In: Talking to children: Language input and acquisition, ed. Snow, C. & Ferguson, C., pp. 219–36. Cambridge University Press. [aDF, MM]Google Scholar
Fernald, A. (1984) The perceptual and affective salience of mothers’ speech to infants. In: The origins and growth of communication, ed. Feagans, L., Garvey, C. & Golinkoff, R.. Ablex. [HB]Google Scholar
Fernald, A. (1991) Prosody in speech to children: Prelinguistic and linguistic functions. Kingsley. [MM]Google Scholar
Fernald, A. (1992a) Human maternal vocalizations to infants as biologically relevant signals: An evolutionary perspective. In: The adapted mind, ed. Barkow, J. H., Cosmides, L. & Tooby, J., pp. 391428. Oxford University Press. [MM, DS]Google Scholar
Fernald, A. (1992b) Maternal vocalizations to infants as biologically relevant signals: An evolutionary perspective. In: The adapted mind, ed. Barkow, J. H., Cosmides, L. & Tooby, J.. Oxford University Press. [DS]Google Scholar
Fernald, A. (1993) Human maternal vocalizations to infants as biologically relevant signals. In: Language acquisition, ed. Bloom, P.. Harvester Wheatsheaf. [SJC]Google Scholar
Fernald, A. (1994) Human maternal vocalizations to infants as biologically relevant signals: An evolutionary perspective. In: Language acquisition core readings, ed. Bloom, P.. MIT Press. [RB, aDF]Google Scholar
Fernald, A. & Mazzie, C. (1991) Prosody and focus in speech to infants and adults. Developmental Psychology 27:209–21. [MM]Google Scholar
Fernald, A., Taeschner, T., Dunn, J., Papousek, M., Boysson-Bardies, B. & Fukui, I. (1989) A cross-language study of prosodic modifications in mothers’ and fathers’ speech to preverbal infants. Journal of Child Language 16:477501. [MM]Google Scholar
Fitch, W. T. (2000) The evolution of speech: A comparative review. Trends in Cognitive Science 4:258–67. [aDF]Google Scholar
Floel, A., Ellger, T., Breitenstein, C. & Knecht, S. (2003) Language perception activates the hand motor cortex: Implications for motor theories of speech perception. European Journal of Neuroscience 18:704708. [AR]Google Scholar
Fogel, A. (1993) Developing through relationships: Origins of communication, self, and culture. Harvester Wheatsheaf/University of Chicago Press. [SJC, aDF]Google Scholar
Foley, R. A. (1995) Language and thought in evolutionary perspective. In: Interpreting archaeology: Finding meaning in the past, ed. Hodder, I., Shanks, M., Alexandri, A., Buchli, V., Carman, J., Last, J., Lucas, G., pp. 7680. Routledge. [MM]Google Scholar
Forbes, E., Cohn, J. F., Lewinsohn, P. & Moore, G. A. (2000) Mother-father differences in parent and infant affect. Paper presented at the International Conference on Infant Studies, Brighton, England (cited in Schmidt & Cohn 2001). [aDF]Google Scholar
Fouts, R., Fouts, D. & van Cantfort, T. E. (1989) The infant Loulis learns signs from cross-fostered chimpanzees. In: Teaching sign language in chimpanzees, ed. Gardner, R. A., Gardner, B. T. & van Cantfort, T. E.. State University of New York Press. [aDF]Google Scholar
Frick, R. W. (1985) Communicating emotion: The role of prosodic features. Psychological Bulletin 97(3):412–29. [MM]Google Scholar
Frodi, A. (1985) When empathy fails. In: Infant crying: Theoretical and research perspectives, ed. Lester, B. M. & Boukydis, C. F. Z.. Plenum. [aDF]Google Scholar
Fuentes, A. (1994) Socioecology of the Mentawai Island langur (Presbytis potenziani). Unpublished Doctoral Dissertation, University of California, Berkeley. [AF]Google Scholar
Fuentes, A. (2000) Hylobatid communities: Changing views on pair bonding and social organization in hominoids. Yearbook of Physical Anthropology 43:3360. [AF]Google Scholar
Fuentes, A. & Tenaza, R. R. (1995) Infant parking in pig-tailed langurs (Simias concolor) Folio Primatologia 65:172–73. [AF, aDF]Google Scholar
Fulton, J. (1941) Encephalization of motor functions during the evolution of the primate nervous system. Ohio Journal of Science 41:173–82. [MM]Google Scholar
Gabunia, L., Anton, S. C., Lordkipanidze, D., Vekua, A., Justus, A. & Swisher, C. C. (2001) Dmanisi and dispersal. Evolutionary Anthropology 10(5):158–70. [AF]Google Scholar
Galaburda, A. M. & Panda, D. N. (1982) Roles of architectonics and connections in the study of primate evolution. In: Primate brain evolution: Methods and concepts, ed. Armstrong, E. & Falk, D., pp. 203–16. Plenum Press. [AR]Google Scholar
Gallese, V., Fadiga, L., Fogassi, L. & Rizzolatti, G. (1996) Action recognition in the premotor cortex. Brain 119(Pt 2):593609. [AR]Google Scholar
Gannon, P. J., Holloway, R. L., Broadfield, D. C. & Braun, A. R. (1998) Asymmetry of chimpanzee planum temporale: Humanlike pattern of Wernicke's brain language area homolog. Science 279:220–22. [rDF]Google Scholar
Geschwind, N. (1965) Disconnexion syndromes in animals and man, Part I. Brain 88:237–94. [EG]Google Scholar
Geschwind, N. (1970) The organization of language and the brain. Science 170:940–44. [EG]Google Scholar
Gibbons, A. (1992) Chimps: More diverse than a barrel of monkeys. Science 255:287–88. [aDF]Google Scholar
Gibson, K. R. (1986) Cognition, brain size and the extraction of embedded food resources. In: Primate ontogeny, cognition and social behaviour, ed. Else, J. D. & Lee, P. C.. Cambridge University Press. [aDF]Google Scholar
Gibson, K. R. (1990) New perspectives on instincts and intelligence: Brain size and the emergence of hierarchical mental construction skills. In: “Language” and intelligence in monkeys and apes, ed. Parker, S. T. & Gibson, K. R.. Cambridge University Press. [aDF]Google Scholar
Gibson, K. R. (1995) Solving the language origins puzzle: Collecting and assembling all pertinent pieces. Behavioral and Brain Sciences 18:189. [EG]Google Scholar
Gibson, K. R. (2001) Bigger is better: primate brain size in relationship to cognition. In: Evolutionary anatomy of the primate cerebral cortex, ed. Falk, D. & Gibson, K. R.. Cambridge University Press. [aDF]Google Scholar
Gigerenzer, G. & Selten, R., eds. (2001) Bounded rationality: The adaptive toolbox. MIT Press. [PB]Google Scholar
Giles, H., Coupland, J. & Coupland, N. (1991) Contexts of accommodation: Developments in applied sociolinguistics. Cambridge University Press. [SJC]Google Scholar
Givon, T. (1979) On understanding grammar. Academic Press. [aDF]Google Scholar
Gleitman, L. R. & Wanner, E. (1982) Language acquisition: The state of the art. In: Language acquisition: The state of the art, ed. Wanner, E. & Gleitman, L. R.. Cambridge University Press. [aDF]Google Scholar
Gogate, L. J., Bahrick, L. E. & Watson, J. D. (2000) A study of multimodal motherese: The role of temporal synchrony between verbal labels and gestures. Child Development 71:878–94. [aDF, EL, CS]Google Scholar
Goldin-Meadow, S. (1999) The role of communication and thinking. Trends in Cognitive Sciences 3(11):419–29. [DD]Google Scholar
Goldin-Meadow, S. & Mylander, C. (1998) Spontaneous sign systems created by deaf children in two cultures. Nature 391(6664):279–81. [DD]Google Scholar
Goldman, H. I. (2001) Parental reports of “MAMA” sounds in infants: An exploratory study. Journal of Child Language 28:497506. [aDF, PFM]Google Scholar
Goodall, J. (1986) The chimpanzees of Gombe: Patterns of behavior. The Belknap Press of Harvard University Press. [arDF]Google Scholar
Goodman, C. S. & Coughlin, B. C. (2000) The evolution of evo-devo biology. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences USA 97:4424–25. [rDF]Google Scholar
Greenspan, S. & Shanker, S. (2004) The first idea. Perseus. [BJK]Google Scholar
Gumperz, J. J. (1996) The linguistic and cultural relativity of inference. In: Rethinking linguistic relativity, ed. Gumperz, J. J. & Levinson, S.. Cambridge University Press. [SJC]Google Scholar
Haig, D. (1993) Genetic conflicts in human pregnancy. Quarterly Review of Biology 68:495531. [DS]Google Scholar
Haiman, J. (1998a) Talk is cheap: Sarcasm, alienation, and the evolution of language. Oxford University Press. [SW]Google Scholar
Haiman, J. (1998b) The metalinguistics of ordinary language. Evolution of Communication 2(1):117–35. [SW]Google Scholar
Hall, J. A. (1984) Nonverbal sex differences: Communication accuracy and expressive style. Johns Hopkins University Press. [aDF]Google Scholar
Halverson, H. M. (1937a) Studies of the grasping responses of early infancy: I. Journal of Genetic Psychology 51:371–92. [aDF]Google Scholar
Halverson, H. M. (1937b) Studies of the grasping responses of early infancy: II. Clinging strength. Journal of Genetic Psychology 51:393424. [aDF]Google Scholar
Halverson, H. M. (1937c) Studies of the grasping responses of early infancy: III. Relation of activity and posture to grasping. Journal of Genetic Psychology 51:425–49. [aDF]Google Scholar
Hamzei, F., Rijntjes, M., Dettmers, C., Glauche, V., Weiller, C. & Buchel, C. (2003) The human action recognition system and its relationship to Broca's area: An fMRI study. Neuroimage 19:637–44. [EG]Google Scholar
Harlow, H. F. (1936) The neuro-physiological correlates of learning and intelligence. Psychological Bulletin 33:479525. [MM]Google Scholar
Harlow, H. F., Harlow, M. K. & Hansen, E. W. (1963) The maternal affectional system of rhesus monkeys. In: Maternal behavior in mammals, ed. Rheingold, H. L.. Wiley. [JLL]Google Scholar
Harnad, S. (1996a) Experimental analysis of naming behavior cannot explain naming capacity. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior 65:262–64. [aDF]Google Scholar
Harnad, S. (1996b) The origin of words: A psychophysical hypothesis. In: Communicating meaning, The evolution and development of language, ed. Velichkovsky, B. M. & Rumbaugh, D. M.. Erlbaum. [aDF]Google Scholar
Hartwig-Scherer, S. (1993) Body weight prediction in early fossil hominids: Towards a taxon-”independent” approach. American Journal of Physical Anthropology 92:1736. [CLB]Google Scholar
Hastie, H. W., Poesio, M. & Isard, S. (2002) Automatically predicting dialogue structure using prosodic features. Speech Communication 36:6379. [aDF]Google Scholar
Hatfield, E., Cacioppo, J. T. & Rapson, R. L. (1994) Emotional contagion. Cambridge University Press. [SJC]Google Scholar
Hauser, M. D., Chomsky, N. & Fitch, W. T. (2002) The faculty of language: What is it, who has it, and how did it evolve? Science 298:1569–79. [SJC, CS]Google Scholar
Hay, R. L. (1976) Environmental setting of hominid activities in Bed I, Olduvai Gorge. In: Human origins, ed. Isaac, G. L. & McCown, E. R.. W. A. Benjamin. [CLB]Google Scholar
Heine, B. & Kuteva, T. (2002) On the evolution of grammatical forms. In: The transition to language, ed. Wray, A.. Oxford University Press. [aDF]Google Scholar
Hewes, G. W. (1973) Primate communication and the gestural origin of language. Current Anthropology 14:532. [aDF, AR]Google Scholar
Hill, K. & Hurtado, A. M. (1996) Ache life history: The ecology and demography of a foraging people. Aldine de Gruyter. [JLL]Google Scholar
Hirsh-Pasek, K. & Golinkoff, R. M. (1996) The origins of grammar: Evidence from early language comprehension. MIT Press. [aDF]Google Scholar
Hirsh-Pasek, K., Nelson, D. G. K., Jusczyk, P. W., Cassidy, K. W., Druss, B. & Kennedy, L. (1987) Clauses are perceptual units for young infants. Cognition 26:269–86. [aDF]Google Scholar
Hobson, P. (1998) The intersubjective foundations of thought. In: Intersubjective communication and emotion in early ontogeny, ed. Braten, S.. Cambridge University Press. [SB]Google Scholar
Hopkins, W. D., Marino, L., Rilling, J. K. & MacGregor, L. A. (1998) Planum temporale asymmetries in great apes as revealed by magnetic resonance imaging (MRI). NeuroReport 9:2913–18. [rDF]Google Scholar
Horne, P. J. & Lowe, F. (1996) On the origins of naming and other symbolic behavior. Journal of Experimental Analysis of Behavior 65:185241. [aDF]Google Scholar
Hurford, J. R. (2002) The roles of expression and representation in language evolution. In: The transition to language, ed. Wray, A.. Oxford University Press. [aDF]Google Scholar
Iacoboni, M., Woods, R. P., Brass, M., Bekkering, H., Mazziota, J. C. & Rizzolatti, G. (1999) Cortical mechanisms of human imitation. Science 286:2526–28. [FA]Google Scholar
Isaac, Glynn L. (1978) Food sharing and human evolution: Archaeological evidence from the Plio-Pleistocene of East Africa. Journal of Anthropological Research 34(5):311–25. [KRR]Google Scholar
Iverson, J. M., Capirci, O., Longobardi, E. & Caselli, M. C. (1999) Gesturing in mother-child interactions. Cognitive Development 14:5775. [aDF]Google Scholar
Jaffe, J., Beebe, B., Feldstein, S., Crown, C. L. & Jasnow, M. D. (2001) Rhythms of dialogue in infancy. Blackwell. [ED]Google Scholar
Jakobson, R. (1960) Why “Mama” and “Papa”? In: Essays in honor of Heinz Werner, ed. Caplan, B. & Wapner, S.. International Universities Press. [PFM]Google Scholar
Janzen, T. & Shaffer, B. (2002) Gesture as the substrate in the process of ASL grammaticization. In: Modality and structure in signed and spoken languages, ed. Meier, R., Quinto, D. & Cormier, K., pp. 199223. Cambridge University Press. [SW]Google Scholar
Jerison, H. (1977) Evolution of the brain. In: The human brain, authored by Wittrock, M. C., Beatty, J., Bogen, J. E., Gazzaniga, M. S., Jerison, H. J., Krashen, S. D., Nebes, R. D. & Teyler, T. J.. Prentice Hall. [EG]Google Scholar
Jerison, H. (1985) Animal intelligence as encephalization. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society of London, Series B: Biological Sciences 1135:2135. [MM]Google Scholar
Jerison, H. (1986) The perceptual worlds of dolphins. In: Dolphin cognition and behavior: A comparative approach, ed. Schusterman, R. J., Thoman, J. A. & Wood, F. G.. Erlbaum. [EG]Google Scholar
