Hostname: page-component-586b7cd67f-t7fkt Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-11-30T17:41:39.875Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Misbelief and the neglect of environmental context

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  28 January 2010

David Dunning
Affiliation:
Department of Psychology, Cornell University, Ithaca, NY 14853. [email protected]://cornellpsych.org/sasi/index.php

Abstract

Focusing on the individual's internal cognitive architecture, McKay & Dennett (M&D) provide an incomplete analysis because they neglect the crucial role played by the external environment in producing misbeliefs and determining whether those misbeliefs are adaptive. In some environments, positive illusions are not adaptive. Further, misbeliefs often arise because the environment commonly fails to provide crucial information needed to form accurate judgments.

Type
Open Peer Commentary
Copyright
Copyright © Cambridge University Press 2010

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

Armor, D. A. & Taylor, S. E. (1998) Situated optimism: Specific outcome expectancies and self-regulation. In: Advances in experimental social psychology, vol. 30, ed. Zanna, M., pp. 309–79. Academic Press.Google Scholar
Caputo, D. D. & Dunning, D. (2005) What you don't know: The role played by errors of omission in imperfect self-assessments. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology 41:488505.Google Scholar
Denrell, J. (2005) Why most people disapprove of me: Experience sampling in impression formation. Psychological Review 112:951–78.Google Scholar
Dillard, A. J., McCaul, K. D. & Klein, W. M. P. (2006) Unrealistic optimism in smokers: Implications for smoking myth endorsement and self-protective motivation. Journal of Health Communication 11:93102.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Dunning, D. (2005) Self-insight: Roadblocks and detours on the path to knowing thyself. Psychology Press.Google Scholar
Dunning, D., Heath, C. & Suls, J. M. (2004) Flawed self-assessment: Implications for health, education, and the workplace. Psychological Science in the Public Interest 5:69106.Google Scholar
Duntley, J. & Buss, D. M. (1998) Evolved anti-homicide modules. Paper presented at the Human Behavior and Evolution Society Conference, Davis, CA, July 1998.Google Scholar
Fetchenhauer, D. & Dunning, D. (2009) Do people trust too much or too little? Journal of Economic Psychology 30:263–76.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Fetchenhauer, D. & Dunning, D. (in press) Why so cynical? Asymmetric feedback underlies misguided skepticism in the trustworthiness of others. Psychological Science.Google Scholar
Jaccard, J., Dodge, T. & Guilamo-Ramos, V. (2005) Metacognition, risk behavior, and risk outcomes: The role of intelligence and perceived knowledge. Health Psychology 24:161–70.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kruger, J. & Dunning, D. (1999) Unskilled and unaware of it: How difficulties in recognizing one's own incompetence lead to inflated self-assessments. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 77:1121–34.Google Scholar
Meyer, D., Leventhal, H. & Gutmann, M. (1985) Common-sense models of illness: The example of hypertension. Health Psychology 4:115–35.Google Scholar
Nisbett, R. E. & Ross, L. (1980) Human inference: Strategies and shortcomings of social judgment. Prentice-Hall.Google Scholar
Smith, E. R. & Collins, E. C. (2009) Contextualizing person perception: Distributed social cognition. Psychological Review 116:343–64.Google Scholar