Crossref Citations
This article has been cited by the following publications. This list is generated based on data provided by
Crossref.
Carruthers, Peter
2003.
On Fodor's Problem.
Mind & Language,
Vol. 18,
Issue. 5,
p.
502.
Vallortigara, Giorgio
Pagni, Piero
and
Sovrano, Valeria Anna
2004.
Separate Geometric and Non-Geometric Modules for Spatial Reorientation: Evidence from a Lopsided Animal Brain.
Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience,
Vol. 16,
Issue. 3,
p.
390.
Machery, Édouard
2004.
Pour une approche évolutionniste de la cognition animale.
Dialogue,
Vol. 43,
Issue. 4,
p.
731.
Victorri, Bernard
2004.
Continu et discret en sémantique lexicale.
Cahiers de praxématique,
p.
75.
Miyake, Akira
Emerson, Michael J
Padilla, Francisca
and
Ahn, Jeung-chan
2004.
Inner speech as a retrieval aid for task goals: the effects of cue type and articulatory suppression in the random task cuing paradigm.
Acta Psychologica,
Vol. 115,
Issue. 2-3,
p.
123.
Sutton, John
2004.
Representation, levels, and context in integrational linguistics and distributed cognition.
Language Sciences,
Vol. 26,
Issue. 6,
p.
503.
Fields, Chris
2004.
The role of aesthetics in problem solving: some observations and a manifesto.
Journal of Experimental & Theoretical Artificial Intelligence,
Vol. 16,
Issue. 1,
p.
41.
Stanovich, Keith E.
2004.
Metarepresentation and the great cognitive divide: A commentary on Henriques' “Psychology Defined”.
Journal of Clinical Psychology,
Vol. 60,
Issue. 12,
p.
1263.
Wynn, Thomas
and
Coolidge, Frederick L.
2004.
The expert Neandertal mind.
Journal of Human Evolution,
Vol. 46,
Issue. 4,
p.
467.
Fernyhough, Charles
2004.
Alien voices and inner dialogue: towards a developmental account of auditory verbal hallucinations.
New Ideas in Psychology,
Vol. 22,
Issue. 1,
p.
49.
Christensen, Wayne
2004.
Self-directedness, integration and higher cognition.
Language Sciences,
Vol. 26,
Issue. 6,
p.
661.
Coward, L.Andrew
and
Sun, Ron
2004.
Criteria for an effective theory of consciousness and some preliminary attempts.
Consciousness and Cognition,
Vol. 13,
Issue. 2,
p.
268.
Carruthers, Peter
2004.
Practical Reasoning in a Modular Mind.
Mind & Language,
Vol. 19,
Issue. 3,
p.
259.
Vicente, Agustin
and
Martinez-Manrique, Fernando
2005.
Semantic Underdetermination and the Cognitive Uses of Language.
Mind and Language,
Vol. 20,
Issue. 5,
p.
537.
Clark, Andy
2005.
Beyond the Flesh: Some Lessons from a Mole Cricket.
Artificial Life,
Vol. 11,
Issue. 1-2,
p.
233.
Slade, Lance
and
Ruffman, Ted
2005.
How language does (and does not) relate to theory of mind: A longitudinal study of syntax, semantics, working memory and false belief.
British Journal of Developmental Psychology,
Vol. 23,
Issue. 1,
p.
117.
Sovrano, Valeria Anna
Bisazza, Angelo
and
Vallortigara, Giorgio
2005.
Animals' use of landmarks and metric information to reorient: effects of the size of the experimental space.
Cognition,
Vol. 97,
Issue. 2,
p.
121.
Apperly, Ian A.
Samson, Dana
and
Humphreys, Glyn W.
2005.
Domain-specificity and theory of mind: evaluating neuropsychological evidence.
Trends in Cognitive Sciences,
Vol. 9,
Issue. 12,
p.
572.
Shanahan, Murray
and
Baars, Bernard
2005.
Applying global workspace theory to the frame problem.
Cognition,
Vol. 98,
Issue. 2,
p.
157.
Machery, Edouard
2005.
You Don't Know How You Think: Introspection and Language of Thought.
The British Journal for the Philosophy of Science,
Vol. 56,
Issue. 3,
p.
469.