Hostname: page-component-586b7cd67f-tf8b9 Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-11-28T03:31:37.446Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Eye movements in reading: Models and data

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  29 March 2004

Keith Rayner*
Affiliation:
Department of Psychology, University of Massachusetts, Amherst, Amherst, MA01003
Alexander Pollatsek*
Affiliation:
Department of Psychology, University of Massachusetts, Amherst, Amherst, MA01003
Erik D. Reichle*
Affiliation:
Department of Psychology, University of Pittsburgh, Pittsburgh, PA15260

Abstract:

The issues the commentators have raised and which we address, include: the debate over how attention is allocated during reading; our distinction between early and late stages of lexical processing; our assumptions about saccadic programming; the determinants of skipping and refixations; and the role that higher-level linguistic processing may play in influencing eye movements during reading. In addition, we provide a discussion of model development and principles for evaluating and comparing models. Although we acknowledge that E-Z Reader is incomplete, we maintain that it provides a good framework for systematically trying to understand how the cognitive, perceptual, and motor systems influence the eyes during reading.

Type
Authors' Response
Copyright
Copyright © Cambridge University Press 2003

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

Altarriba, J., Kambe, G., Pollatsek, A. & Rayner, K. (2001) Semantic codes are not used in integrating information across eye fixations in reading: Evidence from fluent Spanish-English bilinguals. Perception and Psychophysics 63:875890. [aEDR]Google Scholar
Altarriba, J., Kroll, J. F., Sholl, A. & Rayner, K. (1996) The influence of lexical and conceptual constraints on reading mixed language sentences: Evidence from eye fixation and naming times. Memory and Cognition 24:477–92. [aEDR]Google Scholar
Anderson, J. R. (1983) The architecture of cognition. Harvard University Press. [aEDR]Google Scholar
Anderson, J. R. & Lebiere, C. (1998) The atomic components of thought. Erlbaum. [aEDR]Google Scholar
Andrews, S. (1997) The role of orthographic similarity in lexical retrieval: Resolving neighborhood conflicts. Psychonomic Bulletin and Review 4:439–61. [SA]Google Scholar
Andrews, S. & Heathcote, A. (2001) Distinguishing common and task-specific processes in lexical retrieval: A matter of some moment? Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning Memory and Cognition 27:514–44. [SA]Google Scholar
Andrews, S., Miller, B. & Rayner, K. (in press) Eye movements and morphological segmentation of compound words: There is a mouse in the mousetrap. European Journal of Cognitive Psychology. [SA, aEDR, rKR]Google Scholar
Atkinson, R. C. & Juola, J. F. (1973) Factors influencing speed and accuracy of word recognition. In: Attention and performance IV, pp. 583611, ed. Kornblum, S. Academic Press. [rKR]Google Scholar
Atkinson, R. C. & Juola, J. F. (1974) Search and decision processes in recognition memory. In: Contemporary developments in mathematical psychology: Vol. 1. Learning, memory, and thinking, ed. Krantz, D. H., Atkinson, R. C., Luce, R. D. & Suppes, P. Freeman. [rKR]Google Scholar
Balota, D. A., Pollatsek, A. & Rayner, K. (1985) The interaction of contextual constraints and parafoveal visual information in reading. Cognitive Psychology 17:364–90. [aEDR, EMR]Google Scholar
Basso, M. A. & Wurtz, R. H. (1997) Modulation of neuronal activity by target uncertainty. Nature 389(6646):1966–69. [GWM]Google Scholar
Basso, M. A. & Wurtz, R. H. (1998) Modulation of neuronal activity in superior colliculus by changes in target probability. Journal of Neuroscience 18(18):7519–34. [GWM]Google Scholar
Bavelier, D., Corina, D., Jezzard, P., Padmanabhan, S., Clark, V. P., Karni, A., Prinster, A., Braun, A., Lalwani, A., Rauschecker, J. P., Tuner, R. & Neville, H. (1997) Sentence reading: A functional MRI study at 4 Tesla. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience 9:664–86. [aEDR]Google Scholar
Beauvillain, C. & Doré, K. (1998) Orthographic codes are used in integrating information from the parafovea by the saccadic computation system. Vision Research 38:115–23. [SPL, aEDR, GU]Google Scholar
Beauvillain, C., Doré, K. & Baudouin, V. (1996) The “center of gravity” of words: Evidence for an effect of the word-initial letters. Vision Research 36:589603. [aEDR, GU]Google Scholar
Becker, W. & Jürgens, R. (1979) An analysis of the saccadic system by means of double step stimuli. Vision Research 19(9):1967–83. [GWM, KDM, aEDR, rKR, RRa]Google Scholar
Bertram, R. & Hyönä, J. (2002) Word processing and the optimal viewing position. Poster presented at the 3rd International Conference on the Mental Lexicon, Banff, Canada. [JH]Google Scholar
Bertram, R. & Hyönä, J. (2003) The length of a complex word modifies the role of morphological structure: Evidence from eye movements when reading short and long Finnish compounds. Journal of Memory and Language 48:615–34. [JH, rKR]Google Scholar
Bertram, R., Hyönä, J. & Pollatsek, A. (2003) Segmentation cues in processing Finnish compound words. (in preparation). [JH]Google Scholar
Biederman, I. (1981) On the semantics of a glance at a scene. In: Perceptual organization, pp. 213–63, ed. Kubovy, M. & Pomerantz, J. R. Erlbaum. [PDG]Google Scholar
Binder, J. R., Frost, J. A., Hammeke, T. A., Cox, R. W., Rao, S. M. & Prieto, T. (1997) Human brain language areas identified by functional magnetic resonance imaging. The Journal of Neuroscience 17:353–62. [aEDR]Google Scholar
Binder, K. S., Pollatsek, A. & Rayner, K. (1999) Extraction of information to the left of the fixated word in reading. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance 25:1162–72. [aEDR]Google Scholar
Binder, K. S. & Rayner, K. (1998) Contextual strength does not modulate the subordinate bias effect: Evidence from eye fixations and self-paced reading. Psychonomic Bulletin and Review 5:271–76. [aEDR]Google Scholar
Biscaldi, M., Fischer, B. & Hartnegg, K. (2000) Voluntary saccade control in dyslexia. Perception 29:509–21. [BF]Google Scholar
Blanchard, H. E., McConkie, G. W., Zola, D. & Wolverton, G. S. (1984) Time course of visual information utilization during fixations in reading. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance 10:7589. [GWM]Google Scholar
Blanchard, H. E., Pollatsek, A. & Rayner, K. (1989) The acquisition of parafoveal word information in reading. Perception and Psychophysics 46:8594. [aEDR]Google Scholar
Bookheimer, S. Y., Zeffiro, T. A., Blaxton, T., Malow, B. A., Gaillard, W. D., Sato, S., Kufta, C., Fedio, P. & Theodore, W. H. (1997) A direct comparison of PET activation and electrocortical stimulation mapping for language localization. Neurology 48:1056–65. [aEDR]Google Scholar
Borowsky, R. & Besner, D. (1993) Visual word recognition: A multi-stage activation model. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition 19:815–40. [EMR, rKR]Google Scholar
Bouma, H. & deVoogd, A. H. (1974) On the control of eye saccades in reading. Vision Research 14:272–84. [aEDR]Google Scholar
Boutsen, L., Lamberts, K. & Verfaillie, K. (1998) Recognition times of different views of 56 depth-rotated objects: A note concerning Verfaillie and Boutsen (1995). Perception and Psychophysics 60:900907. [PDG]Google Scholar
Briihl, D. & Inhoff, A. W. (1995) Integrating information across fixations during reading: The use of orthographic bodies and of exterior letters. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition 21:5567. [AWI]Google Scholar
Broadbent, D. E. (1971) Decision and stress. Academic Press. [CB]Google Scholar
Brown, P. (1991) DEREK: The direct encoding routine for evoking knowledge. Basic processing in reading: Visual word recognition, ed. Besner, D. & Humphreys, G. W. Erlbaum. [aEDR]Google Scholar
Bruce, C. J. & Goldberg, M. E. (1985) Primate frontal eye fields. I. Single neurons discharging before saccades. Journal of Neurophysiology 53:603–35. [BF]Google Scholar
Brysbaert, M. & Vitu, F. (1998) Word skipping: Implications for theories of eye movement control in reading. In: Eye guidance in reading and scene perception, pp. 125–47, ed. Underwood, G. Elsevier. [MB, FV, aEDR, rKR]Google Scholar
Bullinaria, J. A. (1997) Modeling reading, spelling, and past tense learning with artificial neural networks. Brain and Language 59:236–66. [aEDR]Google Scholar
Byrne, M. D. & Anderson, J. R. (1998) Perception and action. In: The atomic components of thought, ed. Anderson, J. R. & Lebiere, C. Erlbaum. [aEDR]Google Scholar
Caplan, D. (1992) Language: Structure, processing, and disorders. MIT Press. [aEDR]Google Scholar
Caplan, D., Alpert, N. & Waters, G. (1999) PET studies of syntactic processing with auditory sentence presentation. NeuroImage 9:343–51. [aEDR]Google Scholar
Carpenter, P. A. & Just, M. A. (1983) What your eyes do while your mind is reading. In: Eye movements in reading: Perceptual and language processes, pp. 275–305, ed. Rayner, K. Academic Press. [AK, aEDR, rKR]Google Scholar
Carpenter, R. H. S. & Williams, M. L. L. (1995) Neural computation of log likelihood in control of saccadic eye movements. Nature 377:5962. [JMF]Google Scholar
Chelazzi, L., Miller, E. K., Duncan, J. & Desimone, R. (1993) A neural basis for visual search in inferior temporal cortex. Nature 363:345–47. [CB]Google Scholar
Churchland, P. S. (1986) Neurophilosophy: Toward a unified science of the mindbrain. MIT Press. [aEDR]Google Scholar
Churchland, P. S. & Sejnowski, T. J. (1992) The computational brain. MIT Press. [aEDR]Google Scholar
Clark, J. J. & O’Regan, J. K. (1999) Word ambiguity and the optimal viewing position in reading. Vision Research 39:843–57. [aEDR]Google Scholar
Cohen, L., Dehaene, S., Naccache, L., Lehéricy, S., Dehaene-Lambertz, G., Hénaff, M.-A. & Michel, F. (2000) The visual word form area: Spatial and temporal characterization of an initial stage of reading in normal subjects and posterior split-brain patients. Brain 123:291307. [aEDR]Google Scholar
Cohen, J. D., Romero, R. D., Farah, M. J. & Servan-Schreiber, D. (1994) Mechanisms of spatial attention: The relation of macrostructure to microstructure in parietal neglect. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience 6:377–87. [RS]Google Scholar
Colby, C. L., Duhamel, J.-R. & Goldberg, M. E. (1996) Visual, presaccadic, and cognitive activation of single neurons in monkey lateral intraparietal area. Journal of Neurophysiology 76:2841–52. [PDG]Google Scholar
