Hostname: page-component-78c5997874-t5tsf Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-11-04T17:54:11.386Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

More than control freaks: Evaluative and motivational functions of goals

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  08 April 2008

Fabio Paglieri
Affiliation:
Istituto di Scienze e Tecnologie della Cognizione, Consiglio Nazionale delle Ricerche, 00185 Rome, Italy. [email protected]://www.media.unisi.it/cirg/fp/[email protected]://www.istc.cnr.it/createhtml.php?nbr=62
Cristiano Castelfranchi
Affiliation:
Istituto di Scienze e Tecnologie della Cognizione, Consiglio Nazionale delle Ricerche, 00185 Rome, Italy. [email protected]://www.media.unisi.it/cirg/fp/[email protected]://www.istc.cnr.it/createhtml.php?nbr=62

Abstract

True to its sensorimotor inspiration, Hurley's shared circuits model (SCM) describes goal-states only within a homeostatic mechanism for action control, neglecting to consider other functions of goals – namely, evaluation and motivation. This restriction thwarts Hurley's project of identifying the information resources enabling social cognition. In order to master intentional behavior, deliberation, and action understanding, we need to be more than just “control freaks.”

Type
Open Peer Commentary
Copyright
Copyright © Cambridge University Press 2008

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

Castelfranchi, C. (1998) Modelling social action for AI agents. Artificial Intelligence 103:157–82.Google Scholar
Castelfranchi, C. & Paglieri, F. (2007) The role of beliefs in goal dynamics: Prolegomena to a constructive theory of intentions. Synthese 155:237–63.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Csibra, G. & Gergely, G. (2007) “Obsessed with goals”: Functions and mechanisms of teleological interpretation of actions in humans. Acta Psychologica 124:6078.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Dennett, D. (1987) The intentional stance. MIT Press.Google Scholar
Frijda, N. H. (1986) The emotions. Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Gallese, V., Fadiga, L., Fogassi, L. & Rizzolatti, G. (1996) Action recognition in the premotor cortex. Brain 119:593609.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gallese, V. & Goldman, A. (1998) Mirror neurons and the simulation theory of mindreading. Trends in Cognitive Sciences 2:493501.Google Scholar
Miller, G., Galanter, E. & Pribram, K. H. (1960) Plans and the structure of behaviour. Holt, Rinehart & Winston.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Millikan, R. (2006) Styles of rationality. In: Rational animals? ed. Hurley, S. & Nudds, M., pp. 117–26. Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Ortony, A., Clore, G. L. & Collins, A. (1988) The cognitive structure of emotions. Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Pezzulo, G. & Castelfranchi, C. (2007) The symbol detachment problem. Cognitive Processing 8:115–31.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rosenblueth, A., Wiener, N. & Bigelow, J. (1968) Behaviour, purpose, and teleology. In: Modern systems research for the behavioural scientist, ed. Buckley, W., pp. 368–72. Aldine.Google Scholar
Tomasello, M. (1999) The cultural origins of human cognition. Harvard University Press.Google Scholar