Hostname: page-component-586b7cd67f-r5fsc Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-11-27T18:35:49.645Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Human evolutionary history and contemporary evolutionary theory provide insight when assessing cultural group selection

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  09 March 2016

Agustin Fuentes
Affiliation:
Department of Anthropology, University of Notre Dame, Notre Dame, IN 46556. [email protected]@nd.eduhttp://anthropology.nd.edu/faculty-and-staff/faculty-by-alpha/agustin-fuentes/http://anthropology.nd.edu/faculty-and-staff/faculty-by-alpha/marc-kissel/
Marc Kissel
Affiliation:
Department of Anthropology, University of Notre Dame, Notre Dame, IN 46556. [email protected]@nd.eduhttp://anthropology.nd.edu/faculty-and-staff/faculty-by-alpha/agustin-fuentes/http://anthropology.nd.edu/faculty-and-staff/faculty-by-alpha/marc-kissel/

Abstract

Richerson et al. provide a much needed roadmap for assessing cultural group selection (CGS) theory and for applying it to understanding variation between contemporary human groups. However, the current proposal lacks connection to relevant evidence from the human evolutionary record and requires a better integration with contemporary evolutionary theory. The article also misapplies the Fst statistic.

Type
Open Peer Commentary
Copyright
Copyright © Cambridge University Press 2016 

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

Andersson, C., Törnberg, A. & Törnberg, P. (2014) An evolutionary developmental approach to cultural evolution. Current Anthropology 55(2):154–74.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Clark, J. D. & Brown, K. S. (2001) The Twin Rivers Kopje, Zambia: Stratigraphy, fauna, and artefact assemblages from the 1954 and 1956 excavations. Journal of Archaeological Science 28(3):305–30.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Fuentes, A. (2015) Integrative anthropology and the human niche: Towards a contemporary approach to human evolution. American Anthropologist 117(2):302–15. doi: 10.1111/aman.12248.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Grove, M. & Coward, F. (2008) From individual neurons to social brains. Cambridge Archaeological Journal 18:387400. doi: 10.1017/S0959774308000437.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Herrmann, E., Call, J., Hernandez-Lloreda, M. V., Hare, B. & Tomasello, M. (2007) Humans have evolved specialized skills of social cognition: The cultural intelligence hypothesis. Science 317:1360–66.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Jablonka, E. & Lamb, M. (2005) Evolution in four dimensions: Genetic, epigenetic, behavioral, and symbolic variation in the history of life. MIT Press.Google Scholar
Laland, K. N., Tobias, U., Feldman, M., Sterelny, K., Muller, G., Moczek, A., Jabonka, E. & Odling-Smee, J. (2014) Does evolutionary theory need a rethink? Yes, urgently. Nature 514:161–64.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Laland, K. N., Uller, T., Feldman, M. W., Sterelny, K., Müller, G. B., Moczek, A., Jablonka, E. & Odling-Smee, J. (2015) The extended evolutionary synthesis: Its structure, assumptions and predictions Proceedings of the Royal Society of London B 282(1813):20151019. http://dx.doi.org/10.1098/rspb.2015.1019.Google ScholarPubMed
MacKinnon, K. C. & Fuentes, A. (2011) Primates, niche construction, and social complexity: The roles of social cooperation and altruism. In: Origins of altruism and cooperation. Developments in primatology: Progress and prospects, vol. 36, Part 2, ed. Sussman, R. W. & Cloninger, R. C., pp. 121–43. Springer.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Malone, N. M., Fuentes, A. & White, F. J. (2012) Variation in the social systems of extant hominoids: Comparative insight into the social behaviour of early hominins. International Journal of Primatology 33(6):1251–77. doi: 10.10007/s10764-012-9617-0.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Mania, D. & Mania, U. (1988) Deliberate engravings on bone artefacts of Homo erectus . Rock Art Research 5:91107.Google Scholar
Peer, P. Van, Rots, V. & Vroomans, J. (2004) A story of colourful diggers and grinders: The Sangoan and Lupemban at site 8-B-11, Sai Island, Northern Sudan. Before Farming 3:128.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Roebroeks, W., Sier, M. J., Nielsen, T. K., De Loecker, D., Parés, J. M., Arps, C. E. S. & Mücher, H. J. (2012) Use of red ochre by early Neandertals. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences USA 109(6):1889–94.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Sterelny, K. & Hiscock, P. (2014) Symbols, signals, and the archaeological record. Biological Theory 9:13.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Stout, D. (2012) Stone toolmaking and the evolution of human culture and cognition. In: Culture evolves, ed. Whitten, A., Hinde, R. A., Stringer, C. B. & Laland, K. N., pp. 197–14. Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Whiten, A. & Erdal, D. (2012) The human socio-cognitive niche and its evolutionary origins. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society, B 367:2119–29.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed