No CrossRef data available.
Article contents
Independent or interactive routes: What are the constraints?
Published online by Cambridge University Press: 04 February 2010
Abstract
An abstract is not available for this content so a preview has been provided. Please use the Get access link above for information on how to access this content.
![Image of the first page of this content. For PDF version, please use the ‘Save PDF’ preceeding this image.'](https://static.cambridge.org/content/id/urn%3Acambridge.org%3Aid%3Aarticle%3AS0140525X00048202/resource/name/firstPage-S0140525X00048202a.jpg)
- Type
- Continuing Commentary
- Information
- Copyright
- Copyright © Cambridge University Press 1987
References
Brown, G. D. A. (in press) Resolving inconsistency: A computational model of word naming. Journal of Memory and Language. [rLJE, GDAB]Google Scholar
Feldman, L. B. & Turvey, M. T. (1983) Word recognition in Serbo-Croatian is phonologically analytic. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance 9:288–98. [rLJE]Google ScholarPubMed
Glushko, R. J. (1979) The organisation and activation of orthographic knowledge in reading aloud. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance 5:674–91. [GDAB]Google Scholar
Henderson, L. (1982) Orthography and word recognition in reading. Academic Press. [rLJE, GDAB]Google Scholar
Humphreys, G. W. & Evett, L. J. (1985) Are there independent lexical and nonlexical routes in word processing? An evaluation of the dual-route theory of reading. Behavioral and Brain Sciences 8:689–740.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kay, J. (1982) Psychological mechanisms of oral reading of single words. Doctoral dissertation, University of Cambridge. [GDAB]Google Scholar
Kay, J. & Marcel, A. J. (1981) One process, not two, in reading aloud: Lexical analogies do the work of non-lexical rules. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology 33A:397–414. [GDAB]CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lukatela, C., Papadic, D., Ognjenovic, P. & Turvey, M. T. (1980) Lexical decision in a phonologically shallow orthography. Memory and Cognition 8:124–32. [rLJE]CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Patterson, K. E. & Morton, J. (1985) From orthography to phonology: An attempt at an old interpretation. In: Surface dyslexia: Neuropsychological and cognitive studies of phonological reading, ed. Patterson, K. E., Marshall, J. C. & Coltheart, M.. Erlbaum. [GDAB]Google Scholar
Seidenberg, M. S. (1985a) The time course of phonological code activation in two writing systems. Cognition 19:1–30. [rLJE, GDAB]CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Seidenberg, M. S. (1985b) Constraining models of word recognition. Cognition 20:169–90. [GDAB]CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Seidenberg, M. S., Waters, G. S., Barnes, M. A. & Tanenhaus, M. K. (1984) When does irregular spelling or pronunciation influence word recognition? Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior 23:383–404. [GDAB]CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Shallice, T., Warrington, E. K. & McCarthy, B. (1983) Reading without semantics. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology 35A:111–38. [GDAB]CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Turvey, M. T., Feldman, L. B. & Lukatela, G. (1984) The Serbo-Croatian orthography constrains the reader to a phonologically analytic strategy. In: Orthographies and reading, ed. Henderson, L.. Erlbaum. [rLJE]Google Scholar