Jerison, H. (1988) Evolutionary neurology and the origin of language as a cognitive adaptation. In: The genesis of language: A different judgement of evidence, ed. Landsberg, M. E.. Mouton de Gruyter. [EG]Google Scholar
Jerison, H. (1991) Brain size and the evolution of mind. American Museum of Natural History. [EG]Google Scholar
Jones, N. B., ed. (1972) Ethological studies of child behavior. Cambridge University Press. [RS]Google Scholar
Jouanjean-L’Antoune, A. (1997) Reciprocal interaction and the development of communication and language between parents and children. In: Social influence on vocal development, ed. Snowdon, C. T. & Husberger, M.. MIT Press. [CS]Google Scholar
Jürgens, U. (1979) Neural control of vocalization in nonhuman primates. In: Neurobiology of social communication in primates, ed. Steklis, H., Raleigh, M. & M. Academic Press. [MM]Google Scholar
Jürgens, U. (1988) Central control of monkey calls. In: Primate vocal communication, ed. Todt, D., Goedeking, P. & Symmes, D.. Springer Verlag. [EG]Google Scholar
Jusczyk, P. & Aslin, R. (1995) Infants’ detection of the sound patterns of words in fluent speech. Cognitive Psychology 29:123. [HB]Google Scholar
Jusczyk, P. W. & Hohne, E. A. (1997) Infants’ memory for spoken words. Science 277:1984–86. [AR]Google Scholar
Kano, T. (1992) The last ape: Pygmy chimpanzee behavior and ecology. Stanford University Press. [aDF]Google Scholar
Karmiloff, K. & Karmiloff-Smith, A. (2001) Pathways to language: From fetus to adolescent. Harvard University Press. [aDF]Google Scholar
Karssemeijer, G. J., Vos, D. R. & van Hooff, J. A. R. A. M. (1990) The effect of some nonsocial factors on mother-infant contact in long-tailed macaques (Macaca fascicularis). Behaviour 113:273–91. [aDF]Google Scholar
Kawai, M. (1965) Newly-acquired pre-cultural behavior of the natural troop of Japanese monkeys on Koshima islet. Primates 6:130. [aDF, JLL, RS]Google Scholar
Keenan, J. P., Gallup, G. G. Jr. & Falk, D. (2003) The face in the mirror: The search for the origins of human consciousness. Ecco/Harper Collins. [rDF]Google Scholar
Kempe, V. & Brooks, P. (2001) The role of diminutives in the acquisition of Russian gender: Can elements of child-directed speech aid in learning morphology? Language Learning 51:221–56. [aDF]Google Scholar
Kendon, A. (1972) Some relationships between body motion and speech. In: Studies in dyadic communication, ed. Sigman, A. & Pope, B., pp. 77–21. Pergamon Press. [SW]Google Scholar
Kendon, A. (1980) Gesticulation and speech: Two aspects of the process of utterance. In: The relation between verbal and nonverbal communication, ed. Kay, M. R., pp. 206–27. Mouton. [SW]Google Scholar
Kenney, M. D., Mason, W. A. & Hill, S. D. (1979) Effects of age, objects, and visual experience on affective responses of rhesus monkeys to strangers. Developmental Psychology 15:176–84. [JLL]Google Scholar
Kent, R. D. & Read, C. (1992) The acoustic analysis of speech. Singular. [MM]Google Scholar
Kimura, M. & Ohta, T. (1969) The average number of generations until fixation of a mutant gene in a finite population. Genetics 61:763–71. [CLB]Google Scholar
King, B. J. (1996) Syntax and language origins. Language and Communication 16:193203. [LC, aDF]Google Scholar
King, B. J. (2002) On patterned interactions and culture in great apes. In: Anthropology beyond culture, ed. Fox, R. G. & King, B. J.. Berg. [BJK]Google Scholar
King, B. J. (2004) The dynamic dance: Nonvocal social communication in the African great apes. Harvard University Press. [BJK]Google Scholar
King, B. J. & Shanker, S. (2003) How can we know the dancer from the dance? The dynamic nature of African great ape social communication. Anthropological Theory 3(1):526. [BJK]Google Scholar
Kittler, R., Kayser, M. & Stoneking, M. (2003) Molecular evolution of Pediculus humanus and the origin of clothing. Current Biology 13(16):1414–17. [rDF, MM]Google Scholar
Klein, Richard G. (1999) The human career: Human biological and cultural origins, 2nd edition. University of Chicago Press. [RS]Google Scholar
Knight, C. (1998a) Introduction: Grounding language function in social cognition. In: Approaches to the evolution of language, ed. Hurford, J. R., Studdert-Kennedy, M. & Knight, C.. Cambridge University Press. [LC]Google Scholar
Knight, C. (1998b) Ritual/speech coevolution: A solution to the problem of deception. In: Approaches to the evolution of language, ed. Hurford, J. R., Studdert-Kennedy, M. & Knight, C.. Cambridge University Press. [LC]Google Scholar
Knight, C., Power, C. & Watts, I. (1995) The human symbolic revolution: A Darwinian account. Cambridge Archaeological Journal 5:75114. [LC]Google Scholar
Konner, M. J. (1976) Maternal care, infant behavior and development among the !Kung. In: Kalahari hunter-gatherers: Studies of the !Kung San and their neighbors, ed. Lee, R. B. & DeVore, I.. Harvard University Press. [JLL]Google Scholar
Koulomzin, M., Beebe, B., Anderson, S., Jaffe, J., Feldstein, S. & Crown, C. (2002) Infant gaze, head, face and self-touch at 4 months differentiate secure vs. avoidant attachment at 1 year: A microanalytic approach. Attachment and Human Development 4(1):324. [ED]Google Scholar
Kugiumutzakis, G. (1993) Intersubjective vocal imitation in early mother-infant interaction. In: New perspectives in early communicative development, ed. Nadel, J. & Camaioni, L.. Routledge. [SB]Google Scholar
Kugiumutzakis, G. (1998) Neonatal imitation in the intersubjective companion space. In: Intersubjective communication and emotion in early ontogeny, ed. Braten, S.. Cambridge University Press. [SB]Google Scholar
Kuhl, P. K. (1998) Language, culture and intersubjectivity. In: Intersubjective communication and emotion in early ontogeny, ed. Braten, S.. Cambridge University Press. [SB]Google Scholar
Kuhl, P. K., Andruski, J. E., Chistovich, I. A., Chistovich, L. A., Kozhevnikova, E. V., Ryskina, V. L., Stoljarova, E. I., Sundberg, U. & Lacerda, F. (1997) Cross-language analysis of phonetic units in language addressed to infants. Science 277:684–86. [aDF]Google Scholar
Kuhl, P. K. & Meltzoff, A. N. (1988) Speech as an intermodal object of perception. In: Perceptual development in infancy: The Minnesota symposium on child phonology, ed. Yonas, A.. Erlbaum. [aDF]Google Scholar
Kuroda, S. (1984) Rocking gesture as communicative behavior in the wild pygmy chimpanzees in Wambe, Central Zaire. Journal of Ethology 2:127–37. [BJK]Google Scholar
Lai, C. S. L., Fisher, S. E., Hurst, J. A., Vargha-Khadem, F. & Monaco, A. P. (2001) A forkhead-doamin gene is mutated in a severe speech and language disorder. Nature 413:519–23. [aDF]Google Scholar
Laland, K. N., Odling-Smee, J. & Feldman, M. W. (2000) Niche construction, biological evolution and cultural change. Behavioral and Brain Sciences 23:131–75. [SJC]Google Scholar
