Crossref Citations
This article has been cited by the following publications. This list is generated based on data provided by
Crossref.
Shettleworth, Sara J.
2012.
Modularity, comparative cognition and human uniqueness.
Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences,
Vol. 367,
Issue. 1603,
p.
2794.
Heintz, Christophe
2012.
From core cognition to intuitive theories: A psychologist’s account of conceptual change.
Metascience,
Vol. 21,
Issue. 2,
p.
439.
Abrahamson, Dor
2012.
Seeing chance: perceptual reasoning as an epistemic resource for grounding compound event spaces.
ZDM,
Vol. 44,
Issue. 7,
p.
869.
McCaffrey, Joseph
and
Machery, Edouard
2012.
Philosophical issues about concepts.
WIREs Cognitive Science,
Vol. 3,
Issue. 2,
p.
265.
Machamer, Peter
and
Osbeck, Lisa
2012.
Perception, Realism, and the Problem of Reference.
p.
142.
Weng, Juyang
and
Luciw, Matthew
2012.
Brain-Like Emergent Spatial Processing.
IEEE Transactions on Autonomous Mental Development,
Vol. 4,
Issue. 2,
p.
161.
Reigosa-Crespo, Vivian
González-Alemañy, Eduardo
León, Teresa
Torres, Rosario
Mosquera, Raysil
Valdés-Sosa, Mitchell
and
Allen, Philip
2013.
Numerical Capacities as Domain-Specific Predictors beyond Early Mathematics Learning: A Longitudinal Study.
PLoS ONE,
Vol. 8,
Issue. 11,
p.
e79711.
Ziv, Ido
and
Leiser, David
2013.
The need for central resources in answering questions in different domains: Folk psychology, biology, and economics.
Journal of Cognitive Psychology,
Vol. 25,
Issue. 7,
p.
816.
Sun, Ron
2013.
Autonomous generation of symbolic representations through subsymbolic activities.
Philosophical Psychology,
Vol. 26,
Issue. 6,
p.
888.
Abrahamson, Dor
2013.
Toward a taxonomy of design genres.
p.
218.
Котов, Алексей
and
Дагаев, Николай
2013.
Роль предыдущих знаний в порождении эффекта понятийной гибкости.
Психологические исследования,
Vol. 6,
Issue. 29,
Syrett, Kristen
and
Musolino, Julien
2013.
Collectivity, Distributivity, and the Interpretation of Plural Numerical Expressions in Child and Adult Language.
Language Acquisition,
Vol. 20,
Issue. 4,
p.
259.
Russell, Yvan I.
and
Gobet, Fernand
2013.
What is Counterintuitive? Religious Cognition and Natural Expectation.
Review of Philosophy and Psychology,
Vol. 4,
Issue. 4,
p.
715.
Umbres, Radu
2013.
Chasse au dahu et vigilance épistémique.
Terrain,
p.
84.
Hoffmann, Danielle
Hornung, Caroline
Martin, Romain
and
Schiltz, Christine
2013.
Developing number–space associations: SNARC effects using a color discrimination task in 5-year-olds.
Journal of Experimental Child Psychology,
Vol. 116,
Issue. 4,
p.
775.
Falikman, Maria V.
2014.
The Cambridge Handbook of Cultural-Historical Psychology.
p.
474.
Løvschal, Mette
and
Holst, Mads Kähler
2014.
Repeating boundaries – repertoires of landscape regulations in southern Scandinavia in the Late Bronze Age and Pre-Roman Iron Age.
Danish Journal of Archaeology,
Vol. 3,
Issue. 2,
p.
95.
Bjorklund, David F.
and
Ellis, Bruce J.
2014.
Children, childhood, and development in evolutionary perspective.
Developmental Review,
Vol. 34,
Issue. 3,
p.
225.
Abrahamson, Dor
2014.
Building educational activities for understanding: An elaboration on the embodied-design framework and its epistemic grounds.
International Journal of Child-Computer Interaction,
Vol. 2,
Issue. 1,
p.
1.
Hunt, Kathryn
and
Chittka, Lars
2014.
False memory susceptibility is correlated with categorisation ability in humans.
F1000Research,
Vol. 3,
Issue. ,
p.
154.
Target article
What is the significance of The Origin of Concepts for philosophers' and psychologists' theories of concepts?
Related commentaries (1)
Précis of The Origin of Concepts