Coltheart, M. & Rastle, K. (1999) Serial processing in reading aloud: Evidence for dual-route models of reading. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance 20:1197–211. [RS]Google Scholar
Coltheart, M., Rastle, K., Perry, C., Langdon, R. & Ziegler, J. (2001) DRC: A dual route cascaded model of visual word recognition and reading aloud. Psychological Review 108:204–56. [SA]Google Scholar
Corbetta, M. (1998) Frontoparietal cortical networks for directing attention and the eye to visual locations: Identical, independent, or overlapping neural systems? Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences USA 95:831–38. [aEDR]Google Scholar
Corbetta, M., Akbudak, E., Conturo, T. E., Snyder, A. Z., Ollinger, J. L., Drury, H. A., Linenweber, M. R., Petersen, S. E., Raichle, M. E., Van Essen, D. C. & Shulman, G. L. (1998) A common network of functional areas for attention and eye movements. Neuron 21:761–73. [CB]Google Scholar
Corbetta, M., Kincade, J. M., Ollinger, J. M., McAvoy, J. M. & Shulman, G. L. (2000) Voluntary orienting is dissociated from target detection in human posterior parietal cortex. Nature Neuroscience 3(3):292–97. [CB]Google Scholar
Corbetta, M., Miezin, F. M., Schulman, G. L. & Petersen, S. E. (1993) A PET study of visuospatial attention. Journal of Neuroscience 13:1202–26. [aEDR]Google Scholar
Corbetta, M. & Shulman, G. L. (2002) Control of goal-driven and stimulus driven attention in the brain. Nature Reviews Neuroscience 3:201–15. [CB]Google Scholar
Davachi, L., Mitchell, J. P. & Wagner, A. D. (2003) Multiple routes to memory: Distinct medial temporal lobe processes build item and source memories. Proceedings of the National Academy of Science USA 100:(2):157–62. [rKR]Google Scholar
De Graef, P. (1998) Prefixational object perception in scenes: Objects popping out of schemas. In: Eye guidance in reading and scene perception, pp. 315–38, ed. Underwood, G. Elsevier. [PDG]Google Scholar
De Graef, P., Christiaens, D. & d’Ydewalle, G. (1990) Perceptual effects of scene context on object identification. Psychological Research 52:317–29. [PDG]Google Scholar
Den Buurman, R., Boersma, T. & Gerrissen, J. F. (1981) Eye movements and the perceptual span in reading. Reading Research Quarterly 16:227–35. [aEDR]Google Scholar
Deubel, H., O’Regan, K. & Radach, R. (2000) Attention, information processing and eye movement control. In: Reading as a perceptual process, ed. Kennedy, A., Radach, R., Heller, D. & Pynte, J. Elsevier. [RRa]Google Scholar
Deubel, H. & Schneider, W. X. (1996) Saccade target selection and object recognition: Evidence for a common attentional mechanism. Vision Research 36:(1)827–37. [CB, PDG]Google Scholar
Deutsch, A., Frost, R., Peleg, S., Pollatsek, A. & Rayner, K. (in press) Early morphological effects in reading: Evidence from parafoveal preview benefit in Hebrew. Psychonomic Bulletin and Review. 10:415–22. [aEDR]Google Scholar
Deutsch, A., Frost, R., Pollatsek, A. & Rayner, K. (2000) Early morphological effects in word recognition in Hebrew: Evidence from parafoveal preview benefits. Language and Cognitive Processes 15:487506. [aEDR]Google Scholar
Deutsch, A. & Rayner, K. (1999) Initial fixation location effects in reading Hebrew words. Language and Cognitive Processes 14:393421. [aEDR]Google Scholar
Dopkins, S., Morris, R. K. & Rayner, K. (1992) Lexical ambiguity and eye fixations during reading: A test of competing models of lexical ambiguity resolution. Journal of Memory and Language 31:461–77. [aEDR]Google Scholar
Doré-Mazars, K., Collins, T. & Vergilino-Perez, D. (2003) About the effects of word-length and word-frequency on the decision and metrics of refixations in long words. Psychological Science (submitted). [KD-M]Google Scholar
Doré-Mazars, K., Pouget, P. & Beauvillain, C. (in press) Attentional selection during the preparation of eye movements. Psychological Research. [CB]Google Scholar
Duffy, S. A., Kambe, G. & Rayner, K. (2001) The effect of prior disambiguating context on the comprehension of ambiguous words: Evidence from eye movements. In: On the consequences of meaning selection: Perspectives of resolving lexical ambiguity, ed. Gorfein, D. American Psychological Association. [aEDR]Google Scholar
Duffy, S. A., Morris, R. K. & Rayner, K. (1988) Lexical ambiguity and fixation times in reading. Journal of Memory and Language 27:429–46. [aEDR, rKR]Google Scholar
Duncan, J., Ward, W. & Shapiro, K. (1994) Direct measurement of attentional dwell time in human vision. Nature 369:313–15. [RRa]Google Scholar
Ehrlich, S. F. & Rayner, K. (1981) Contextual effects on word perception and eye movements during reading. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior 20:641–55. [aEDR, rKR]Google Scholar
Engbert, R. & Kliegl, R. (2001) Mathematical models of eye movements in reading: A possible role for autonomous saccades. Biological Cybernetics 85:7787. [RE, GWM, aEDR, RRe]Google Scholar
Engbert, R. & Kliegl, R. (2003) Noise-enhanced performance in reading. Neurocomputing 50:473–78. [RE]Google Scholar
Engbert, R., Longtin, A. & Kliegl, R. (2002) A dynamic model of saccade generation in reading based on spatially distributed lexical processing. Vision Research 42(5):621–36. [AK, RE, LH, GWM, MP, aEDR, rKR]Google Scholar
Engbert, R., Longtin, A. & Kliegl, R. (2004) Complexity of eye movements in reading. International Journal of Bifurcation and Chaos 14. (in press). [RE]Google Scholar
Engbert, R., & Trukenbrad, H. (2003) Modeling eye movements in visual search. Poster presented at the 12th European Conference on Eye Movements, Dundee, Scotland, 2003. {rkR}Google Scholar
Epelboim, J., Booth, J. R. & Steinman, R. M. (1993) Reading unspaced text: Implications for theories of eye movements. Vision Research 34:1735–66. [AK]Google Scholar
Epelboim, J., Booth, J. R. & Steinman, R. M. (1996) Much ado about nothing: The place of space in text. Vision Research 36:465–70. [AK]Google Scholar
Erdmann, B. & Dodge, R. (1898) Psychologische Untersuchungen uber das Lesen. N. Niemeyer. [aEDR]Google Scholar
Erlhagen, W. & Schöner, G. (2002) Dynamic field theory of movement preparation. Psychological Review 109:545–72. [RE]Google Scholar
Everatt, J., Bradshaw, M. F. & Hibbard, P. B. (1998) Individual differences in reading and eye movement control. In: Eye guidance in reading and scene perception, pp. 223–42, ed. Underwood, G. Elsevier. [GU]Google Scholar
Everatt, J. & Underwood, G. (1992) Parafoveal guidance and priming effects during reading: A special case of the mind being ahead of the eyes. Consciousness and Cognition 1:186–97. [GU]Google Scholar
Felleman, D. J. & Van Essen, D. C. (1991) Distributed hierarchical processing in the primate cerebral cortex. Cerebral Cortex 1:147. [aEDR]Google Scholar
Feng, G. (2001) SHARE: A Stochastic Hierarchical Architecture for Reading Eyemovement, Department of Psychology, University of Illinois, Champaign-Urbana. (Unpublished doctoral dissertation). Available at: www.garyfeng.com/dissertation/Feng_2001_dissertation.pdf [GF]Google Scholar
Feng, G., Miller, K. & Shu, H. (2003) Reading development in English and Chinese: Results from an eye-movement study. (submitted). [GF]Google Scholar
Feng, G., Miller, K. F., Zhang, H. & Shu, H. (2001) Rowed to recovery: The use of phonological and orthographic information in reading Chinese and English. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition 27:1079–100. [GF]Google Scholar
Ferreira, F. & Clifton, C. Jr. (1986) The independence of syntactic processing. Journal of Memory and Language 25:348–68. [AK]Google Scholar
Findlay, J. M. & Walker, R. (1999) A model of saccadic eye movement generation based on parallel processing and competitive inhibition. Behavioral and Brain Sciences 22:661721. Also available online: http://psychology.dur.ac.uk/staff/pubs/JMF-1999b.pdf. [CB, JMF, aEDR]Google Scholar
Fischer, B. (1987) The preparation of visually guided saccades. Reviews of Physiology, Biochemistry, and Pharmacology 106:135. [BF]Google Scholar
Fischer, B., Biscaldi, M. & Gezeck, S. (1997a) On the development of voluntary and reflexive components in human saccade generation. Brain Research 754:285–97. [BF]Google Scholar
Fischer, B., Gezeck, S. & Hartnegg, K. (1997b) The analysis of saccadic eye movements from gap and overlap paradigms. Brain Research Protocols 2:4752. [BF]Google Scholar
Fischer, B. & Hartnegg, K. (2000a) Effects of visual training on saccade control in dyslexia. Perception 29:531–42. [BF]Google Scholar
Fischer, B. & Hartnegg, K. (2000b) Stability of gaze control in dyslexia. Strabismus 8:119–22. [BF]Google Scholar
Fischer, B., Hartnegg, K. & Mokler, A. (2000) Dynamic visual perception of dyslexic children. Perception 29:523–30. [BF]Google Scholar
Fischer, B. & Ramsperger, E. (1984) Human express saccades: Extremely short reaction times of goal directed eye movements. Experimental Brain Research 57:191–95. [GWM]Google Scholar
Fischer, B. & Ramsperger, E. (1986) Human express saccades: Effects of randomization and daily practice. Experimental Brain Research 64:569–78. [BF]Google Scholar
Francis, W. N. & Kučera, H. (1982) Frequency analysis of English usage: Lexicon and grammar. Houghton Mifflin. [aEDR]Google Scholar
Frazier, L. & Rayner, K. (1982) Making and correcting errors during sentence comprehension: Eye movements in the analysis of structurally ambiguous sentences. Cognitive Psychology 14:178210. [GER, aEDR, rKR]Google Scholar
Frisson, S. & Pickering, M. J. (1999) The processing of metonymy: Evidence from eye movements. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition 25:1366–83. [rKR]Google Scholar
Gautier, V., O’Regan, J. K. & LaGargasson, J. F. (2000) ‘The skipping’ revisited in French: Programming saccades to skip the article ‘les’. Vision Research 40:2517–31. [aEDR]Google Scholar
Garrod, S. & Terras, M. (2000) The contribution of lexical and situational knowledge to resolving discourse roles: Bonding and resolution. Journal of Memory and Language 42:526–44. [rKR]Google Scholar
Germeys, F., De Graef, P. & Verfaillie, K. (2002) Transsaccadic identification of saccade target and flanker objects. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance 28:868–83. [PDG]Google Scholar
Goldberg, M. E. (1994) The cortical control of saccadic eye movements. In: Eye movements in reading, ed. Ygge, J. & Lennerstrand, G. Pergamon Press. [aEDR]Google Scholar
Goodale, M. A. & Milner, A. D. (1992) Separate visual pathways for perception and action. Trends in Neurosciences 15:2025. [aEDR]Google Scholar
Grainger, J. (2003) Moving eyes and reading words: How can a computational model combine these two? In: The mind's eye: Cognition and applied aspects of eye movement research, ed. Hyönä, J., Radach, R. & Deubel, H. Elsevier. [LH]Google Scholar
Grainger, J. & Jacobs, A. M. (1996) Orthographic processing in visual word recognition: A multiple read-out model. Psychological Review 103(3):518–65. [SA, LH]Google Scholar
Grainger, J. & Jacobs, A. M. (1998) On localist connectionism and psychological science. In: Localist connectionistic approaches to human cognition, pp. 138, ed. Grainger, J. & Jacobs, A. M. Erlbaum. [LH]Google Scholar
Grill-Spector, K., Kushnir, T., Hendler, T., Edelman, S., Itzchak, Y. & Malach, R. (1998) A sequence of object-processing stages revealed by fMRI in the human occipital lobe. Human Brain Mapping 6:316–28. [aEDR]Google Scholar
Guitton, D., Buchtel, H. A. & Douglas, R. M. (1985) Frontal lobe lesions in man cause difficulties in suppressing reflexive glances and in generating goaldirected saccades. Experimental Brain Research 58:455–72. [BF]Google Scholar
Hallett, P. (1978) Primary and secondary saccades to goals defined by instructions. Vision Research 18:1279–96. [BF]Google Scholar
Hays, W. L. (1988) Statistics, 4th edition. Holt, Rinehart & Winston. [GF]Google Scholar
Heinze, H. J., Mangun, G. R., Burchert, W., Hinrichs, H., Scholz, M., Munte, T. F., Gos, A., Scherg, M., Johannes, S., Hundeshagen, H., Gazzaniga, M. S. & Hillyard, S. A. (1994) Combined spatial and temporal imaging of brain activity during visual selective attention in humans. Nature 372:543–46. [CB]Google Scholar
Heller, D. & Radach, R. (1999) Eye movements in reading: Are two eyes better than one? In: Current oculomotor research: Physiological and psychological aspects, ed. Becker, W., Deubel, H. & Mergner, T. Plenum Press. [RS]Google Scholar
Henderson, J. M. & Ferreira, F. (1990) Effects of foveal processing difficulty on the perceptual span in reading: Implications for attention and eye movement control. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition 16:417–29. [JP, EMR, aEDR, GU]Google Scholar
Henderson, J. M. & Ferreira, F. (1993) Eye movement control during reading: Fixation measures reflect foveal but not parafoveal processing difficulty. Canadian Journal of Experimental Psychology 47:201–21. [AK, aEDR, rKR]Google Scholar
Henderson, J. M., Weeks, P. A. Jr. & Hollingworth, A. (1999) The effects of semantic consistency on eye movements during complex scene viewing. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance 25(1):210–28. [CB, PDG]Google Scholar
Herdman, C. M., Chernecki, D. & Norris, D. (1999) Naming case alternated words. Memory and Cognition 27:254–66. [EMR]Google Scholar
Hillyard, S. A. & Anllo-Vento, L. (1998) Event-related brain potentials of visual selective attention. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences USA 95:781–87. [CB]Google Scholar
Hintzman, D. L. (1984) MINERVA 2: A simulation model of human memory. Behavior Research Methods, Instruments and Computers 16:96101. [rKR]Google Scholar
Hintzman, D. L. (1986) “Schema abstraction” in a multiple-trace memory model. Psychological Review 93:411–28. [rKR]Google Scholar
Hintzman, D. L. (1987) Recognition and recall in MINERVA 2: Analysis of the “recognitionfailure” paradigm. In: Modeling cognition, pp. 215–29, ed. Morris, P. Wiley. [rKR]Google Scholar
Hintzman, D. L. (1988) Judgments of frequency and recognition memory in a multiple-trace memory model. Psychological Review 95:528–51. [rKR]Google Scholar
Hintzman, D. L. (1990) Human learning and memory: Connections and dissociations. Annual Review of Psychology 41:109–39. [rKR]Google Scholar
Hintzman, D. L. (1991) Why are formal models useful in psychology? In: Relating theory and data: Essays on human memory in honor of Bennet B. Murdock, ed. Hockley, W. E. & Lewandowsky, S. Erlbaum. [aEDR]Google Scholar
Hoffman, J. E. & Subramaniam, B (1995) The role of visual attention in saccadic eye movements. Perception and Psychophysics 57:787–95. [CB, aEDR]Google Scholar
Horwitz, B., Rumsey, J. M. & Donohue, B. C. (1998) Functional connectivity of the angular gyrus in normal reading and dyslexia. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences USA 95:8939–44. [aEDR]Google Scholar
Huey, E. B. (1908) The psychology and pedagogy of reading. Macmillan. [aEDR, rKR]Google Scholar
Humphreys, M. S., Bain, J. D. & Pike, R. (1989) Different ways to cue a coherent memory system: A theory for episodic, semantic, and procedural tasks. Psychological Review 96:208–33. [SA]Google Scholar
Hyönä, J. (1995a) An eye movement analysis of topic-shift effect during repeated reading. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition 21:1365–73. [SPL]Google Scholar
Hyönä, J. (1995b) Do irregular letter combinations attract readers’ attention? Evidence from fixation locations in words. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance 2:6881. [aEDR, GU]Google Scholar
Hyönä, J. (2000) Processing of Finnish compound words in reading. In: Reading as a perceptual process, ed. Kennedy, A., Radach, R., Heller, D., & Pynte, J. Elsevier. [aEDR]Google Scholar
Hyönä, J. & Bertram, R. (in press) Do frequency characteristics of non-fixated words influence the processing of fixated words during reading? European Journal of Cognitive Psychology. [rKR]Google Scholar
Hyönä, J., Niemi, P. & Underwood, G. (1989) Reading long words embedded in sentences: Informativeness of word halves affects eye movements. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance 15:142–52. [aEDR, GU]Google Scholar
Hyönä, J. & Olson, R. K. (1995) Eye movement patterns among dyslexic and normal readers: Effects of word length and word frequency. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition 21:1430–40. [aEDR]Google Scholar
Hyönä, J. & Pollatsek, A. (1998) Reading Finnish compound words: Eye fixations are affected by component morphemes. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance 24:1612–27. [JP, aEDR]Google Scholar
Hyönä, J. & Pollatsek, A. (1999) The role of component morphemes on eye fixations when reading Finnish compound words. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance 24:1612–27. [JH, rKR]Google Scholar
Ikeda, M. & Saida, S. (1978) Span of recognition in reading. Vision Research 18:8388. [aEDR]Google Scholar
Inhoff, A. W. (1984) Two stages of word processing during eye fixations in the reading of prose. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior 23:612–24. [aEDR]Google Scholar
Inhoff, A. W. (1989a) Lexical access during eye fixations in reading: Are word access codes used to integrate lexical information across interword fixations? Journal of Memory and Language 28:444–61. [AWI]Google Scholar
Inhoff, A. W. (1989b) Parafoveal processing of words and saccade computation during eye fixations in reading. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance 15:544–55. [AWI]Google Scholar
Inhoff, A. W. (1990) Integrating information across eye fixations in reading. Acta Psychologica 73:281–97. [AWI]Google Scholar
Inhoff, A. W. & Briihl, D. (1991) Semantic processing of unattended text during selective reading: How the eyes see it. Perception and Psychophysics 49:289–94. [aEDR]Google Scholar
Inhoff, A. W., Briihl, D. & Schwartz, J. (1996) Compound word naming in reading, on-line naming and delayed naming tasks. Memory and Cognition 24:466–76. [GU]Google Scholar
Inhoff, A. W. & Liu, W. (1998) The perceptual span and oculomotor activity during the reading of Chinese sentences. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance 24:2034. [aEDR]Google Scholar
Inhoff, A. W., Pollatsek, A., Posner, M. I. & Rayner, K. (1989) Covert attention and eye movements during reading. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology 41A:6389. [aEDR]Google Scholar
Inhoff, A. W. & Radach, R. (1998) Definition and computation of oculomotor measures in the study of cognitive processes. In: Eye guidance in reading and scene perception, ed. Underwood, G. Elsevier. pp. 128 cited by [PDG]; pp. 29–53 by [GF, aEDR]Google Scholar
Inhoff, A. W., Radach, R., Eiter, B. & Juhasz, B. (2003) Distinct subsystems for the parafoveal processing of spatial and linguistic information during eye fixations in reading. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Experimental Psychology 56A:803–28. [AWI]Google Scholar
Inhoff, A. W., Radach, R., Starr, M. S. & Greenberg, S. (2000a) Allocation of visuospatial attention and saccade programming during reading. In: Reading as a perceptual process, ed. Kennedy, A., Radach, R. Heller, D. & Pynte, J. Elsevier. [aEDR, rKR]Google Scholar
Inhoff, A. W. & Rayner, K. (1986) Parafoveal word processing during eye fixations in reading: Effects of word frequency. Perception and Psychophysics 40(6):431–39. [KDM, aEDR, rKR]Google Scholar
Inhoff, A. W., Starr, M. & Shindler, K. L. (2000b) Is the processing of words during eye fixations in reading strictly serial? Perception and Psychophysics 62(7):1474–84. [CB, AWI, MP, aEDR]Google Scholar
Inhoff, A. W. & Topolski, R. (1992) Lack of semantic activation from unattended text during passage reading. Bulletin of the Psychonomic Society 30:365–66. [aEDR]Google Scholar
Irwin, D. E. (1998) Lexical processing during saccadic eye movements. Cognitive Psychology 36:127. [aEDR]Google Scholar
Ishai, A., Ungerleider, L. G., Martin, A., Schouten, J. L. & Haxby, J. V. (1999) Distributed representation of objects in the human ventral visual pathway. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences USA 96:9379–84. [aEDR]Google Scholar
Ishida, T. & Ikeda, M. (1989) Temporal properties of information extraction in reading studied by a text-mask replacement technique. Journal of the Optical Society A: Optics and Image Sciences 6:1624–32. [aEDR, rKR]Google Scholar
Itti, L. & Koch, C. (2000) A saliency-based search mechanism for overt and covert shifts of visual attention. Vision Research 40:1489–506. [CB]Google Scholar
Jacobs, A. M. & Grainger, J. (1994) Models of visual word recognition – sampling the state of the art. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance 20:1311–34. [rKR]Google Scholar
Juhasz, B., Starr, M. S., Inhoff, A. W. & Placke, L. (2003) The effects of morphology on the processing of compound words: Evidence from naming, lexical decision, and eye fixations. British Journal of Psychology 94:233–44. [aEDR, rKR]Google Scholar
Just, M. A. & Carpenter, P. A. (1980) A theory of reading: From eye fixations to comprehension. Psychological Review 87:329–54. [aEDR]Google Scholar
