Hostname: page-component-cd9895bd7-gvvz8 Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-12-27T09:32:24.705Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Humans did not evolve from bats

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  04 February 2010

Steven Pinker
Affiliation:
Department of Brain and Cognitive Sciences, Massachusetts Institute of Technology, Cambridge, MA 02739 Electronic mail: [email protected]
Paul Bloom
Affiliation:
Department of Psychology, University of Arizona, Tucson, AZ 85721 Electronic mail: [email protected]

Abstract

Image of the first page of this content. For PDF version, please use the ‘Save PDF’ preceeding this image.'
Type
Authors' Response
Copyright
Copyright © Cambridge University Press 1994

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

Bickerton, D. (1981) Roots of language. Karoma. [AG]Google Scholar
Hubel, D. & Wiesel, T. (1962) Receptive fields, binocular interaction and functional architecture. Journal of Physiology 160:106–54. [AG]CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Liberman, A. M. & Mattingly, I. G. (1985) The motor theory of speech perception revised. Cognition 21:136. [HMS]CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lindblom, B. (1990) Adaptive complexity in sound patterns. Behavioral and Brain Sciences 13(4):743–44. [HMS]CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Matthews, R. (1989) Introduction. In: Learnability and linguistic theory, ed. Matthews, R. & Demopoulos, W.. Klnwer. [AG]CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Pinker, S. & Bloom, P. (1990) Natural language and natural selection. Behavioral and Brain Sciences 13(4):707–84. [rSP, AG, HMS]CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ridley, M. (1990) Arbitrariness no argument against adaptation. Behavioral and Brain Sciences 13(4):756. [AG]Google Scholar
Sober, E. (1990) Anatomizing the rhinoceros. Behavioral and Brain Sciences 13(4):764–65. [AG]Google Scholar
Sperber, D. (1990) The evolution of the language faculty: A paradox and its solution. Behavioral and Brain Sciences 13(4):756–58. [AG]CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Stabler, E. (1983) How are grammars represented? Behavioral and Brain Sciences 6:391402. [AG]CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Studdert-Kennedy, M. (1990) This view of language. Behavioral and Brain Sciences 13(4):758–59. [HMS]CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Suga, N., O'Neill, W. E., Kujirai, K. & Manabe, T. (1983) Specificity of combination-sensitive neurons for processing of complex biosonar signals in auditory cortex of the mustached bat. Neurophysiology 49:15731627. [HMS]Google Scholar
Sullivan, W. E. & Konishi, M. (1986) Neural map of interaural phase difference in the owl's brainstem. Proceedings of the National Academy of Science, USA 83:84008404. [HMS]Google Scholar
Sussman, H. M. (1988) The neurogenesis of phonology. In: Phonological processes and brain mechanisms, ed. Whitaker, H.. Springer-Verlag. [HMS]Google Scholar
Sussman, H. M. (1989) Neural coding of relational invariance in speech: Human language analogs to the barn owl. Psychological Review 96:631–42. [HMS]Google Scholar
Wagner, H., Takahashi, T. & Konishi, M. (1987) Representation of interaural time difference in the central nucleus of the barn owl's inferior colliculus. Journal of Neuroscience 7:3105–16. [HMS]CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Wilkins, W. & Dumford, J. (1990) In defense of exaptation. Behavioral and Brain Sciences 13(4):763–64. [AG]Google Scholar