Lamb, M. E., Thompson, R. A., Gardner, W. & Charnov, E. L. (1985) Infantmother attachment: The origins and developmental significance of individual differences in strange situation behavior. Erlbaum. [aDF]Google Scholar
Leakey, R. (1995) The origin of humankind. Phoenix. [SB]Google Scholar
Leavens, D. A. & Hopkins, W. D. (1998) Intentional communication by chimpanzees: A cross-sectional study of the use of referential gestures. Developmental Psychology 34:813–22. [DS]Google Scholar
Lee, R. B. (1979) The !Kung San: men, women, and work in a foraging society. Cambridge University Press. [JLL]Google Scholar
Legerstee, M. (1990) Infant use of multimodal information to imitate speech sounds. Infant Behavior and Development 13:343–54. [aDF]Google Scholar
Leutenegger, W. (1972) Newborn size and pelvic dimensions of Australopithecus. Nature 240:568–69. [aDF]Google Scholar
LeVine, R. & LeVine, B. (1966) Nyansongo: a Gusii community in Kenya. Wiley. [JLL]Google Scholar
LeVine, R. A. (1980) A cross-cultural perspective on parenting. In: Parenting in a multicultural society, ed. Fantini, M. D. & Cárdenas, R.. Longman. [JLL]Google Scholar
Levitt, A. G. (1993) The acquisition of prosody: Evidence from French- and English-learning infants. In: Development neurocognition: Speech and face processing in the first year of life, ed. de Boysson-Bardies, B., de Schonen, S., Jusczyk, P., McNeilage, P. & Morton, J.. Kluwer. [aDF]Google Scholar
Lewis, L. B., Antone, C. & Johnson, J. S. (1999) Effects of prosodic stress and serial position on syllable omission in first words. Developmental Psychology 35:4559. [aDF]Google Scholar
Liberman, A. M. (1957) Some results of research on speech perception. Journal of the Acoustical Society of America 29:117–23. [SB]Google Scholar
Liberman, A. M. (1993) Haskin Laboratories Status Report on Speech Research 113:132. [SB]Google Scholar
Liberman, A. M. (1996) Speech: A special code. MIT Press. [CS]Google Scholar
Liberman, A. M. & Mattingly, I. G. (1985) The motor theory of speech perception revised. Cognition 21:136. [AR]Google Scholar
Lieberman, P. (1979) Hominid evolution, supralaryngeal vocal tract physiology, and the fossil evidence for reconstructions. Brain Language 7:101–26. [AR]Google Scholar
Locke, J. L. (1983) Phonological acquisition and change. Academic Press. [JLL]Google Scholar
Locke, J. L. (1993) The child's path to spoken language. Harvard University Press. [SB, JLL]Google Scholar
Locke, J. L. (2001) First communion: The emergence of vocal relationships. Social Development 10:294308. [JLL]Google Scholar
Locke, J. L. (2001) (submitted) Parental selection of vocal behaviors in the evolution of spoken language. [JLL]Google Scholar
Longhi, E. (2003) The temporal structure of mother-infant interactions in musical contexts. Doctoral dissertation, The University Of Edinburgh. [EL]Google Scholar
Lorberbaum, J. P., Newman, J. D., Horwitz, A. R., Dubno, J. R., Lydiard, R. B., Hamner, M. B., Bohning, D. E. & George, M. S. (2002) A potential role for thalamocingulate circuitry in human maternal behavior. Biological Psychiatry 51:431–45. [rDF, JDN]Google Scholar
Lovejoy, C. O. (1981) The origin of man. Science 211(4480):341–50. [KRR]Google Scholar
Lyn, H. & Savage-Rumbaugh, S. (2000) Observational word learning in two bonobos (Pan paniscus): Ostensive and non-ostensive contexts. Language and Communication 20:255–73. [aDF]Google Scholar
Lyons, D. M., Kim, S., Schatzberg, A. F. & Levine, S. (1998) Postnatal foraging demands alter adrenocortical activity and psychosocial development. Developmental Psychobiology 32:285–91. [aDF]Google Scholar
Macfarlane, W. V. (1976) Aboriginal palaeophysiology. In: The origin of the Australians, ed. Kirk, R. L. & Thorne, A. G.. Humanities Press. [CLB]Google Scholar
MacKain, K., Stuffert-Kennedy, M., Spieker, M. & Stern, D. (1983) Infant intermodal speech perception is a left-hemisphere function. Science 219:1347–49. [aDF]Google Scholar
MacLean, P. D. (1985) Brain evolution relating to family, play and the separation call. Archives of General Psychiatry 42:405–17. [JDN]Google Scholar
MacLean, P. D. (1990) The triune brain in evolution: Role in paleocerebral functions. Plenum. [JDN]Google Scholar
MacNeilage, P. F. (1998) The frame/content theory of evolution of speech production. Behavioral and Brain Sciences 21:499546. [aDF, JLL, PFM]Google Scholar
MacNeilage, P. F. (2000) The explanation of “mama.” Behavioral and Brain Sciences 23:440–41. [aDF]Google Scholar
MacNeilage, P. F., Davis, B. L., Kinney, A. & Matyear, C. L. (2000) The motor core of speech: A comparison of serial organization patterns in infants and languages. Child Development 71:153–63. [PFM]Google Scholar
McNeill, D. (1992) Hand and mind: What gestures reveal about thought. University of Chicago Press. [SW]Google Scholar
McNeill, D. (2000) Language and gesture. Cambridge University Press. [SW]Google Scholar
Maddieson, I. (1984) Patterns of sounds. Cambridge University Press. [PFM]Google Scholar
Maestripieri, D. & Call, J. (1996) Mother-infant communication in primates. Advances in the Study of Behavior 25:613–42. [arDF, JLL]Google Scholar
Mandel, D. R., Jusczyk, P. W. & Kemler Nelson, D. G. (1994) Does sentential prosody help infants organize and remember speech information? Cognition 53:155–80. [aDF]Google Scholar
Marler, P., Evans, C. S. & Hauser, M. D. (1992) Animal signals: Motivational, referential, or both? In: Nonverbal vocal communication, ed. Papousek, H., Jurgens, U. & Papousek, M.. Cambridge University Press. [aDF]Google Scholar
Masataka, N. (1992) Motherese in a signed language. Infant Behavior and Development 15:453–60. [MM]Google Scholar
Masataka, N. (1998) Perception of motherese in Japanese sign language by 6-month-old hearing infants. Developmental Psychology 34:241–46. [aDF]Google Scholar
Massaro, D. W. (1998) Perceiving talking faces. MIT Press. [aDF]Google Scholar
Matsuzawa, T. (1996) Chimpanzee intelligence in nature and in captivity: Isomorphism of symbol use and tool use. In: Great ape societies, ed. McGrew, W. C., Marchant, L. F. & Nishida, T.. Cambridge University Press. [rDF]Google Scholar
McClure, E. B. (2000) A meta-analytic review of sex differences in facial expression processing and their development in infants, children, and adolescents. Psychological Bulletin 126:424–53. [aDF]Google Scholar
McGrew, W. C. (1992) Chimpanzee material culture: Implications for human evolution. Cambridge University Press. [aDF]Google Scholar
McGrew, W. C. (1998) Culture in nonhuman primates? Annual Review of Anthropology 27:301–28. [CS]Google Scholar
McShane, J. (1979) The development of naming. Linguistics 17:879905. [PFM]Google Scholar
Mead, G. H. (1934) Mind, self, and society. University of Chicago Press. [SB]Google Scholar