Just, M. A. & Carpenter, P. A. (1987) The psychology of reading and language comprehension. Allyn and Bacon. [aEDR]Google Scholar
Just, M. A. & Carpenter, P. A. (1992) A capacity theory of comprehension: Individual differences in working memory. Psychological Review 99:122–49. [aEDR]Google Scholar
Kajii, N., Nazir, T. A. & Osaka, N. (2001) Eye movement control in reading unspaced text: The case of the Japanese script. Vision Research 41:2503–10. [NO]Google Scholar
Kambe, G. (in press) Parafoveal processing of prefixed words during eye fixations in reading: Evidence against morphological influences on parafoveal preprocessing. Perception and Psychophysics. [aEDR]Google Scholar
Kambe, G., Rayner, K. & Duffy, S. A. (2001) Global context effects on processing lexically ambiguous words: Evidence from eye fixations. Memory and Cognition 29:363–72. [aEDR]Google Scholar
Kennedy, A. (1998) The influence of parafoveal words on foveal inspection time: Evidence for a processing trade-off. In: Eye guidance in reading and scene perception, ed. Underwood, G. Elsevier. [AK, aEDR]Google Scholar
Kennedy, A. (2000a) Attention allocation in reading. In: Reading as a perceptual process, ed. Kennedy, A., Radach, R., Heller, D. & Pynte, J. Elsevier. [AK]Google Scholar
Kennedy, A. (2000b) Parafoveal processing in word recognition. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology 53A:429–55. [AK]Google Scholar
Kennedy, A., Brooks, R., Flynn, L. & Prophet, C. (2003) The reader's spatial code. In: The mind's eye: Cognitive and applied aspects of eye movement research, ed. Hyönä, J., Radach, R. & Deubel, H. Elsevier. [AK, MP]Google Scholar
Kennedy, A. & Murray, W. S. (1987) Spatial coding and reading: Some comments on Monk (1985). Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology 39A:649718. [AK]Google Scholar
Kennedy, A., Murray, W. S. & Boissiere, C. (in press) Parafoveal pragmatics revisited. European Journal of Cognitive Psychology. [WSM]Google Scholar
Kennedy, A., Pynte, J. & Ducrot, S. (2002) Parafoveal-on-foveal interactions in word recognition. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology 55A(4):1307–38. [CB, AK, aEDR, rKR]Google Scholar
Kennedy, A., Radach, R., Heller, D. & Pynte, J. (2000) Reading as a perceptual process. Elsevier. [aEDR]Google Scholar
Kennison, S. M. & Clifton, C. (1995) Determinants of parafoveal perview benefit in high and low working memory capacity readers: Implications for eye movement control. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition 21:6881. [aEDR, EMR]Google Scholar
Kim, Y.-H., Gitelman, D. R., Nobre, A. C., Parrish, T. B., LaBar, K. S. & Mesulam, M. M. (1999) The large-scale neural network for spatial attention displays multifunctional overlap but differential asymmetry. NeuroImage 9:269–77. [aEDR]Google Scholar
Kinsbourne, M. (1970) A model for the mechanism of unilateral neglect of space. Transactions of the American Neurological Association 95:143–46. [RS]Google Scholar
Klein, C., Fischer, B., Hartnegg, K., Heiss, W. H. & Roth, M. (2000) Optomotor and neuropsychological performance in old age. Experimental Brain Research 135:141–54. [BF]Google Scholar
Klein, C., Fischer, B., Hartnegg, K., Heiss, W. H. & Roth, M. (1977) Hemineglect and hemispheric rivalry. Advances in Neurology 18:4149. [RS]Google Scholar
Kliegl, R. & Engbert, R. (2003) SWIFT explorations: In: The mind's eye: Cognitive and applied aspects of eye movement research, pp. 391411, ed. Hyönä, J., Radach, R. & Deubel, H. Elsevier. [RE, aEDR, rKR]Google Scholar
Kliegl, R., Grabner, E., Rolfs, M. & Engbert, R. (in press) Length, frequency, and predictablity effects of word on eye movements in reading. European Journal of Cognitive Psychology.Google Scholar
Kliegl, R. M., Olson, R. K. & Davidson, B. J. (1982) Regression analyses as a tool for studying reading processes: Comment on Just and Carpenter's eye fixation theory. Memory and Cognition 10:287–96. [GF]Google Scholar
Klitz, T. S., Legge, G. E. & Tjan, B. S. (2000) Saccade planning in reading with central scotomas: Comparison of human and ideal performance. In: Reading as a perceptual process, ed. Kennedy, A., Radach, R., Heller, D. & Pynte, J. Elsevier. [aEDR]Google Scholar
Kolers, P. A. (1976) Buswell's discoveries. In: Eye movements and psychological processes, ed. Monty, R. A. & Senders, J. W. Erlbaum. [aEDR]Google Scholar
LaBerge, D. L. (1990) Attention. Psychological Science 1:156–62. [aEDR]Google Scholar
Lamme, V. A. F. & Roelfsema, P. R. (2000) The distinct modes of vision offered by feedforward and recurrent processing. Trends in Neurosciences 23:571–79. [aEDR]Google Scholar
Lavigne, F., Vitu, F. & d’Ydewalle, G. (2000) The influence of semantic context on initial eye landing sites in words. Acta Psychologia 104:191204. [aEDR, FV]Google Scholar
Lee, H., Rayner, K. & Pollatsek, A. (1999) The time course of phonological, semantic, and orthographic coding in reading: Evidence from the fast-priming technique. Psychonomic Bulletin and Review 6:624–34. [rKR]Google Scholar
Leff, A. P., Scott, S. K., Rothwell, J. C. & Wise, R. J. S. (2001) The planning and guiding of reading saccades: A repetitive transcranial magnetic stimulation study. Cerebral Cortex 11:918–23. [aEDR]Google Scholar
Legge, G. E., Klitz, T. S. & Tjan, B. S. (1997) Mr. Chips: An ideal-observer model of reading. Psychological Review 104:524–53. [aEDR]Google Scholar
Leigh, R. J. & Zee, D. S. (1999) The neurology of eye movements. Oxford University Press. [aEDR]Google Scholar
Lima, S. D. (1987) Morphological analysis in sentence reading. Journal of Memory and Language 26:8499. [aEDR]Google Scholar
Lima, S. D. & Inhoff, A. W. (1985) Lexical access during eye fixations in reading: Effects of word-initial letter sequences. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance 11:272–85. [AWI,aEDR]Google Scholar
Liversedge, S. P. & Findlay, J. M. (2000) Saccadic eye movements and cognition. Trends in Cognitive Science 4:614. [aEDR]Google Scholar
Liversedge, S. & Underwood, G. (1998) Foveal processing load and landing position effects in reading. In: Eye guidance in reading and scene perception, pp. 201–21, ed. Underwood, G. Elsevier. [GU]Google Scholar
Luna, B., Thulborn, K. R., Strojwas, M. H., McCurtain, B. J., Berman, R. A., Genovese, C. R. & Sweeney, J. A. (1998) Dorsal cortical regions subserving visually guided saccades in humans: An fMRI study. Cerebral Cortex 8:4047. [aEDR]Google Scholar
MacWhinney, B. (1995) The CHILDES project: Tools for analyzing talk, 2nd edition. Erlbaum. [RRe]Google Scholar
Mandler, G. (1980) Recognizing: The judgement of previous occurrence. Psychological Review 87:252–71. [rKR]Google Scholar
Mangun, G. R., Hopfinger, J., Kussmaul, C. L., Flechter, E. & Heinze, H. J. (1997) Covariations in ERP and PET measures of spatial selective attention in human extrastriate cortex. Human Brain Mapping 5:273–79. [CB]Google Scholar
Martinez, A., DiRusso, F., Anllo-Vento, L., Sereno, M. L., Buxton, R. B. & Hillyard, S. A. (2001) Putting spatial attention on the map: Timing and localization of stimulus selection processes in striate and extrastriate visual areas. Vision Research 41:1437–57. [CB]Google Scholar
Maunsell, J. H. R. & Newsome, W. T. (1987) Visual processing in monkey extrastriate cortex. Annual Review of Neuroscience 10:363401. [aEDR]Google Scholar
Mayall, K. A., Humphreys, G. W., Mechelli, A., Olson, A. & Price, C. J. (2001) The effects of case mixing on word recognition: Evidence from a PET study. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience 13:844–53. [EMR]Google Scholar
Mayall, K. A., Humphreys, G. W. & Olson, A. (1997) Disruption to word or letter processing? The origins of case mixing effects. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory and Cognition 23:1275–86. [EMR]Google Scholar
McClelland, J. L. (1986) The programmable blackboard model of reading. In: Parallel distributed: Explorations in the microstructure of cognition, vol. 2: Psychological and biological models, ed. McClelland, J. L Rumelhart, D. E. & the PDP Research Group. MIT Press. [aEDR]Google Scholar
McClelland, J. L. & Rumelhart, D. E. (1981) An interactive activation model of context effects in letter perception: Part I. An account of basic findings. Psychological Review 88:375407. [CB, GER, aEDR]Google Scholar
McClelland, J. L., Rumelhart, D. E. & the PDP Research Group (1986) Parallel distributed: Explorations in the microstructure of cognition, vol. 2: Psychological and biological models. MIT Press. [aEDR]Google Scholar
McConkie, G. W. (1979) On the role and control of eye movements in reading. In: Processing of visible language: Vol. 1, pp. 3748, ed. Kolers, P. A., Wrolstad, M. E. & Bouma, H. Plenum Press. [AK, aEDR]Google Scholar
McConkie, G. W. & Dyre, B. P. (2000) Eye fixation durations in reading: Models of frequency distributions. In: Reading as a perceptual process, ed. Kennedy, A., Radach, R., Heller, D. & Pynte, J. Elsevier. [GF, aEDR]Google Scholar
McConkie, G. W., Kerr, P. W. & Dyre, B. P. (1994) What are “normal” eye movements during reading: Toward a mathematical description. In: Eye movements in reading, ed. Ygge, J. & Lennerstrand, G. Pergamon Press. [aEDR]Google Scholar
McConkie, G. W., Kerr, P. W., Reddix, M. D. & Zola, D. (1988) Eye movement control during reading: I. The location of initial eye fixations on words. Vision Research 28:1107–18. [aEDR, rKR]Google Scholar
McConkie, G. W., Kerr, P. W., Reddix, M. D., Zola, D. & Jacobs, A. M. (1989) Eye movement control during reading: II. Frequency of refixating a word. Perception and Psychophysics 46:245–53. [KDM, JP, aEDR]Google Scholar
McConkie, G. W. & Rayner, K. (1975) The span of the effective stimulus during a fixation in reading. Perception and Psychophysics 17:578–86. [NO, aEDR, rKR]Google Scholar
McConkie, G. W. & Rayner, K. (1976) Asymmetry of the perceptual span in reading. Bulletin of the Psychonomic Society 8:365–68. [aEDR]Google Scholar
McConkie, G. W. & Yang, S-N. (2003) How cognition affects eye movements during reading. In: The mind's eye: Cognitive and applied aspects of eye movement research, ed. Hyönä, J., Radach, R. & Deubel, H. Elsevier. [GWM]Google Scholar
McConkie, G. W. & Zola, D. (1987) Visual attention during eye fixations while reading. In: Attention and performance XII: The psychology of reading, pp. 385401, ed. Coltheart, M. Erlbaum. [AK]Google Scholar
