Hostname: page-component-cd9895bd7-7cvxr Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-12-27T09:21:02.453Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

On relativity of time and the causal status of consciousness

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  04 February 2010

Ali Habibi
Affiliation:
Department of Psychology, Vanderbilt University, Nashville, TN 37240 Electronic mail: [email protected]; [email protected]
Michael S. Bendele
Affiliation:
Department of Psychology, Vanderbilt University, Nashville, TN 37240 Electronic mail: [email protected]; [email protected]

Abstract

Image of the first page of this content. For PDF version, please use the ‘Save PDF’ preceeding this image.'
Type
Continuing Commentary
Copyright
Copyright © Cambridge University Press 1993

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

Allport, D. A., Tipper, S. P. & Chmiel, N. R. J. (1985) Perceptual integration and postcategorical filtering. In: Attention and performance XI, ed. Posner, M. I.. Erlbaum. [WTN]Google Scholar
Anderson, J. R. (1976) Language, memory, and thought. Erlbaum. [WTN]Google Scholar
Anderson, J. R. (1983) The architecture of cognition. Harvard University Press. [WTN]Google Scholar
Arnheim, R. (1969) Visual thinking. University of California Press. [JG]Google Scholar
Baars, B. J. (1989) A cognitive theory of consciousness. Cambridge University Press. [rMV, DN]Google Scholar
Baars, B. J. (1991) A curious coincidence? Consciousness as an object of scientific scrutiny fits our personal experience remarkably well. Behavioral and Brain Sciences 14(4): 669–70. [rMV]CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bloch, N. (1991) Evidence against epiphenomenalism. Behavioral and Brain Sciences 14(4): 670–72. [rMV]CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bohm, D. (1952) A suggested interpretation of the quantum theory in terms of hidden variables. Physical Review 85:166–89. [rMV]CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bohm, D. & Hiley, B. J. (1987) An ontological basis for the quantum theory. Physics Reports 144:323–48. [rMV]CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Broadbent, D. E. (1958) Perception and communication. Pergamon Press. [rMV, JC]CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Cherry, C. (1953) Some experiments on the reception of speech with one and with two ears. Journal of the Acoustical Society of America 25:975–79. [rMV]CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Deikman, A. J. (1966) Deautomatization and the mystical experience. Psychiatry 29:324–38. [KRR]CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Dennett, D. C. (1991) Consciousness explained. Little Brown. [rMV]Google Scholar
Dennett, D. C. & Kinsbourne, M. (1992) Time and the observer: The where and when of consciousness in the brain. Behavioral and Brain Sciences 15:183200. [rMV]CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Dixon, N. F. (1971) Subliminal perception: The nature of a controversy. McGraw-Hill. [JG]Google Scholar
Einstein, A. (1952) Relativity. The special and the general theory (15th ed., Lawson, R., Trans.) Crown Publishers. [AH]Google Scholar
Erdelyi, M. H. (1974) A new look at the New Look: Perceptual defense and vigilance. Psychological Review 81:125. [JG]CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Folse, H. J. (1985) The philosophy of Niels Bohr: The framework of complementarity. Elsevier. [rMV]Google Scholar
Glass, A. L., Holyoak, K. J. & Santa, J. L. (1979) Cognition. Addison-Wesley. [JG]Google Scholar
Holender, D. (1986) Semantic activation without conscious identification in dichotic listening, parafoveal vision, and visual masking: A survey and appraisal. Behavioral and Brain Sciences 9:166. [JG]CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Jahn, R. G. & Dunne, B. J. (1987) Margins of reality: The role of consciousness in the physical world. Harcourt Brace Jovanovich. [rMV, KRR]Google Scholar
Kahneman, D. & Treisman, A. (1984) Changing views of attention and autoroaticity. In: Varieties of attention, ed. Parasuraman, R. & Davies, D. R.. Academic Press. [rMV]Google Scholar
Kosslyn, S. M. (1980) Image and mind. Harvard University Press. [JG]Google Scholar
La Berge, D. (1981) Automatic information processing: A review. In: Attention and performance IX, ed. Long, J. & Baddeley, A.. Erlbaum. [rMV]Google Scholar
Libet, B. (1985) Unconscious cerebral initiative and the role of conscious will in voluntary action. Behavioral and Brain Sciences 8:529–66. [rMV, AH]CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Libet, B. (1991) Conscious functions and brain processes. Behavioral and Brain Sciences 14:685–86. [rMV]CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Libet, B., Wright, E. W. Jr., Feinstein, B. & Pearl, D. K. (1979) Subjective referral of the timing for a conscious experience: A functional role for the somatosensory specific projection system in man. Brain 102:193224. [rMV]CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Mandler, G. (1991) The processing of information is not conscious, but its products often are. Behavioral and Brain Sciences 14(4): 688–89. [rMV]CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Marcel, A. J. (1980) Conscious and preconscious recognition of polysemous words: Locating the selective effects of prior verbal context. In: Attention and performance VIII, ed. Nickerson, R. S.. Erlbaum. [WTN]Google Scholar
Marks, D. (1983) Mental imagery and consciousness: A theoretical review. In: Imagery: Current theory, research, and application, ed. Sheikh, A. A.. Wiley. [JG]Google Scholar
Navon, D. (1989) The importance of being visible: On the role of attention in a mind viewed as an anarehic intelligence system. I. Basic tenets. European Journal of Cognitive Psychology 1:191213. [DN]CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Navon, D. (1991) The function of consciousness or of information? Behavioral and Brain Sciences 14:690–91. [rMV, DN]CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Neely, J. H. (1976) Semantic priming and retrieval from lexical memory: Evidence for facilitatory and inhibitory processes. Memory & Cognition 4:648–54. [WTN]CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Neely, J. H. (1977) Semantic priming and retrieval from semantic memory: Roles of inhibitionless spreading activation and limited-capacity attention. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General 106:227–34. [WTN]Google Scholar
Neill, W. T. (1977) Inhibitory and facilitatory-processes in attention, journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance 3:444–50. [WTN]Google Scholar
Neill, W. T. (1985) Levels of processing in disruptive effects of prior information. Memory & Cognition 13:477–84. [WTN]CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Neill, W. T. (1989) Ambiguity and context: An activation-suppression model. In: Resolving semantic ambiguity, ed. Gorfein, D. S.. Springer-Verlag. [WTN]Google Scholar
Neill, W. T. (1991) Consciousness and the inhibitory control of cognition. Invited address to the meeting of the American Psychological Association, San Francisco, 08. [WTN]Google Scholar
Neill, W. T., Lissner, L. S. & Beck, J. L. (1990) Negative priming in samedifferent matching: Further evidence for a central locus of inhibition. Perception & Psychophysics 48:398400. [WTN]CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Neill, W. T. & Westberry, R. L. (1987) Selective attention and the suppression of cognitive noise. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition 13:327–34. [WTN]Google Scholar
Neisser, U. (1976) Cognition and reality: Principles and implications of cognitive psychology. Freeman. [JG]Google Scholar
Perry, R. B. (1904) Conceptions and misconceptions of consciousness. Psuchologinal Review 11:282–96. [KRR]CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Posner, M. I. (1982) Cumulative development of attention theory. American Psychologist 37:168–79. [WTN]CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Posner, M. I. & Snyder, C. R. R. (1975) Facilitation and inhibition in the processing of signals. In: Attention and performance V, ed. Rabbitt, P. M. A. & Dornic, S.. Academic Press. [WTN]Google Scholar
Rao, K. R. (1989) Meditation: Secular and sacred: Review and assessment of some recent research. Journal of the Indian Academy of Applied Psychology 15:5174. [KRR]Google Scholar
Rao, K. R. & Palmer, J. (1987) The anomaly called psi: Recent research and criticism. Behavioral and Brain Sciences 10:539643. [KHR]CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Richardson, A. (1983) Imagery: Definition and types. In: Imagery: Current theory, research, and application, ed. Sheikh, A. A.. Wiley. [JG]Google Scholar