Meltzoff, A. N. (1990) Foundations for developing a concept of self: The role of imitation in relating self to other and the value of social mirroring, social modeling, and self practice in infancy. In: The self in transition: Infancy to childhood. ed. Cicchetti, D. & Beeghly, M.. University of Chicago Press. [JLL]Google Scholar
Meltzoff, A. N. (1988) Imitation, objects, tools and the rudiments of language in human ontogeny. Human Evolution 3:4564. [aDF]Google Scholar
Meltzoff, A. N. & Kuhl, P. K. (1994) Faces and speech: Intermodal processing of biological relevant signals in infant and adults. In: The development of intersensory perception: Comparative perspectives, ed. Lewkowick, D. J. & Lickliter, R.. Erlbaum. [CS]Google Scholar
Meltzoff, A. N. & Moore, M. K. (1977) Imitation of facial and manual gestures by human neonates. Science 198:7578. [SB]Google Scholar
Meltzoff, A. N. & Moore, M. K. (1998) Infant intersubjectivity: Broadening the dialogue to include imitation, identity and intention. In: Intersubjective communication and emotion in early ontogeny, ed. Braten, S.. Cambridge University Press. [SB]Google Scholar
Messinger, D. S. & Fogel, A. (1998) Give and take: The development of conventional infant gestures. Merrill-Palmer Quarterly 44:566–90. [aDF]Google Scholar
Meyer, M., Alter, D., Friederici, A., Lohmann, G. & von Cramon, D. (2002) fMRI reveals brain regions mediating slow prosodic modulations in spoken sentences. Human Brain Mapping 17(2):7388. [MM]Google Scholar
Mitani, J. & Brandt, K. (1994) Social factors influence the acoustic variability in the long-distance calls of male chimpanzees. Ethology 96:233–52. [aDF]Google Scholar
Mitani, J., Hasegawa, T., Gros-Louis, J., Marler, P. & Byrne, R. (1992) Dialects in wild chimpanzees? American Journal of Primatology 27:233–43. [aDF]Google Scholar
Mitchell, R., Elliott, R., Barry, M., Cruttenden, A. & Woodruff, P. (2003) The neural response to emotional prosody, as revealed by functional magnetic resonance imaging. Neuropsychologia 41(10):1410–21. [MM]Google Scholar
Monnot, M. (1998) Mother-infant communication and infant health. In: M. Monnot, Biological anthropology, p. 216. University of Cambridge. [MM]Google Scholar
Monnot, M. (1999) Function of infant-directed speech. Human Nature: An Interdisciplinary Biosocial Perspective 10(4):415–43. [aDF, MM]Google Scholar
Monrad-Krohn, G. H. (1963) The third element of speech: Prosody and its disorders. In: Problems in dynamic neurology, ed. Halpern, L.. Hebrew University Press. [MM]Google Scholar
Moo, L. R., Slotnick, S. D., Tesoro, M. A., Zee, D. S. & Hart, J. (2003) Interlocking finger test: A bedside screen for parietal lobe dysfunction. Journal of Neurology, Neurosurgery and Psychiatry 74:530–32. [FA]Google Scholar
Moore, J. (1996) Savanna chimpanzees, referential models and the last common ancestor. In: Great ape societies, ed. McGrew, W. C., Marchant, L. F. & Nishida, T.. Cambridge University Press. [arDF]Google Scholar
Morgan, J. L. (1986) From simple input to complex grammar. MIT Press. [aDF]Google Scholar
Morgan, J. L. & Demuth, K. (1996) Signal to syntax: Bootstrapping from speech to grammar in early acquisition. Erlbaum. [aDF]Google Scholar
Morley, I. (2002) Evolution of the physiological and neurological capacities for music. Cambridge Archaeological Journal 12(2):195216. [ED]Google Scholar
Morton, J. B. & Trehub, S. E. (2001) Children's understanding of emotion in speech. Child Development 72:834–43. [aDF]Google Scholar
Munn, C. A. (1986) The deceptive use of alarm calls by sentinel species in mixedspecies flocks of neotropical birds. In: Deception: Perspectives on human and nonhuman deceit, ed. Mitchell, R. W. & Thompson, N. S.. State University of New York Press. [LC]Google Scholar
Murdock, G. P. (1959) Cross-language parallels in parental kin terms. Anthropological Linguistics 1:15. [PFM]Google Scholar
Murray, L. & Trevarthen, C. (1985) Emotional regulation of interactions between two-month-olds and their mothers. In: Social perception in infants, ed. Field, T. M. & Fox, N. A., pp. 177–98. Ablex. [ED]Google Scholar
Nadel, J. (1996) Early interpersonal timing and the perception of social contingencies. Infant Behavior and Development 19:202. [ED]Google Scholar
Nadel, J., Carchon, I., Kervella, C., Marcelli, D. & Réserbet-Plantey, D. (1999) Expectancies for social contingency in 2-month-olds. Developmental Science 2(2):164–73. [ED]Google Scholar
Nakamichi, M., Kato, E., Kojima, Y. & Itoigawa, N. (1998) Carrying and washing of grass roots by free-ranging Japanese macaques at Katsuyama. Folia Primatologica 69:3540. [aDF]Google Scholar
Newman, J. D. (1985a) Squirrel monkey communication. In: Handbook of squirrel monkey research, ed. Rosenblum, L. A. & Coe, C. L.. Plenum. [JDN]Google Scholar
Newman, J. D. (1985b) The infant cry of primates: An evolutionary perspective. In: Infant crying: Theoretical and research perspectives, ed. Lester, B. M. & Boukydis, C. F. Z.. Plenum. [JDN]Google Scholar
Newman, J. D. (2003a) The primate isolation call: A comparison with precocial birds and nonprimate mammals. In: Comparative vertebrate cognition: Are primates superior to non-primates? ed. Rogers, L. J. and Kaplan, G.. Plenum. [JDN]Google Scholar
Newman, J. D. (2003b) Vocal communication and the triune brain. Physiology and Behavior 79:495502. [JDN]Google Scholar
Newmeyer, F. J. (2002) Uniformitarian assumptions and language evolution research. In: The transition to language, ed. Wray, A.. Oxford University Press. [aDF]Google Scholar
Nicolson, N. A. (1977) A comparison of early behavioral development in wild and captive chimpanzees. In: Primate bio-social development, ed. Chavalier-Skolnikoff, S. & Poirier, F. E.. Garland. [aDF]Google Scholar
Nishida, T. (1968) The social group of wild chimpanzees in the Mahale mountains. Primates 9:167224. [arDF, AF]Google Scholar
Nwokah, E. E., Hsu, H.-C., Davies, P. & Fogel, A. (1999) The integration of laughter and speech in vocal communication: A dynamic systems perspective. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research 42:880–94. [aDF]Google Scholar
O’Connell, J. F., Hawkes, K., Lupo, K. D., and Blurton-Jones, N. G. (2002) Male strategies and Plio-pleistocene archeology. Journal of Human Evolution 43:831–72. [AF]Google Scholar
Owings, D. H. & Morton, E. S. (1998) Animal vocal communication. Cambridge University Press. [SJC]Google Scholar
Owren, M. J., Seyfart, R. M. & Cheney, D. L. (1997) The acoustic features of vowel-like grunt calls in chacma baboons (Papio cynocephalus ursinus): Implications for production processes and functions. Journal of the Acoustical Society of America 101:2951–63. [PB]Google Scholar
Papousek, H. & Papousek, M. (1981.) Musical element in the infant's vocalisation: Their significance for communication, cognition and creativity. In: Advances in infancy research, vol.1, ed. Lipsitt, L. P.. Ablex. [EL]Google Scholar