McConkie, G. W. & Zola, D. (1979) Is visual information integrated across successive fixations in reading? Perception and Psychophysics 25:211–24. [aEDR]Google Scholar
McConkie, G. W. & Zola, D. (1984) Eye movement control during reading: The effect of words units. In: Cognition and motor processes, ed. Prinz, W. & Sanders, A. F. Springer-Verlag. [aEDR]Google Scholar
McConkie, G. W., Zola, D., Grimes, J., Kerr, P. W., Bryant, N. R. & Wolff, P. M. (1991) Children's eye movements during reading. In: Vision and visual dyslexia, ed. Stein, J. F. Macmillan Press. [aEDR, rKR]Google Scholar
McPeek, R. M., Skavenski, A. A. & Nakayama, K. (2000) Concurrent processing of saccades in visual search. Vision Research 40:2499–516. [aEDR]Google Scholar
Meseguer, E., Carreiras, M. & Clifton, C. J. (2002) Overt reanalysis strategies and eye movements during reading on mild garden sentences. Memory and Cognition 30:551–61. [MP, rKR]Google Scholar
Mesulam, M. M. (1990) Large-scale neurocognitive networks and distributed processing for attention, language, and memory. Annals of Neurology 28:597613. [aEDR]Google Scholar
Mesulam, M. M. (1998) From sensation to cognition. Brain 121:1013–52. [aEDR]Google Scholar
Molker, A. & Fischer, B. (1999) The recognition and correction of involuntary prosaccades in an antisaccade task. Experimental Brain Research 125:551–16. [aEDR]Google Scholar
Monaghan, P., Shillcock, R. & McDonald, S. (2003) Hemispheric asymmetries in the split-fovea model of semantic processing. Brain and Language 86:8398. [RS]Google Scholar
Morris, R. K. (1994) Lexical and message-level sentence context effects on fixation times in reading. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition 20:92103. [GER]Google Scholar
Morris, R. K., Rayner, K. & Pollatsek, A. (1990) Eye movement guidance in reading: The role of parafoveal letter and space information. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance 16:268–81. [aEDR, rKR]Google Scholar
Morrison, R. E. (1984) Manipulation of stimulus onset delay in reading: Evidence for parallel programming of saccades. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance 10:667–82. [RE, DH, GWM, NO, RRa, aEDR, rKR]Google Scholar
Morrison, R. E. & Rayner, K. (1981) Saccade size in reading depends upon character spaces and not visual angle. Perception and Psychophysics 30:395–96. [aEDR]Google Scholar
Motter, B. C. & Mountcastle, V. B. (1980) Active directed gaze controls the excitability of the light sensitive neurons of the inferior parietal lobule in the waking monkey. Neuroscience Abstracts 6, No. 230:14673. [BF]Google Scholar
Muller, R.-A., Rothermel, R. D., Behen, M. E., Muzik, O., Mangner, T. J. & Chugani, H. T. (1997) Receptive and expressive language activations for sentences: A PET study. NeuroReport 8:3767–70. [aEDR]Google Scholar
Munoz, D. P. & Wurtz, R. H. (1992) Role of the rostral superior colliculus in active visual fixation and execution of express saccades. Journal of Neurophysiology 67:10001002. [BF]Google Scholar
Murray, W. S. (1998) Parafoveal pragmatics. In: Eye guidance in reading and scene perception, pp. 181–99, ed. Underwood, G. Elsevier. [WSM, aEDR]Google Scholar
Murray, W. S. & Clayes, E. (1998) Plausibility and syntactic complexity. Paper presented at Architectures and Mechanisms for Language Processing Conference, Freiburg, September 1998. [WSM]Google Scholar
Murray, W. S., Dobbin, C., Murray, A. M. & Clayes, E. (1999) Plausibility and sentence interpretation. Paper presented at Architectures and Mechanisms for Language Processing Conference, Edinburgh, September 1999. [WSM]Google Scholar
Murray, W. S. & Kennedy, A. (1988) Spatial coding and the processing of anaphor by good and poor readers: Evidence from eye movement analyses. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology 40A:693718. [AK]Google Scholar
Murray, W. S. & Liversedge, S. P. (1994) Referential context effects on syntactic processing. In: Perspectives on sentence processing, pp. 359–88, ed. Clifton, C., Frazier, L. & Rayner, K. Erlbaum. [SPL, WSM]Google Scholar
Murray, W. S. & Rowan, M. (1998) Early, mandatory, pragmatic processing. Journal of Psycholinguistic Research (CUNY Special Issue) 27:122. [WSM]Google Scholar
Newell, A. (1990) Unified theories of cognition. Harvard University Press. [aEDR]Google Scholar
Ngo, T. T., Miller, S. M., Liu, G. B. & Pettigrew, J. D. (2000) Binocular rivalry and perceptual coherence. Current Biology 10:134–36. [RS]Google Scholar
Niswander, E., Pollatsek, A. & Rayner, K. (2000) The processing of derived and inflected suffixed words during reading. Language and Cognitive Processes 15:389420. [aEDR, rKR]Google Scholar
Nobre, A. C., Gitelman, D. R., Dias, E. C. & Mesulam, M. M. (2000) Covert visual spatial orienting and saccades: Overlapping neural Systems NeuroImage 11:210–16. [CB]Google Scholar
Oliva, A. & Torralba, A. (2001) Modeling the shape of the scene: A holistic representation of the spatial envelope. International Journal of Computer Vision 42:145–75. [PDG]Google Scholar
Olson, R. K., Kliegl, R. & Davidson, B. J. (1983) Dyslexic and normal readers’ eye movements. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance 9:816–25. [BF]Google Scholar
O’Regan, J. K. (1979) Eye guidance in reading: Evidence for linguistic control hypothesis. Perception and Psychophysics 25:501509. [aEDR]Google Scholar
O’Regan, J. K. (1980) The control of saccade size and fixation duration in reading: The limits of linguistic control. Perception and Psychophysics 28:112–17. [aEDR]Google Scholar
O’Regan, J. K. (1983) Elementary perceptual and eye movement control processes in reading. In: Eye movements in reading: Perceptual and language processes, ed. Rayner, K. Academic Press. [aEDR]Google Scholar
O’Regan, J. K. (1990) Eye movements and reading. In: Eye movements and their role in visual and cognitive processes, pp. 395453, ed. Kowler, E. Elsevier. [aEDR, rKR, RRe]Google Scholar
O’Regan, J. K. (1992a) Eye movements and reading. In: Eye movements and visual cognition: Scene perception and reading, pp. 333–54, ed. Rayner, K. Springer-Verlag. [JP]Google Scholar
O’Regan, J. K. (1992b) Optimal viewing position in words and the strategy-tactics theory of eye movements in reading. In: Eye movements and visual cognition: Scene perception and reading, ed. Rayner, K. Springer-Verlag. [aEDR, rKR]Google Scholar
O’Regan, J. K. & Lévy-Schoen, A. (1987) Eye movement strategy and tactics in word recognition and reading. In: Attention and performance XII: The psychology of reading, ed. Coltheart, M. Erlbaum. [aEDR]Google Scholar
O’Regan, J. K., Lévy-Schoen, A., Pynte, J. & Brugaillère, B. (1984) Convenient fixation location within isolated words of different length and structure. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance 10(2):250–57. [KDM, aEDR, rKR]Google Scholar
Osaka, M., Nishizaki, Y., Komori, M. & Osaka, N. (2002) Effect of focus in verbal working memory: Critical role of focus word in reading. Memory and Cognition 30:562–71. [NO]Google Scholar
Osaka, M., Osaka, N., Kondo, H., Morishita, M., Fukuyama, H., Aso, T. & Shibasaki, H. (2003) Neural basis of individual differences in working memory: An fMRI study. NeuroImage 18:789–97. [NO]Google Scholar
Osaka, N. (1992) Size of saccade and fixation duration of eye movements during reading: Psychophysics of Japanese text processing. Journal of the Optical Society of America A 9:513. [NO, aEDR]Google Scholar
Osaka, N. & Oda, K. (1994) Moving window for reading experiments. Behavior Research Methods, Instruments and Computers 26:4953. [NO]Google Scholar
Osaka, N. & Osaka, M. (2002) Individual differences in working memory during reading with and without parafoveal information: A moving window study. American Journal of Psychology 115:501–13. [NO]Google Scholar
O’Shea, R. & Corballis, P. M. (2001) Binocular rivalry between complex stimuli in split-brain observers. Brain and Mind 2:151–60. [RS]Google Scholar
Ottes, F. P., van Gisbergen, J. A. M. & Eggermont, J. J. (1984) Metrics of saccade responses to visual double stimuli: Two different modes. Vision Research 24:1169–79. [RR]Google Scholar
Paap, K. R., Newsome, S. L., McDonald, J. E. & Schvaneveldt, R. W. (1982) An activation-verification model for letter and word recognition: The word-superiority effect. Psychological Review 89:573–94. [LH, aEDR]Google Scholar
Pacht, J., Schroyens, W. & d’Ydewalle, G. (1999) Inhibitory neighborhood effects in visual word recognition. Paper presented at the Tenth European Conference on Eye Movements, September, Utrecht, The Netherlands. [JP]Google Scholar
Pavlidis, G. T. (1981) Do eye movements hold the key to dyslexia? Neuropsychologia 19:5764. [BF]Google Scholar
Perea, M. & Pollatsek, A. (1998) The effects of neighborhood frequency in reading and lexical decision. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance 24:767–79. [MP, aEDR]Google Scholar
Perfetti, C. A. & Bell, L. (1991) Phonemic activation during the first 40 ms of word identification: Evidence from backwards masking and priming. Journal of Memory and Language 30:473–85. [rKR]Google Scholar
Peterson, S. E., Fox, P. T., Posner, M. I., Mintun, M. & Raichle, M. E. (1989) Positron emission tomographic studies of the cortical anatomy of single-word processing. Nature 331:585–89. [aEDR]Google Scholar
Peterson, S. E., Fox, P. T., Snyder, A. Z. & Raichle, M. E. (1990) Activation of extrastriate and frontal cortical areas by visual words and word-like stimuli. Science 249:1041–44. [aEDR]Google Scholar
Pickering, M. J. & Frisson, S. (2001) Processing ambiguous verbs: Evidence from eye movements. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition 27:556–73. [rKR]Google Scholar
Plaut, D. C., McClelland, J. L., Seidenberg, M. S. & Patterson, K (1996) Understanding normal and impaired word reading: Computational principles in quasi-regular domains. Psychological Review 103:56115. [aEDR]Google Scholar
Poldrack, R. A., Desmond, J. E., Glover, G. H. & Gabrieli, J. D. E. (1998) The neural basis of visual skill learning: An fMRI study of mirror reading. Cerebral Cortex 8:110. [aEDR]Google Scholar
Pollatsek, A., Bolozky, S., Well, A. D. & Rayner, K. (1981) Asymmetries in the perceptual span for Israeli readers. Brain and Language 14:174–80. [aEDR]Google Scholar
Pollatsek, A., Hyönä, J. & Bertram, R. (2000) The role of morphological constituents in reading Finnish compound words. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance 26:820–33. [SA, JH, aEDR, rKR]Google Scholar
