Hostname: page-component-586b7cd67f-t8hqh Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-11-24T02:05:28.410Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Conceptual short-term memory (CSTM) supports core claims of Christiansen and Chater

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  02 June 2016

Mary C. Potter*
Affiliation:
Department of Brain and Cognitive Sciences, Massachusetts Institute of Technology, Cambridge, MA 02139. [email protected]://mollylab-1.mit.edu/lab/

Abstract

Rapid serial visual presentation (RSVP) of words or pictured scenes provides evidence for a large-capacity conceptual short-term memory (CSTM) that momentarily provides rich associated material from long-term memory, permitting rapid chunking (Potter 1993; 2009; 2012). In perception of scenes as well as language comprehension, we make use of knowledge that briefly exceeds the supposed limits of working memory.

Type
Open Peer Commentary
Copyright
Copyright © Cambridge University Press 2016 

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

Chun, M. M. & Potter, M. C. (1995) A two-stage model for multiple target detection in rapid serial visual presentation. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance 21:109–27.Google ScholarPubMed
Davenport, J. L. & Potter, M. C. (2004) Scene consistency in object and background perception. Psychological Science 15:559–64.Google Scholar
Davenport, J. L. & Potter, M. C. (2005) The locus of semantic priming in RSVP target search. Memory and Cognition 33:241–48.Google Scholar
Endress, A. D. & Potter, M. C. (2014a) Large capacity temporary visual memory. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General. 143(1):548–65. doi: 10.1037/a0033934.Google Scholar
Forster, K. I. (1970) Visual perception of rapidly presented word sequences of varying complexity. Perception & Psychophysics 8:215–21.Google Scholar
Forster, K. I. & Davis, C. (1984) Repetition priming and frequency attenuation in lexical access. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition 10:680698.Google Scholar
Intraub, H. (1981) Rapid conceptual identification of sequentially presented pictures. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance 7:604–10.Google Scholar
Kutas, M. & Hillyard, S. A. (1980) Reading senseless sentences: Brain potentials reflect semantic incongruity. Science 207:203205.Google Scholar
Lawrence, D. H. (1971a) Temporal numerosity estimates for word lists. Perception and Psychophysics 10:7578.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lawrence, D. H. (1971b) Two studies of visual search for word targets with controlled rates of presentation. Perception and Psychophysics 10:8589.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Loftus, G. R. (1983) Eye fixations on text and scenes. In: Eye movements in reading: Perceptual and language processes, ed. Rayner, K., pp. 359–76. Academic Press.Google Scholar
Lombardi, L. & Potter, M. C. (1992) The regeneration of syntax in short term memory. Journal of Memory and Language 31:713–33.Google Scholar
Luck, S. J., Vogel, E. K. & Shapiro, K. L. (1996) Word meanings can be accessed but not reported during the attentional blink. Nature 383:616–18.Google Scholar
Meng, M. & Potter, M. C. (2008) Detecting and remembering pictures with and without visual noise. Journal of Vision 8(9):7. Available at: http://journalofvision.org/8/9/7/ Google Scholar
Meng, M. & Potter, M. C. (2011) An attentional blink for nontargets? Attention, Perception, and Psychophysics 73:440–46.Google Scholar
Neely, J. H. (1991) Semantic priming effects in visual word recognition: A selective review of current findings and theories. In: Basic processes in reading: Visual word recognition, ed. Besner, D. & Humphreys, G. W., pp. 264–36. Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Potter, M. C. (1976) Short-term conceptual memory for pictures. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Learning and Memory 2:509–22.Google Scholar
Potter, M. C. (1984) Rapid serial visual presentation (RSVP): A method for studying language processing. In: New methods in reading comprehension research, ed. Kieras, D. & Just, M., pp. 91–18. Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Potter, M. C. (1993) Very short-term conceptual memory. Memory and Cognition 21:156–61.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Potter, M. C. (2009) Conceptual short term memory. Scholarpedia 5(2):3334.Google Scholar
Potter, M. C. (2012) Conceptual short term memory in perception and thought. Frontiers in Psychology 3:113. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2012.00113.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Potter, M. C. & Faulconer, B. A. (1975) Time to understand pictures and words. Nature 253:437–38.Google Scholar
Potter, M. C. & Lombardi, L. (1990) Regeneration in the short-term recall of sentences. Journal of Memory and Language 29:633–54.Google Scholar
Potter, M. C. & Lombardi, L. (1998) Syntactic priming in immediate recall of sentences. Journal of Memory and Language 38:265–82.Google Scholar
Potter, M. C., Kroll, J. F. & Harris, C. (1980) Comprehension and memory in rapid sequential reading. In: Attention and Performance VIII, ed. Nickerson, R., pp. 395–18. Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Potter, M. C., Kroll, J. F., Yachzel, B., Carpenter, E. & Sherman, J. (1986) Pictures in sentences: Understanding without words. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General 115:281–94.Google Scholar
Potter, M. C., Moryadas, A., Abrams, I. & Noel, A. (1993) Word perception and misperception in context. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition 19:322.Google Scholar
Potter, M. C., Staub, A. & O'Connor, D. H. (2002) The time course of competition for attention: Attention is initially labile. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance 28:1149–62.Google ScholarPubMed
Potter, M. C., Stiefbold, D. & Moryadas, A. (1998) Word selection in reading sentences: Preceding versus following contexts. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition 24:68100.Google Scholar
Potter, M. C., Valian, V. V. & Faulconer, B. A. (1977) Representation of a sentence and its pragmatic implications: Verbal, imagistic, or abstract? Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior 16:112.Google Scholar
Potter, M. C., Wyble, B., Hagmann, C. E. & McCourt, E. S. (2014) Detecting meaning in RSVP at 13 ms per picture. Attention, Perception, and Performance 76(2):270–79. doi: 10.3758/s13414-013-0605-z.Google Scholar
Potter, M. C., Wyble, B., Pandav, R. & Olejarczyk, J. (2010) Picture detection in RSVP: Features or identity? Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance 36:1486–94.Google Scholar
Rayner, K., ed. (1983) Eye movements in reading: Perceptual and language processes. Academic Press.Google Scholar
Rayner, K., ed. (1992) Eye movements and visual cognition: Scene perception and reading. Springer.Google Scholar
Sperling, G., Budiansky, J., Spivak, J. G. & Johnson, M. C. (1971) Extremely rapid visual search: The maximum rate of scanning letters for the presence of a numeral. Science 174:307–11.Google Scholar