Papousek, M. & Papousek, H. (1992) Beyond emotional bonding: The role of preverbal communication. Infant Mental Health Journal 13(1):4353. [MM]Google Scholar
Parker, S. T. (1993) Imitation and circular reactions as evolved mechanisms for cognitive construction. Human Development 36:309–23. [aDF]Google Scholar
Parker, S. T. (1996) Apprenticeship in tool-mediated extractive foraging: The origins of imitation, teaching, and self-awareness in great apes. In: Reaching into thought, the minds of the great apes, ed. Russon, A. E., Bard, K. A. & Parker, S. T.. Cambridge University Press. [aDF]Google Scholar
Pawlby, S. J. (1977) Imitative interaction. In: Studies in mother-infant interaction, ed. Schaffer, H. R.. Academic Press. [JLL]Google Scholar
Penfield, W. & Roberts, L. (1959) Speech and brain mechanisms. Princeton University Press. [EG]Google Scholar
Petitto, L. A. (2000) On the biological foundations of human language. In: The signs of language revisited: An anthology in honor of Ursula Bellugi and Edward Klima, ed. Emmorey, K. & Lane, H.. Erlbaum. [aDF]Google Scholar
Piaget, J. (1952) The origins of intelligence in children. Norton. [aDF]Google Scholar
Pinker, S. (1987) The bootstrapping problem in language acquisition. In: Mechanisms of language acquisition, ed. MacWhinney, M.. Erlbaum. [aDF]Google Scholar
Pinker, S. (1994) The language instinct: How the mind creates language. Penguin/William Morrow. [PB, aDF]Google Scholar
Pinker, S. & Bloom, P. (1990) Natural language and natural selection. Behavioral and Brain Sciences 13:707–84. [aDF]Google Scholar
Ploog, D. (1988) Neurobiology and pathology of subhuman vocal communication and human speech. In: Primate vocal communication, ed. Todt, D., Goedeking, P. & Symmes, D.. Springer Verlag. [EG]Google Scholar
Ploog, D. W. (1992) The evolution of vocal communication. In: Nonverbal vocal communication: Comparative and developmental approaches, ed. Papousek, H., Jurgens, U. & Papousek, M.. Cambridge University Press. [MM]Google Scholar
Plooij, F. X. (1984) The behavioral development of free-living chimpanzee babies and infants. Ablex. [arDF]Google Scholar
Potts, R. (1984) Home bases and early hominids. American Scientist 72:338–47. [rDF]Google Scholar
Povinelli, D. J., Reaux, J. E., Theall, L. A. & Giambrone, S. (2000) Folk physics for apes: The chimpanzee's theory of how the world works. Oxford University Press. [CS]Google Scholar
Preston, S. & de Waal, B. M. (2002) Empathy and its proximal bases. Behavioral and Brain Sciences 25:172. [SJC]Google Scholar
Preuschoft, S. (2000) Primate faces and facial expressions. Social Research 67:245–71. [aDF]Google Scholar
Preuschoft, S. & van Hooff, J.A.R.A.M. (1995) Homologizing primate facial displays: A critical review of methods. Folia Primatologica 65:121–37. [aDF]Google Scholar
Preuss, T. M., Stepniewska, I. & Kaas, J. H. (1996) Movement representation in the dorsal and ventral premotor areas of owl monkeys: A microstimulation study. Journal of Comparative Neurology 371:649–76. [AR]Google Scholar
Provine, R. R. (1993) Laughter punctuates speech: Lingistic, social and gender contexts of laughter. Ethology 95:291–98. [aDF]Google Scholar
Provine, R. R. (1996) Laughter. American Scientist 84:3845. [aDF, RRP]Google Scholar
Provine, R. R. (2000) Laughter: A scientific investigation. Viking. [aDF, RRP]Google Scholar
Pye, C. (1986) Kiche Mayan speech to children. Language 59:583604. [KRR]Google Scholar
Ramus, F., Hauser, M. D., Miller, C., Morris, D. & Mehler, J. (2000) Language discrimination by human newborns and by cotton-top tamarin monkeys. Science 288:349–51. [AR]Google Scholar
Redican, W. K. (1975) Facial expressions in nonhuman primates. In: Primate behavior: Developments in field and laboratory research, ed. Rosenblum, L. A.. Academic Press. [JLL]Google Scholar
Richman, B. (1980) Did human speech originate in coordinated vocal music? Semiotica 32:233–44. [JLL]Google Scholar
Rizzolatti, G. & Arbib, M. A. (1998) Language within our grasp. Trends in Neurosciences 21(5):188–94. [SB, DD, arDF, AR]Google Scholar
Rizzolatti, G. & Arbib, M. A. (1999) From grasping to speech: Imitation might provide a missing link: Reply. Trends in Neurosciences 22:152. [DD]Google Scholar
Rizzolatti, G., Fadiga, L., Gallese, G. & Fogassi, L. (1996) Premotor cortex and the recognition of motor actions. Cognitive Brain Research 3:131–41. [DD, rDF, EG, AR]Google Scholar
Rizzolatti, G., Fogassi, L. & Gallese, V. (2002) Motor and cognitive functions of the ventral premotor cortex. Current Opinion in Neurobiology 12:149–54. [DD]Google Scholar
Rogalewski, A., Breitenstein, C., Jansen, A., Floel, A., Foerster, A.-F., Deppe, M., & Knecht, S. (Submitted) From mouth to hand: Melody of speech activates the cortical representation of the right hand. [AR]Google Scholar
Rome-Flanders, T. & Cronk, C. (1995) A longitudinal study of infant vocalizations during mother-infant games. Journal of Child Language 22:259–74. [aDF]Google Scholar
Ross, C. (2001) Park or ride? Evolution of infant carrying in primates. International Journal of Primatology 22:749–71. [AF, arDF]Google Scholar
Ross, E. D. (1982) The divided self. The Sciences 22:812. [MM]Google Scholar
Ross, E. D. (1997) Right hemisphere syndromes and the neurology of emotions. In: Behavioral neurology and the legacy of Norman Geschwind, ed. Schacter, S. C. & Devinsky, O., pp. 183–94. Raven Press. [MM]Google Scholar
Ross, E. D. (2000) Affective prosody and the aprosodias. In: Principles of behavioral and cognitive neurology ed. Mesulam, M. M., pp. 316–31. Oxford University Press. [MM]Google Scholar
Ross, E. D. & Mesulam, M. M. (1979) Dominant language functions of the right hemisphere? Prosody and emotional gesturing. Archives of Neurology 36:144–48. [MM]Google Scholar
Ross, E. D., Thompson, R. D. & Yenkosky, J. P. (1997) Lateralization of affective prosody in brain and the callosal integration of hemispheric language functions. Brain and Language 56:2754. [MM]Google Scholar
Ruff, C. B. & Walker, A. (1993) Body size and body shape. In: The Nariokotome Homo erectus skeleton, ed. Walker, A. & Leakey, R.. Harvard University Press. [CLB]Google Scholar
Savage-Rumbaugh, E. S. (1984) Pan paniscus and Pan troglodytes: Contrasts in preverbal communicative competence. In: The pygmy chimpanzee: Evolutionary biology and behavior, ed. Susman, R. L.. Plenum. [aDF]Google Scholar
Savage-Rumbaugh, S., Shanker, S. G. & Taylor, T. J. (1998) Apes, language, and the human mind. Oxford University Press. [aDF]Google Scholar
Savage-Rambaugh, E. S., Sue, E., Murphy, J., Seveik, R. A., Brakke, K. B., Williams, S. L. & Rambaugh, D. M. (1993) Language comprehension in ape and child. Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development (Serial No. 233) 58, Nos. 3–4. [SB]Google Scholar
Schaller, G. B. (1963) The mountain gorilla. University of Chicago Press. [rDF]Google Scholar