Pollatsek, A., Lesch, M., Morris, R. K. & Rayner, K. (1992) Phonological codes are used in integrating information across saccades in word identification and reading. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance 18:148–62. [aEDR, rKR]Google Scholar
Pollatsek, A., Perea, M. & Binder, K. S. (1999a) The effects of neighborhood size in reading and lexical decision. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance 25:1142–58. [SA, MP, aEDR]Google Scholar
Pollatsek, A., Raney, G. E., LaGasse, L. & Rayner, K. (1993) The use of information below fixation in reading and in visual search. Canadian Journal of Experimental Psychology 47:179200. [aEDR]Google Scholar
Pollatsek, A. & Rayner, K. (1982) Eye movement control in reading: The role of word boundaries. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance 8:817–33. [aEDR, rKR]Google Scholar
Pollatsek, A. & Rayner, K. (1990) Eye movements and lexical access in reading. In: Comprehension processes in reading, pp. 143–63, ed. Balota, D. A., Flores d’Arcais, G. B. & Rayner, K. Erlbaum. [JP, aEDR]Google Scholar
Pollatsek, A. & Rayner, K. (1999) Is covert attention really unnecessary? Behavioral and Brain Sciences 22:695–96. [AK, aEDR]Google Scholar
Pollatsek, A., Rayner, K. & Balota, D. A. (1986) Inferences about eye movement control from the perceptual span in reading. Perception and Psychophysics 40:123–30. [aEDR, rKR]Google Scholar
Pollatsek, A., Rayner, K., Fischer, M. H. & Reichle, E. D. (1999b) Attention and eye movements in reading. In: Reading and dyslexia: Visual and attentional processes, ed. Everatt, J. Routledge. [aEDR]Google Scholar
Pollatsek, A., Rayner, R. & Lee, H. W. (2000) Phonological coding in word perception and reading. In: Reading as a perceptual process, pp. 399426, ed. Kennedy, A., Radach, R., Heller, D. & Pynte, J. Elsevier. [LH, rKR]Google Scholar
Pollatsek, A., Reichle, E. D. & Rayner, K. (2003) Modeling eye movements in reading: Extensions of the E-Z Reader model. In: The mind's eyes: Cognitive and applied aspects of oculomotor research, pp. 361–90, ed. Hyönä, J., Radach, R. & Deubel, H. Elsevier. [aEDR, rKR]Google Scholar
Posner, M. I. (1980) Orienting of attention. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology 32:325. [aEDR]Google Scholar
Posner, M. I., Aldullaev, Y. G., McCandliss, B. D. & Sereno, S. C. (1999a) Anatomy, circuitry and plasticity of word reading. In: Reading and dyslexia: Visual and attentional processes, ed. Everatt, J. Routledge. [aEDR]Google Scholar
Posner, M. I., Aldullaev, Y. G., McCandliss, B. D. & Sereno, S. C. (1999b) Neuroanatomy, circuitry and plasticity of word reading. NeuroReport 10:R12R23. [aEDR]Google Scholar
Posner, M. I. & Raichle, M. I. (1997) Images of mind. Scientific American Library. [aEDR]Google Scholar
Price, C. J. (1997) Functional anatomy of reading. In: Human brain function, ed. Frackowiak, R. S. J., Friston, K. J., Frith, C. D., Dolan, R. J. & Mazziotta, J. C. Academic Press. [aEDR]Google Scholar
Pugh, K. R., Mencl, W. E., Shaywitz, B. A., Shaywitz, S. E., Fulbright, R. K., Constable, R. T., Skudlarski, P., Marchione, K. E., Jenner, A. R., Fletcher, J. M., Liberman, A. M., Shankweiler, D. P., Katz, L., Lacadie, C. & Gore, J. C. (2000) The angular gyrus in developmental dyslexia: Taskspecific differences in functional connectivity with posterior cortex. Psychological Science 11:5156. [aEDR]Google Scholar
Pulvermüller, F., Assadollahi, R. & Elbert, T. (2001) Neuromagnetic evidence for early semantic access in word recognition. European Journal of Neuroscience 13:201205. [SCS]Google Scholar
Pynte, J. (1996) Lexical control of within-word eye movements. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance 22:958–69. [JP]Google Scholar
Pynte, J., Kennedy, A. & Ducrot, S. (in press) The influence of parafoveal typographical errors on eye movements in reading. European Journal of Cognitive Psychology. [rKR]Google Scholar
Pynte, J., Kennedy, A., Murray, W. S. & Courrieu, P. (1988) The effects of spatialisation on the processing of ambiguous pronominal reference. In: Eye movement research: Physiological and psychological aspects, pp. 214–25, ed. Luer, G., Lass, U. & Shallo-Hoffman, J. Hogrefe. [AK]Google Scholar
Raaijmakers, J. G. W. & Shiffrin, R. M. (1981) Search of associative memory. Psychological Review 88:93134. [SA]Google Scholar
Radach, R. & Heller, D. (2000) Relations between spatial and temporal aspects of eye movement control. In: Reading as a perceptual process, ed. Kennedy, A., Radach, R., Heller, D. & Pynte, J. Elsevier. [RRa, aEDR, rKR]Google Scholar
Radach, R., Inhoff, A. & Heller, D. (in press) Orthographic regularity gradually modulates saccade amplitudes in reading. European Journal of Cognitive Psychology. [SPL]Google Scholar
Radach, R. & Kempe, V. (1993) An individual analysis of initial fixation positions in reading. In: Perception and cognition: Advances in eye movement research, ed. d’Ydewalle, G. & Van Rensbergen, J. North Holland. [aEDR]Google Scholar
Radach, R. & McConkie, G. W. (1998) Determinants of fixation positions in words during reading. In: Eye guidance in reading and scene perception, pp. 77100, ed. Underwood, G. Elsevier. [AK, aEDR]Google Scholar
Rafal, R. & Robertson, L. (1995) The neurology of visual attention. In: The cognitive neurosciences, ed. Gazzaniga, M. S. MIT Press. [aEDR]Google Scholar
Raney, G. E., Atilano, R. & Gomez, L. (1996) Language representation and bilingual reading. Paper presented at the meeting of the Psychonomic Society, Chicago, IL, November 1996. [GER, rKR]Google Scholar
Raney, G. E. & Rayner, K. (1995) Word frequency effects and eye movements during two readings of a text. Canadian Journal of Experimental Psychology 49:151–72. [aEDR]Google Scholar
Raney, G. E., Therriault, D. & Minkoff, S. (2000) Repetition effects from paraphrased text: Evidence for an integrative model of text representation. Discourse Processes 28:6181. [GER, rKR]Google Scholar
Rayner, K. (1975) The perceptual span and peripheral cues in reading. Cognitive Psychology 7:6581. [JP, aEDR]Google Scholar
Rayner, K. (1977) Visual attention in reading: Eye movements reflect cognitive processes. Memory and Cognition 4:443–48. [aEDR]Google Scholar
Rayner, K. (1978) Eye movements in reading and information processing. Psychological Bulletin 85:618–60. [aEDR]Google Scholar
Rayner, K. (1979) Eye guidance in reading: Fixation locations within words. Perception 8:2130. [aEDR, rKR]Google Scholar
Rayner, K. (1986) Eye movements and the perceptual span in beginning and skilled readers. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology 41:211–36. [aEDR]Google Scholar
Rayner, K. (1995) Eye movements and cognitive processes in reading, visual search, and scene perception. In: Eye movement research: Mechanisms, processes and applications, pp. 322, ed. Findlay, J. M., Walker, R. & Kentridge, R. W. North Holland. [rKR]Google Scholar
Rayner, K. (1998) Eye movements in reading and information processing: 20 years of research. Psychological Bulletin 124:372422. [JP, aEDR, rKR]Google Scholar
Rayner, K., Ashby, J., Pollatsek, A. & Reichle, E. D. (2003a) The effects of frequency and predictability on eye fixations in reading: Implications for the E-Z Reader model. (submitted). [rKR]Google Scholar
Rayner, K., Balota, D. A. & Pollatsek, A. (1986) Against parafoveal semantic preprocessing during eye fixations in reading. Canadian Journal of Psychology 40:473–83. [aEDR]Google Scholar
Rayner, K. & Bertera, J. H. (1979) Reading without a fovea. Science 206:468–69. [aEDR]Google Scholar
Rayner, K., Binder, K. S., Ashby, J. & Pollatsek, A. (2001) Eye movement control in reading: Word predictability has little influence on initial landing positions in words. Vision Research 41:943–54. [aEDR, LS]Google Scholar
Rayner, K., Carlson, M. & Frazier, L. (1983a) The interaction of syntax and semantics during sentence processing: Eye movements in the analysis of semantically biased sentences. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior 22:358–74. [SPL, rKR]Google Scholar
Rayner, K. & Duffy, S. A. (1986) Lexical ambiguity and fixation times in reading: Effects of word frequency, verb complexity, and lexical ambiguity. Memory and Cognition 14:191201. [aEDR]Google Scholar
Rayner, K. & Duffy, S. A. (1988) Online comprehension processes and eye movements in reading. In: Reading research: Advances in theory and practice, ed. Daneman, M., MacKinnon, G. E. & Waller, T. G. Academic Press. [aEDR]Google Scholar
Rayner, K. & Fischer, M. H. (1996) Mindless reading revisited: Eye movements during reading and scanning are different. Perception and Psychophysics 58:734–47. [aEDR, rKR]Google Scholar
Rayner, K., Fischer, M. H. & Pollatsek, A. (1998a) Unspaced text interferes with both word identification and eye movement control. Vision Research 38:1129–144. [aEDR, rKR]Google Scholar
Rayner, K. & Frazier, L. (1989) Selection mechanisms in reading lexically ambiguous word. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition 15:779–90. [aEDR]Google Scholar
Rayner, K., Inhoff, A. W., Morrison, R., Slowiczek, M. L. & Bertera, J. H. (1981) Masking of foveal and parafoveal vision during eye fixations in reading. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance 7:167–79. [aEDR, rKR]Google Scholar
Rayner, K., Kambe, G. & Duffy, S. A. (2000) The effect of clause wrap-up on eye movements during reading. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology 53A:1061–80. [aEDR]Google Scholar
Rayner, K., Liversedge, S. P., White, S. J. & Vergilino-Perez, D. (2003b) Reading disappearing text: Cognitive control of eye movements. Psychological Science 14:385–89. [rKR]Google Scholar
Rayner, K. & McConkie, G. W. (1976) What guides a reader's eye movements. Vision Research 16:829–37. [aEDR, rKR]Google Scholar
Rayner, K. & Morris, R. K. (1992) Eye movement control in reading: Evidence against semantic preprocessing. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance 18:163–72. [aEDR, GU]Google Scholar
Rayner, K. & Morrison, R. E. (1981) Eye movements and identifying words in parafoveal vision. Bulletin of the Psychonomic Society 17:135–38. [aEDR, rKR]Google Scholar
Rayner, K. & Pollatsek, A. (1981) Eye movement control during reading: Evidence for direct control. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology 33A:351–73. [aEDR]Google Scholar
Rayner, K. & Pollatsek, A. (1987) Eye movements in reading: A tutorial review. In: Attention and performance XII, pp. 327–62, ed. Coltheart, M. Erlbaum. [rKR]Google Scholar
Rayner, K. & Pollatsek, A. (1989) The psychology of reading. Prentice Hall. [aEDR, rKR]Google Scholar