Russell, B. (1948) Human knowledge: Its scope and limits. Simon and Schuster. [KRR]Google Scholar
Searle, J. R. (1983) Intentionality: An essay in the philosophy of mind. Cambridge University Press. [KRR]CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Shapiro, D. H. Jr. & Walsh, R. N. (1984) Meditation: Classic and contemporary perspectives. Aldine. [KRR]Google Scholar
Shepard, R. N. (1978) The mental image. American Psychologist 33:125–37. [JG]CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Shepard, R. N. & Cooper, L. A. (1982) Mental images and their transformations. MIT Press. [JG]Google Scholar
Shevrin, H. (1991) A lawful first-person psychology involving a causal consciousness: A psychoanalytic solution. Behavioral and Brain Sciences 14:693–94. [rMV]CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Sloman, A. (1991) Developing concepts of consciousness. Behavioral and Brain Sciences 14(4): 694–95. [rMV]CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Smith, E. E., Haviland, S. E., Reder, L. M., Brownell, H. & Adams, N. (1976) When preparation fails: Disruptive effects of prior information on perceptual recognition. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance 2:151–61. [WTN]Google ScholarPubMed
Sperry, R. W. (1976) Mental phenomena as causal determinants in brain function. In: Consciousness and the brain, ed. Globus, G. G., Maxwell, G. & Savodnik, I.. Plenum Press. [KRR]Google Scholar
Stroop, J. R. (1935) Studies of interference in serial verbal reactions. Journal of Experimental Psychology 18:643–62. [WTN]CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Tipper, S. P. (1985) The negative priming effect: Inhibitory priming by ignored objects. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology 37A:571–90. [WTN]CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Tipper, S. P., Brehaut, J. C. & Driver, J. (1990) Selection of moving and static objects for the control of spatially directed action. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance 16:492504. [WTN]Google ScholarPubMed
Tipper, S. P. & Cranston, M. (1985) Selective attention and priming: Inhibitory and facilitatory effects of ignored primes. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology 37A:591611. [WTN]CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Tulving, E. (1984) Multiple book review of Elements of episodic memory. Behavioral and Brain Sciences 7:223–68. [JG]CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Velmans, M. (1990) Consciousness, brain and the physical world. Philosophical Psychology 3:7799. [rMV]CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Velmans, M. (1991a) Is human information processing conscious? Behavioral and Brain Sciences 14:651726. [rMV, JG, AH, DN, WTN, KRR]CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Velmans, M. (1991r) Consciousness from a first-person perspective. Behavioral and Brain Sciences 14:702–19. [rMV, DN, KRR]CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Velmans, M. (1992a) Synopsis of “Consciousness, brain and the physical world.” Philosophical Psychology 5:155–57. [rMV]Google Scholar
Velmans, M. (1992b) The world as-perceived, the world as-described by physics, and the thing-itself: A reply to Rentoul and Wetherick. Philosophical Psychology 5:167–72. [rMV]CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Velmans, M. (1993) A reflexive science of consciousness. In: Experimental and theoretical studies of consciousness. Ciba Foundation Symposium No. 174. Wiley [rMV]Google ScholarPubMed
Walker, E. H. (1970) The nature of consciousness. Mathematical Biosciences 7:131–78. [KRR]CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Walker, E. H. (1977) Quantum mechanical tunneling in synaptic and emphaptic transmission. International Journal of Quantum Chemistry 11:103–27. [KRR]CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Yates, J. (1985) The content of awareness is a model of the world. Psychological Review 92:249–84. [WTN]CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Yee, P. L. (1991) Semantic inhibition of ignored words during a figure classification task. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology 43A:127–53. [WTN]CrossRefGoogle Scholar