Scherer, K. R. (1974) Acoustic concomitants of emotional dimensions: Judging affect from sythesized tone sequences. In: Non-verbal communication, ed. Weitz, S., pp. 105–11. Oxford University Press. [MM]Google Scholar
Scherer, K. R. & Oshinsky, J. S. (1977) Cue utilization in emotion attributes from auditory stimuli. Motivation and Emotion 1:331–46. [MM]Google Scholar
Schieffelin, B. B. & Ochs, E. (1983) A cultural perspective on the transition from prelinguistic to linguistic communication. In: The transition from prelinguistic to linguistic communication, ed. Golinkoff, R. M.. Erlbaum. [KRR]Google Scholar
Schleidt, W. M. (1973) Tonic communication: Continual effects of discrete signs in animal communication systems. Journal of Theoretical Biology 42:359–86. [JLL]Google Scholar
Schmidt, K. L. & Cohn, J. F. (2001) Human facial expressions as adaptations: Evolutionary questions in facial expression research. Yearbook of Physical Anthropology 44:324. [arDF]Google Scholar
Schore, A. N. (1994) Affect regulation and the origin of the self: The neurobiology of emotional development. Erlbaum. [ED]Google Scholar
Senghas, A. & Coppola, M. (2002) Children creating language: How Nicaraguan sign language acquired a spatial grammar. Psychological Science 12:323–28. [KRR]Google Scholar
Shanker, S. (2001) What children know when they know what a name is. Current Anthropology, 42/4:481513. [SJC]Google Scholar
Shanker, S. & King, B. J. (2002) The emergence of a new paradigm in ape language research. Behavioral and Brain Sciences 25:605–26. [BJK]Google Scholar
Shatz, M. (1982) On mechanisms of language acquisition: Can features of the communicative environment account for the development? In: Language acquisition: The state of the art, ed. Wanner, E. & Gleitman, L.. Cambridge University Press. [aDF]Google Scholar
Shipman, P. (1986) Scavenging or hunting in early hominids: Theoretical framework and tests. American Anthropologist 88:2743. [CLB]Google Scholar
Silk, J. B. (2002) Grunts, girneys, and good intentions: The origins of strategic commitment in nonhuman primates. In: Evolution and the capacity for commitment, ed. Nesse, R.. Russell Sage Press. [PB]Google Scholar
Small, M. F. (1998) Our babies, ourselves. Anchor Books. [arDF]Google Scholar
Smith, B. H. (1992) Life history and the evolution of human maturation. Evolutionary Anthropology 1:134–42. [CLB]Google Scholar
Smith, B. H. (1993) Physiological age of KNM-WT 15000. In: The Nariokotome Homo erectus skeleton, ed. Walker, A. & Leakey, R.. Harvard University Press. [CLB]Google Scholar
Snow, C. E. (1972) Mothers’ speech to children learning language. Child Development 43:549–65. [aDF]Google Scholar
Snow, D. (1998) A prominence account of syllable reduction in early speech development: The child's prosodic phonology of tiger and giraffe. Journal of Speech, Language and Hearing Research 41:1171–84. [aDF]Google Scholar
Snow, D. (2002) Intonation in the monosyllabic utterances of 1-year-olds. Infant Behavior and Development 24:393407. [aDF]Google Scholar
Snowdon, C. T. (1990) Language capacities of nonhuman animals. Yearbook of Physical Anthropology 33:215–43. [aDF]Google Scholar
Sober, E. & Wilson, D. S. (1998) Unto others: The evolution and psychology of unselfish behavior. Harvard University Press. [RS]Google Scholar
Soken, N. H. & Pick, A. D. (1999) Infants’ perception of dynamic affective expressions: Do infants distinguish specific expressions? Child Development 70:1275–82. [aDF]Google Scholar
Sokol, R. I., Webster, K. L., Elfenbein, D., Thompson, N. S. & Stevens, D. A. (submitted) Whining as mother-directed speech. [RS]Google Scholar
Soltis, J. (2004) The signal functions of early infant crying. Behavioral and Brain Sciences 27(4):. [rDF]Google Scholar
Spurrett, D. & Cowley, S. J. (2004) How to do things without words. Language Sciences 26(5):443–66. [SJC]Google Scholar
Stamenov, M. & Gallese, V., eds. (2002) Mirror neurons and the evolution of brain and language. John Benjamins. [SB]Google Scholar
Stanford, C. B. (1998) The social behavior of chimpanzees and bonobos. Current Anthropology 39:399420. [arDF, AF]Google Scholar
Stephan, K. E., Marshall, J. C., Friston, K. J., Rowe, J. B., Ritzl, A., Zilles, K. & Fink, G. R. (2003) Lateralized cognitive processes and lateralized task control in the human brain. Science 301:384–86. [FA]Google Scholar
Stern, D. (1977) The first relationship. Fontana. [SJC, DS]Google Scholar
Stern, D., Hofer, L., Haft, W. & Dore, J. (1985) Affect attunement: The sharing of feeling states between mother and infant by means of inter-modal fluency. In: Social perception in infants, ed. Field, T. M., pp. 249–68. Ablex. [ED]Google Scholar
Stern, D. N. (1985) The interpersonal world of the infant. Basic Books. [SB]Google Scholar
Stern, D. N. (2000) Introduction to the paperback edition. In: The interpersonal world of the infant. Basic Books. [SB]Google Scholar
Stern, D. N., Speiker, S., Barnett, R. K. & MacKain, K. (1983) The prosody of maternal speech: Infant age and context related changes. Journal of Child Language 10:115. [aDF]Google Scholar
Studdert-Kennedy, M. (in press) How did language go discrete? In: Evolutionary prerequisites for language, ed. Tallerman, M.. Oxford University Press. [JLL]Google Scholar
Sutton, D. & Jürgens, U. (1988) Neural control of vocalization. In: Comparative primate biology, vol. 4: Neurosciences. Liss. [EG]Google Scholar
Taglialatela, J. P., Savage-Rumbaugh, S. & Baker, L. A. (2003) Vocal production by a language-competent Pan paniscus. International Journal of Primatology 24:117. [aDF]Google Scholar
Taylor, T. J. (1997) The origin of language: Why it never happened. Language Sciences 19(1):6677. [SJC]Google Scholar
Taylor, T. J. (2000) Language constructing language: The implications of reflexivity for linguistic theory. Language Sciences 22(4):483–99. [SJC]Google Scholar
Thompson, N. S. (2000) Shifting the natural selection metaphor to the group level. Behavior and Philosophy 28:83101. [RS]Google Scholar
Thompson, N. S., Olson, C. & Dessureau, B. (1996) Babies’ cries: Who's listening? Who's being fooled? Social Research 63:3:763–84. [RS]Google Scholar
Thompson, P. M., Cannon, T. D., Narr, K. L., van Erp, T., Poutanen, V.-P., Huttunen, M., Lonnqvist, J., Standertskjold-Nordenstam, C.-G., Kaprio, J., Khaledy, M., Dali, R., Zoumalan, C. I. & Toga, A. W. (2001) Genetic influences on brain structure. Nature Neuroscience 4:1253–58. [aDF]Google Scholar
Tinbergen, N. (1952) Derived activities: Their causation, biological significance, origin, and emancipation during evolution. Quarterly Review of Biology 27:132. [ED]Google Scholar
Tincoff, R. & Jusczyk, P. W. (1999) Some beginnings of word comprehension in sixmonth- olds. Psychological Science 10:172–75. [aDF]Google Scholar