Rayner, K. & Pollatsek, A. (1996) Reading unspaced text is not easy: Comments on the implications of Epelboim et al.'s (1994) study for models of eye movement control in reading. Vision Research 36:461–65. [AK]Google Scholar
Rayner, K., Pollatsek, A. & Binder, K. S. (1998b) Phonological codes and eye movements in reading. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition 24:476–97. [rKR]Google Scholar
Rayner, K. & Raney, G. E. (1996) Eye movement control in reading and visual search: Effects of word frequency. Psychonomic Bulletin and Review 3:238–44. [aEDR, rKR]Google Scholar
Rayner, K., Reichle, E. D. & Pollatsek, A. (1998c) Eye movement control in reading: An overview and model. In: Eye guidance in reading and scene perception, pp. 243–68, ed. Underwood, G. Elsevier. [MB, aEDR, rKR]Google Scholar
Rayner, K., Reichle, E. D. & Pollatsek, A. (2000) Eye movement control in reading: Updating the E-Z Reader model to account for initial fixation locations and refixations. In: Reading as a perceptual process, Kennedy, A., Radach, R., Heller, D., & Pynte, J. Elsevier. [aEDR]Google Scholar
Rayner, K. & Sereno, S. C. (1994) Eye movements in reading: Psycholinguistic studies. In: Handbook of psycholinguistics, ed. Gernsbacher, M. Academic Press. [aEDR]Google Scholar
Rayner, K., Sereno, S. C., Lesch, M. F. & Pollatsek, A. (1995) Phonological codes are automatically activated during reading: Evidence from an eye movement priming paradigm. Psychological Science 6:2632. [rKR, SCS]Google Scholar
Rayner, K., Sereno, S. C., Morris, R. K., Schmauder, A. R. & Clifton, C. (1989) Eye movements and online language comprehension processes. Language and Cognitive Processes 4 [Special issue]: 2149. [aEDR]Google Scholar
Rayner, K., Sereno, S. C. & Raney, G. E. (1996) Eye movement control in reading: A comparison of two types of models. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance 22(5):1188–200. [JH, aEDR, rKR, FV]Google Scholar
Rayner, K., Slowiaczek, M. L., Clifton, C. & Bertera, J. H. (1983b) Latency of sequential eye movements: Implications for reading. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance 9:912–22. [SPL, aEDR]Google Scholar
Rayner, K. & Well, A. D. (1996) Effects of contextual constraint on eye movements in reading: A further examination. Psychonomic Bulletin and Review 3:504509. [aEDR, rKR, LS]Google Scholar
Rayner, K., Well, A. D. & Pollatsek, A. (1980) Asymmetry of the effective visual field in reading. Perception and Psychophysics 27:537–44. [aEDR]Google Scholar
Rayner, K., Well, A. D., Pollatsek, A. & Bertera, J. H. (1982) The availability of useful information to the right of fixation in reading. Perception and Psychophysics 31:537–50. [PDG, aEDR]Google Scholar
Rayner, K., White, S. J., Kambe, G., Miller, B. & Liversedge, S. P. (2003c) On the processing of meaning from parafoveal vision during eye fixations in reading. In: The mind's eye: Cognitive and applied aspects of eye movements, ed. Hyona, J., Radach, R. & Deubel, H. Elsevier. [WSM, aEDR, rKR]Google Scholar
Reichle, E. D.& Perfetti, C. A. (2003) Morphology in word identification: A wordexperience model that accounts for morpheme frequency effects. Scientific Studies of Reading. 7:219–38. [rKR]Google Scholar
Reichle, E. D., Pollatsek, A., Fisher, D. L. & Rayner, K. (1998) Toward a model of eye movement control in reading. Psychological Review 105:125–57. [SA, RE, GF, aEDR, rKR, EMR]Google Scholar
Reichle, E. D. & Rayner, K. (2001) Cognitive processes and models of reading. In: Models of the visual system, ed. Hung, G. & Ciufferda, K. Plenum. [aEDR]Google Scholar
Reichle, E. D., Rayner, K. & Pollatsek, A. (1999) Eye movement control in reading: Accounting for initial fixation locations and refixations within the E-Z Reader model. Vision Research 39:4403–411. [GF, JP, aEDR, rKR, EMR]Google Scholar
Reilly, R. (1993) A connectionist framework for modeling eye movement control in reading. In: Perception and cognition: Advances in eye movement research, ed. d’Ydewalle, G. & Van Rensbergen, North Holland. [aEDR]Google Scholar
Reilly, R. & O’Regan, J. K. (1998) Eye movement control in reading: A simulation of some word-targeting strategies. Vision Research 38:303–17. [aEDR, RRe]Google Scholar
Reilly, R. & Radach, R. (2003) Foundations of an interactive activation model of eye movement control in reading. In: The mind's eye: Cognitive and applied aspects of eye movements, ed. Hyönä, J., Radach, R. & Deubel, H. Elsevier. [JH, RRa, LH, aEDR, rKR, RRe]Google Scholar
Reingold, E. M. & Stampe, D. M. (1999) Saccadic inhibition in complex visual tasks. In: Current oculomotor research: Physiological and psychological aspects, pp. 249–55, ed. Becker, W. & Mergner, T. Plenum. [GWM]Google Scholar
Reynolds, J. H., Pasternak, T. & Desimone, R. (2000) Attention increases sensitivity of V4 neurons. Neuron 26:703–14. [rKR, SCS]Google Scholar
Robinson, D. L., Goldberg, M. E. & Stanton, G. B. (1978) Parietal association cortex in the primate: Sensory mechanisms and behavioral modulations. Journal of Neurophysiology 41:910–32. [BF]Google Scholar
Rousselet, G. A., Fabre-Thorpe, M. & Thorpe, S. J. (2002) Parallel processing in high-level categorization of natural images. Nature Neuroscience 5:629–30. [SCS]Google Scholar
Rumelhart, D. E., Hinton, G. E. & Williams, R. J. (1986a) Learning internal representations by error propagation. In: Parallel distributed processing: Explorations in the microstructure of cognition, vol. 1: Foundations, ed. Rumelhart, D. E., McClelland, J. L. & the PDP Research Group. MIT Press. [aEDR]Google Scholar
Rumelhart, D. E., McClelland, J. L. & the PDP Research Group. (1986b) Parallel distributed processing: Explorations in the microstructure of cognition, vol. 1: Foundations. MIT Press. [aEDR]Google Scholar
Sagi, D. & Julesz, B. (1985) “Where” and “what” in vision. Science 228:1217–19. [aEDR]Google Scholar
Salvucci, D. D. (2000) An interactive model-based environment for eye movement protocol visualization and analysis. In: Proceedings of the Eye Tracking Research and Applications Symposium. ACM Press. [aEDR]Google Scholar
Salvucci, D. D. (2001) An integrated model of eye movements and visual encoding. Cognitive Systems Research 1:201–20. [aEDR]Google Scholar
Salvucci, D. D. & Anderson, J. R. (1998) Tracing eye movement protocols with cognitive process models. Proceedings of the Twentieth Annual Conference of the Cognitive Science Society. Erlbaum. [aEDR]Google Scholar
Salvucci, D. D. & Anderson, J. R. (2001)Automated eye movement protocol analysis. Human Computer Interaction 16:3986. [aEDR]Google Scholar
Schall, J. D. & Hanes, K. G. (1993) Neural basis of target selection in frontal eye field during visual search. Nature 366:467–69. [CB]Google Scholar
Schiller, P. H., Sandell, J. H. & Maunsell, J. H. (1987) The effect of frontal eye field and superior colliculus lesions on saccadic latencies in the rhesus monkey. Journal of Neurophysiology 57:1033–49. [BF]Google Scholar
Schilling, H. E. H., Rayner, K. & Chumbley, J. I. (1998) Comparing naming, lexical decision, and eye fixation times: Word frequency effects and individual differences. Memory and Cognition 26:1270–81. [MB, GF, aEDR, rKR, Rre, LS]Google Scholar
Schlosser, M. J., Aoyagi, N., Fulbright, R. K., Gore, J. C. & McCarthy, G. (1998) Functional MRI studies of auditory comprehension. Human Brain Mapping 6:113. [aEDR]Google Scholar
Schmidt, D. C. & Fayad, M. (1997) Object-oriented application frameworks. Communications of the Association for Computing Machinery 40:3238. [RRe]Google Scholar
Schneider, W. X. & Deubel, H. (2002) Selection-for-perception and selection-for-spatial-motor-action are coupled by visual attention: A review of recent findings and new evidence for stimulus driven saccade control. In: Attention and performance XIX: Common mechanisms in perception and action, pp. 609–27, ed. Prinz, W. & Hommel, B. Oxford University Press. [RRa]Google Scholar
Schreuder, R. & Baayen, R. H. (1995) Modelling morphological processing. In: Morphological aspects of language processing, pp. 131–54, ed. Feldman, L. B. Erlbaum. [JH]Google Scholar
Schroyens, W., Vitu, F., Brysbaert, M. & d’Ydewalle, G. (1999) Eye movement control during reading: Foveal load and parafoveal processing. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology 52A(4):1021–46. [aEDR, FV]Google Scholar
Schustack, M. W., Ehrlich, S. F. & Rayner, K. (1987) The complexity of contextual facilitation in reading: Local and global influences. Journal of Memory and Language 26:322–40. [aEDR, rKR]Google Scholar
Seidenberg, M. S. (1989) Visual word recognition and pronunciation: A computational model and its implications. In: Lexical representation and process, ed. Marslen-Wilson, W. MIT Press. [aEDR]Google Scholar
Seidenberg, M. S. & McClelland, J. L. (1989) A distributed, developmental model of word recognition and naming. Psychological Review 96:523–68. [aEDR, RS]Google Scholar
Sereno, A. B. (1996) Parsing cognitive processes: Psychopathological and neurophysiological constraints. In: Psychopathology: The evolving science of mental disorder, pp. 407–32, ed. Matthysse, S., Levy, D. L., Kagan, J. & Benes, F. M. Cambridge University Press. [SCS]Google Scholar
Sereno, S. C. (1992) Early lexical effects when fixating a word in reading. In: Eye movements and visual cognition: Scene perception and reading, ed. Rayner, K. Springer-Verlag. [aEDR]Google Scholar
Sereno, S. C. (1995) Resolution of lexical ambiguity: Evidence from an eye movementpriming paradigm. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition 21:582–95. [GER, aEDR, SCS]Google Scholar
Sereno, S. C., Brewer, C. C. & O’Donnell, P. J. (2003) Context effects in word recognition: Evidence for early interactive processing. Psychological Science 14:328–33. [SCS]Google Scholar
Sereno, S. C., Pacht, J. M. & Rayner, K. (1992) The effect of meaning frequency on processing lexically ambiguous words: Evidence from eye fixations. Psychological Science 3:296300. [aEDR, SCS]Google Scholar
Sereno, S. C. & Rayner, K. (1992) Fast priming during eye fixations in reading. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance 18:173–84. [SCS]Google Scholar
Sereno, S. C. & Rayner, K. (2000) Spelling-sound regularity effects on eye fixations in reading. Perception and Psychophysics 62:402409. [SCS]Google Scholar
Sereno, S. C., Rayner, K. & Posner, M. I. (1998) Establishing a time-line of processing during reading: Evidence from eye movements and event-related potentials. NeuroReport 9:2195–200. [RRa, aEDR, SCS]Google Scholar
Shillcock, R., Ellison, T. M. & Monaghan, P. (2000) Eye-fixation behavior, lexical storage, and visual word recognition in a split-processing model. Psychological Review 107:824–51. [aEDR, RS]Google Scholar
Shioiri, S. & Ikeda, M. (1989) Useful resolution for picture perception as a function of eccentricity. Perception 18:347–61. [PDG]Google Scholar
Sieroff, E., Pollatsek, A. & Posner, M. I. (1988) Recognition of visual letter strings following injury to the posterior visual spatial attention system. Cognitive Neuropsychology 5:427–49. [rKR]Google Scholar
Somers, D. C., Dale, A. M., Seiffert, A. E. & Tootell, R. B. H. (1999) Functional MRI reveals spatially specific attentional modulation in human primary visual cortex. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences USA 96:1663–68. [aEDR]Google Scholar
Sommer, M. & Schiller, P. (1992) Express saccades elicited during natural fixations of visual search. Neuroscience Abstracts 18, No. 587(9):1398. [BF]Google Scholar
Sparks, D. L. & Mays, L. (1990) Signal transformations required for the generation of saccadic eye movements. Annual Review of Neuroscience 13:309–36. [aEDR]Google Scholar
Starr, M. S. & Inhoff, A. W. (in press) Attention allocation to the right and left of a fixated work: Use of orthographic information from multiple words during reading. European Journal of Cognitive Psychology. [AWI, aEDR, rKR]Google Scholar
Starr., M. S. & Rayner, K. (2001) Eye movements during reading: Some current controversies. Trends in Cognitive Science 5:156–63. [MP, aEDR]Google Scholar
Stein, J. F. & Fowler, M. S. (1993) Unstable binocular control in dyslexic children. Journal of Research in Reading 16(1):3045. [BF]Google Scholar
Stelmach, L. B., Campsall, J. M. & Herdman, C. M. (1997) Attentional and ocular movements. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance 23:823–44. [aEDR]Google Scholar
Sternberg, S. (1966) High-speed scanning in human memory. Science 153:652–54. [rKR]Google Scholar
Sternberg, S. (1969) The discovery of processing stages: Extensions of Donder's method. Acta Psychologica 30:276315. [rKR]Google Scholar
Sternberg, S. (1975) Memory scanning: New findings and current controversies. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology 27:132. [rKR]Google Scholar
Stromswold, K., Caplin, D., Alpert, N. & Rauch, S. (1996) Localization of syntactic comprehension by Positron Emission Tomography. Brain and Language 52:452–73. [aEDR]Google Scholar
Suppes, P. (1990) Eye movement models for arithmetic and reading performance. In: Eye movements and their role in visual and cognitive processes, ed. Kowler, E. Elsevier. [aEDR]Google Scholar
Suppes, P. (1994) Stochastic models of reading. In: Eye movements in reading, ed. Ygge, J. & Lennerstrand, G. Pergamon Press. [aEDR]Google Scholar
Suppes, P., Cohen, M., Laddaga, R., Anliker, J. & Floyd, H. (1982) Research on eye movements in arithmetic performance. In: Cognition and eye movements, ed. Groner, R. & Fraisse, P. North-Holland. [aEDR]Google Scholar
Suppes, P., Cohen, M., Laddaga, R., Anliker, J. & Floyd, H. (1983) A procedural theory of eye movements in doing arithmetic. Journal of Mathematical Psychology 27:341–69. [aEDR]Google Scholar
Sweeney, J. A., Mintun, M. A., Kwee, S., Wiseman, M. B., Brown, D. L., Rosenberg, D. R. & Carl, J. R. (1996) Positron emission tomography study of voluntary saccadic eye movements and spatial working memory. Journal of Neurophysiology 75:454–68. [aEDR]Google Scholar
Tanaka, K. (1996) Inferotemporal cortex and object vision. Annual Review of Neuroscience 19:109–39. [aEDR]Google Scholar
Theios, J. & Amrhein, P. C. (1989) Theoretical analysis of the cognitive processing of lexical and pictorial stimuli: Reading, naming, and visual and conceptual comparisons. Psychological Review 96:524. [PDG]Google Scholar
Thibadeau, R., Just, M. A. & Carpenter, P. A. (1982) A model of the time course and content of reading. Cognitive Science 6:157203. [aEDR]Google Scholar
Traxler, M. J. & Pickering, M. J. (1996) Plausibility and the processing of unbounded dependencies: An eye-tracking study. Journal of Memory and Language 35:454–75. [WSM]Google Scholar
Treisman, A. M. (1969) Strategies and models of selective attention. Psychological Review 76:242–99. [aEDR, rKR]Google Scholar
Treisman, A. M. & Gelade, G. (1980) A feature-integration theory of attention. Cognitive Psychology 12:97136. [RRa]Google Scholar
Underwood, G., Bloomfield, R. & Clews, S. (1989) Information influences the pattern of eye fixations during sentence comprehension. Perception 17:267–78. [GU]Google Scholar
Underwood, G., Clews, S. & Everatt, J. (1990) How do readers know where to look next? Local information distributions influence eye fixations. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology 42A:3965. [aEDR, GU]Google Scholar
Underwood, N. R. & McConkie, G. W. (1985) Perceptual span for letter distinctions during reading. Reading Research Quarterly 20:153–62. [aEDR]Google Scholar
Ungerleider, L. G., Courtney, S. M. & Haxby, J. V. (1998) A neural system for human visual working memory. Proceedings of the National Academy of Science 95:883–90. [aEDR]Google Scholar
Ungerleider, L. G. & Haxby, J. V. (1994) “What” and “where” in the human brain. Current Opinion in Neurobiology 4:157–65. [aEDR]Google Scholar
Ungerleider, L. G. & Mishkin, M. (1982) Two cortical visual systems. In: Analysis of visual behavior, ed. Ingle, D. J., Goodale, M. A. & Mansfield, R. J. W. MIT Press. [aEDR]Google Scholar
Uttal, W. R. (1990) On some two-way barriers between models and mechanisms. Perception and Psychophysics 48:188203. [rKR]Google Scholar
Van Essen, D. C. & De Yoe, E. A. (1995) Concurrent processing in the primate visual cortex. In: The cognitive neurosciences, ed. Gazzaniga, M. S. MIT Press. [aEDR]Google Scholar
Vecera, S. P. & Farah, M. J. (1997) Is visual image segmentation a bottom-up or an interactive process? Perception and Psychophysics 59:1280–96. [PDG]Google Scholar
Vergilino, D. & Beauvillain, C. (2000) The planning of refixation saccades in reading. Vision Research 40(25):3527–38. [KDM, JP, aEDR]Google Scholar
Vergilino-Perez, D. & Beauvillain, C. (in press) The ability of the saccadic system to change motor plans in scanning letter strings. Psychonomic Bullletin and Review. [KDM]Google Scholar
Vitu, F. (1991a) The existence of a center of gravity effect during reading. Vision Research 31(7/8):1289–313. [FV]Google Scholar
Vitu, F. (1991b) The influence of parafoveal processing and linguistic context on the optimal landing position effect. Perception and Psychophysics 50:5875. [aEDR]Google Scholar
Vitu, F., Brysbaert, M. & Lancelin, D. (in press) A test of parafoveal-on-foveal effects with pairs of orthographically related words. European Journal of Cognitive Psychology. [FV]Google Scholar
Vitu, F. & McConkie, G. W. (2000) Regressive saccades and word perception in adult reading. In: Reading as a perceptual process, pp. 301–26, ed. Kennedy, A., Radach, R., Heller, D. & Pynte, J. Elsevier. [FV]Google Scholar
Vitu, F., McConkie, G. W., Kerr, P. & O’Regan, J. K. (2001) Fixation location effects on fixation durations during reading: An inverted optimal viewing position effect. Vision Research 41:3513–33. [JH, aEDR, rKR, FV]Google Scholar
Vitu, F., McConkie, G. W. & Zola, D. (1998) About regressive saccades in reading and their relation to word identification. In: Eye guidance in reading and scene perception, ed. Underwood, G. Elsevier. [FV]Google Scholar
Vitu, F., O’Regan, J. K., Inhoff, A. W. & Topolski, R. (1995) Mindless reading: Eyemovement characteristics are similar in scanning letter strings and reading texts. Perception and Psychophysics 57(3):352–64. [GWM, FV]Google Scholar
Vonk, W., Radach, R. & van Rijn, H. (2000) Eye guidance and the saliency of word beginnings in reading text. In: Reading as a perceptual process, ed. Kennedy, A., Radach, R., Heller, D. & Pynte, J. North Holland/Elsevier. [aEDR, GU]Google Scholar
Walker, R., Kentridge, R. W. & Findlay, J. M. (1995) Independent contributions of the orienting of attention, fixation offset and bilateral stimulation on human saccadic latencies. Experimental Brain Research 103:294310. [JMF]Google Scholar
Watt, S. M. & Murray, W. S. (1996) Prosodic form and parsing commitments. Journal of Psycholinguistic Research (CUNY Special Issue) 25:291318. [AK]Google Scholar
Wenban-Smith, M. G. & Findlay, J. M. (1991) Express saccades: Is there a separate population in humans? Experimental Brain Research 87:218–22. [JMF]Google Scholar
White, S. J. & Liversedge, S. P. (in press) Orthographic familiarity influences initial eye fixation positions in reading. European Journal of Cognitive Psychology. [SPL, rKR]Google Scholar
White, S. J. & Liversedge, S. P. (2003) Orthographic regularity influences the eyes’ landing position in reading. (submitted). [SPL]Google Scholar
Wiley, J. & Rayner, K. (2000) Effects of titles on the processing of text and lexically ambiguous words: Evidence from eye movements. Memory and Cognition 28:1011–21. [aEDR, GER]Google Scholar
Woldorff, M. G., Fox, P. T., Matzke, M., Lancaster, J. L., Veeraswamy, S., Zamarripa, F., Seabolt, M., Glass, T., Gao, J. H., Martin, C. C. & Jerabek, P. (1997) Retinotopic organization of the early visual-spatial attention effects as revealed by PET and ERPs. Human Brain Mapping 5:280–86. [CB]Google Scholar
Wolfe, J. M. & Bennett, S. C. (1996) Preattentive object files: Shapeless bundles of basic features. Vision Research 37:2543. [aEDR]Google Scholar
Wolverton, G. S. & Zola, D. (1983) The temporal characteristics of visual information extraction during reading. In: Eye movements in reading: Perceptual and language processes, ed. Rayner, K. Academic Press. [aEDR]Google Scholar
Yang, S.-N. & McConkie, G. W. (2001) Eye movements during reading: A theory of saccade initiation time. Vision Research 41:3567–85. [aEDR, rKR]Google Scholar
Yang, S.-N. & McConkie, G. W. (in press) Saccade generation during reading: Are words necessary? European Journal of Cognitive Psychology. [GWM, rKR]Google Scholar
Yeshurun, Y. & Carrasco, M. (1998) Attention improves or impairs visual performance by enhancing spatial resolution. Nature 396:7275. [SCS, rKR]Google Scholar
Yonelinas, A. P. (2002) The nature of recollection and familiarity: A review of 30 years of research. Journal of Memory and Language 46:441517. [rKR]Google Scholar
Zola, D. (1984) Redundancy and word perception during reading. Perception and Psychophysics 36:277–84. [aEDR]Google Scholar