Tobias, P. V. Department of Anatomical Sciences, University of Witwatersrand, Johannesburg, South Africa, personal communication regarding Louis Leakey's belly laugh. [aDF]Google Scholar
Tomasello, M. (1999a) Social cognition before the revolution. In: Early social cognition, ed. Rochat, P.. Erlbaum. [SB]Google Scholar
Tomasello, M. (1999b) The cultural origins of human cognition. Harvard University Press. [aDF]Google Scholar
Tomasello, M. & Camaioni, L. (1997) A comparison of the gestural communication of apes and human infants. Human Development 40:724. [aDF]Google Scholar
Tomasello, M., Savage-Rumbaugh, S. & Kruger, A. C. (1993) Imitative learning of actions on objects by children, chimpanzees, and enculturated chimpanzees. Child Development 64:16881705. [aDF]Google Scholar
Toth, N., Schick, K., Savage-Rumbaugh, S., Sevcik, R. & Savage-Rumbaugh, D. (1993) Pan the tool-maker: Investigations into the stone tool-making and toolusing capabilities of a bonobo (Pan paniscus). Journal of Archaeological Science 20:8191. [rDF]Google Scholar
Tracer, D. (2002) Did australopithecines crawl? American Journal of Physical Anthropology Supplement 34:156–57. [KRR]Google Scholar
Trainor, L. J., Austin, C. M. & Desjardins, R. N. (2000) Is infant-directed speech prosody a result of the vocal expression of emotion? Psychological Science 11:188–95. [aDF]Google Scholar
Trainor, L. J., Clark, E. D., Huntley, A. & Adams, B. A. (1997) The acoustic basis of preference for infant-directed singing. Infant Behavior and Development 20:383–96. [aDF]Google Scholar
Trehub, S. E., Trainor, L. J. & Unyk, A. M. (1993) Music and speech processing in the first year of life. In: Advances in child development and behavior, ed. Reese, H. W.. Academic Press. [aDF]Google Scholar
Trevarthen, C. (1974) Conversations with a two-month-old. New Scientist 2:230–35. [SB]Google Scholar
Trevarthen, C. (1990) Signs before speech. In: The semiotic web, ed. Sebeok, T. A. & Umiker-Sebeok, J.. Mouton de Gruyter. [SB]Google Scholar
Trevarthen, C. (1997) Fetal and neonatal psychology: Intrinsic motives and learning behavior. In: Advances in perinatal medicine, ed. Cockburn, F., pp. 282–91. Parthenon. [ED]Google Scholar
Trevarthen, C. (1998) The concept and foundations of infant intersubjectivity. In: Intersubjective communication and emotion in early ontogeny, ed. Braten, S.. Cambridge University Press. [SB]Google Scholar
Trevarthen, C. (1999) Musicality and the intrinsic motive pulse: Evidence from human psychobiology and infant communication. Musicae Scientiae [Special Issue 1999–2000]:155215. [ED]Google Scholar
Trevarthen, C. & Aitken, K. (1994) Brain development, infant communication, and empathy disorders: Intrinsic factors in child mental health. Development and Psychopathology 6:597633. [ED]Google Scholar
Trevarthen, C., Kokkinaki, T. & Fiammenghi, G. (1999) What infant imitations can communicate: with mothers, with fathers and with peers. In: Imitation in infancy, ed. Nadel, J. & Butterworth, G.. Cambridge University Press. [SJC]Google Scholar
Trevarthen, C. & Malloch, S. (2000) The dance of wellbeing: Defining the music therapeutic effect. Nordic Journal of Music Therapy 9(2):317. [ED]Google Scholar
Trivers, R. L. (1974) Parent-offspring conflict. American Zoologist 14:249–64. [DS]Google Scholar
Tronick, E. Z. (1989) Emotions and emotional communication in infants. American Psychologist 44:112–26. [ED]Google Scholar
van Schaik, C. P., Ancrenaz, M., Borgen, G., Galdikas, B., Knott, C. D., Singleton, I., Suzuki, A., Utami, S. S. & Merrill, M. (2003) Orangutan cultures and the evolution of material culture. Science 299:102105. [PB]Google Scholar
Vekua, A., Lordkipanidze, D., Rightmire, G. P., Agusti, J., Fering, R., Maisuradze, G., Mouskhelishivili, A., Nioradze, M., Ponce de Leon, M., Tappen, M., Tvalchrelidze, M. & Zolliker, C. (2002) A new skull from Dmanisi, Georgia. Science 297:8589. [CLB]Google Scholar
Walker, A. & Leakey, R. (1993) The Nariokotome Homo erectus skeleton. Harvard University Press. [aDF]Google Scholar
Wallis, J. (1997) A survey of reproductive parameters in the free-ranging chimpanzees in Gombe National Park. Journal of Reproduction and Fertility 109:297307. [rDF]Google Scholar
Warren, J. M. (1976) Tool use in mammals. In: Evolution of brain and behavior in vertebrates, ed. Masterson, R., Bitterman, M., Campbell, C. & Horton, N.. Erlbaum. [RS]Google Scholar
Washburn, S. L. (1959) Speculations on the interrelations of the history of tools and biological evolution. Human Biology 31:2131. [CLB]Google Scholar
Washburn, S. L. (1960) Tools and human evolution. Scientific American 203:6375. [CLB]Google Scholar
Wheeldon, L. (2000) Generating prosodic structure. In: Aspects of language production, ed. Wheeldon, L.. MIT Press. [aDF]Google Scholar
Wheeler, P. E. (1988) Stand tall and stay cool. New Scientist 12:6265. [arDF]Google Scholar
Whiten, A., Goodall, J., McGrew, W. C., Nishida, T., Reynolds, V., Sugiyama, Y., Tutin, C. E. G., Wrangham, R. W. & Boesch, C. (1999) Cultures in chimpanzees. Nature 399:682–85. [CS]Google Scholar
Wiesenfeld, A. R. & Klorman, R. (1978) The mother's psychophysiological reactions to contrasting affective expressions by her own and an unfamiliar infant. Developmental Psychology 14:294304. [DS]Google Scholar
Wilcox, S. (2002) The gesture-language interface: Evidence from signed languages. In: Progress in sign language research: In honor of Siegmund Prillwitz/ Fortschritte in der Gebärdenspechforshung: Festschrift für Siegmund Prillwitz, ed. Schulmeister, R. & Reinitzer, H., pp. 6381. SIGNUM-Verlag. [SW]Google Scholar
Wilkins, W. K. and Wakefield, J. (1995) Brain evolution and neurolinguistic preconditions. Behavioral and Brain Sciences 18:161226. [EG]Google Scholar
Wolff, P. H. (1969) The natural history of crying and other vocalizations in early infancy. In: Determinants of infant behavior, ed. Foss, B. M.. Methuen. [aDF]Google Scholar
Wrangham, R. W. (1979) On the evolution of ape social systems. Social Science Information 18:335–69. [AF]Google Scholar
Wrangham, R. W., Jones, J. H., Laden, G., Pilbeam, D. & Conklin-Brittain, N. L. (1999) The raw and the stolen: Cooking and the ecology of human origins. Current Anthropology 40:567–94. [AF]Google Scholar
Wray, A. (1998) Protolanguage as a holistic system for social interaction. Language and Communication 18:4767. [SJC]Google Scholar
Wray, A. (2002) Dual processing in protolanguage: Performance without competence. In: The transition to language, ed. Wray, A.. Oxford University Press. [LC]Google Scholar
Zihlman, A. L. (1981) Women as shapers of the human adaptation. In: Woman the gatherer, ed. Dahlberg, F.. Yale University Press. [aDF, RS]Google Scholar
Zimmerman, E., Newman, J. D. & Jürgens, U., eds. (1995) Current topics in primate vocal communication. Plenum. [PB]Google Scholar