No CrossRef data available.
Article contents
Affordance perception and the Y-magnocellular pathway
Published online by Cambridge University Press: 04 February 2010
Abstract
An abstract is not available for this content so a preview has been provided. Please use the Get access link above for information on how to access this content.
- Type
- Open Peer Commentary
- Information
- Copyright
- Copyright © Cambridge University Press 1989
References
Abeles, M. (1982) Local cortical circuits: An electrophysiological study. Springer-Verlag. [aGWS, JF]CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Aiple, F. & Krüger, J. (1988) Neural synchrony in monkey striate cortex: Interocular signal flow and dependency on spike rates. Experimental Brain Research 72:141–49. [FA]CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Andersen, R. A., Essick, C. K. & Siegel, R. M. (1985) Encoding of spatial location by posterior parietal neurons. Science 234:456–58. [DCP, PAS]CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Anderson, J. A., Silverstein, J. W., Ritz, S. A. & Jones, R. S. (1977) Distinctive features, categorical perception, and probability learning: Some applications of a neural model. Psychological Review 84:413–51. [JKK]CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Antes, J. R. & Penland, J. G. (1981) Picture context effects on eye movement patterns. In: Eye movements: Cognition and visual perception, ed Fischer, D. F., Monty, R. A. & Senders, J. W.. Erlbaum. [JW]Google Scholar
Attneave, F. (1954) Some informational aspects of visual perception. Psychological Review 61:183–93. [JW]CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Attneave, F. (1974) Apparent movement and the what-where connection. Psychologia 17:108–20. [MRWD]Google Scholar
Averbach, E. & Coriell, A. S. (1961) Short-term memory in vision. Bell Systems Technical journal 40:309–28. [BEB]CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Axelrod, R. & Hamilton, W. D. (1981) The evolution of cooperation. Science 211:1390–96. [aCWS]CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bach, M. & Krüger, J. (1986) Correlated neuronsl variability in monkey visual cortex revealed by a multi-microelectrode. Experimental Brain Research 61:451–56. [aGWS]CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bahrick, L. E., Walker, A. S. & Neisser, U. (1981) Selective looking by infants. Cognitive Psychology 13:377–90. [JD]CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Baird, J. C. & Wagner, M. (1980) Modelling the creation of cognitive maps. In: Spatial orientation: Theory, research, and application, ed. Pick, H. L., Jr & Acredolo, L. P.. Plenum Press. [aCWS]Google Scholar
Ballard, D. H. (1981) Generalizing the Hough transform to detect arbitrary shapes. Pattern Recognition 13:111–22. [rCWS]CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ballard, D. H. (1984) Parameter networks: Towards a theory of low-level vision. Artificial Intelligence 22:235–67. [aGWS]CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ballard, D. H. (1986) Cortical connections and parallel processing: Structure and function. Behavioral and Brain Sciences 9:67–120. [arGWS, PAS]CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ballard, D. H. (1987) Cortical connections and parallel processing: Structure and function. In: Vision, brain, and co-operative computation, ed. Arbib, M. A. & Hanson, A. R.. MIT Press. [JF]Google Scholar
Ballard, D. H., Hinton, G. E. & Sejnowski, T. J. (1983) Parallel visual computation. Nature 306:21–26. [aGWS, JKK]CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ballard, D. H. & Kimball, O. A. (1983) Rigid body motion from depth and optical flow. Computer Graphics and Image Processing 22:95–115. [rGWS]CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Barlow, H. B. (1972) Single units and sensation: A neuron doctrine for perceptual psychology. Perception 1:371–94. [aCWS]CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bartram, D. J. (1978) Post-iconic visual storage: Chunking in the reproduction of briefly displayed visual patterns. Cognitive Psychology 10:324–55. [aCWS]CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bayliss, G. C., Rolls, E. T. & Leonard, C. M. (1985) Selectivity between faces in the responses of a population of neurones in the cortex in the superior temporal sulcus of the monkey. Brain Research 342:91–102. [MPY]CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bell, T. (1988) Sequential processing using attractor transitions. In: The proceedings of the 1988 connectionist models summer school, ed Touretzky, D. S., Hinton, G. E. & Sejnowski, T. J.. Morgan Kaufmann. [JKK]Google Scholar
Biederman, I. (1981) On the semantics of a glance at a scene. In: Perceptual organization. ed. Kubovy, M. & Pomerantz, J. R.. Erlbaum. [JW]Google Scholar
Biederman, I. (1987) Recognition-by-components: A theory of human image understanding. Psychological Review 94:115–47. [arCWS, HJM, JW]CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bliss, T. V. P. & Gardner-Medwin, A. R. (1973) Long-lasting potentiation of synaptic transmission in the dentate area of the unanesthetized rabbit following stimulation of the perforant path. Journal of Physiology 232:357–74. [aCWS]CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Blomfield, S. & Marr, D. (1970) How the cerebellum may be used. Nature 227:1224–28. [aCWS]CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bouma, H. (1978) Visual search and reading: Eye movements and the functional visual field: A tutorial review. In: Attention and performance, ed Requin, J.. Erlbaum. [JW]Google Scholar
Breitmeyer, B. G. & Ganz, L. (1977) Temporal studies with flashed gratings: Inferences about human transient and sustained channels. Vision Research 17:861–65. [DMP]CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Briand, K. A. & Klein, R. M. (1987) Is Posner's “beam” the same as Treisman's “glue”?: On the relation between visual orienting and feature integration theory, Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance 13:228–41. [CPW]Google ScholarPubMed
Bridgeman, B., Kirch, M. & Sperling, A. (1981) Segregation of cognitive and motor aspects of visual function using induced motion. Perception and Psychophysics 29:336–42. [BB]CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bridgeman, B., Lewis, S., Heit, G. & Nagle, M. (1979) Relation between cognitive and motor-oriented systems of visual position perception. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance 5:692–700. [BB]Google ScholarPubMed
Bridgeman, B. & Stark, L. (1979) Omnidirectional increase in threshold for image shifts during saccadic eye movements. Perception and Psychophysics 25:241–43. [BB]CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Broadbent, D. E. (1975) The magic number seven after fifteen years. In: Studies in long term memory, ed Kennedy, A. & Wilkes, A.. Wiley. [rCWS]Google Scholar
Broadbent, D. E. & Broadbent, M. H. P. (1987) From detection to identification: Response to multiple targets in rapid serial visual presentation. Perception & Psychophysics 42:105–13. [JD]CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Browse, R. A. & Butler, B. E. (1985) Could three frames suffice? Behavioral and Brain Sciences 8:290–91. [aCWS]CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bruce, C., Desimone, R. & Cross, C. C. (1981) Visual properties of neurons in a polysensory area in superior temporal sulcus of the macaque. Journal of Neurophysiology 46:369–84. [PAS]CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Burt, P. & Sperling, G. (1981) Time, distance, and feature tradeoffs in visual apparent motion. Psychological Review 88:137–51. [MRWD]CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bushnell, M. C., Goldberg, M. E. & Robinson, D. L. (1981) Behavioral enhancement of visual responses in monkey cerebral cortex. I. Modulation in posterior parietal cortex related to selective visual attention. Journal of Neurophysiology 46:755–72. [aCWS]CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Butler, B. E. (1975) Selective attention and target search with brief visual displays. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology 27:467- 77 [BEB]CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Butler, B. E. (1980a) Selective attention and stimulus localization in visual perception. Canadian Journal of Psychology 34:119–33. [BEB]CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Butler, B. E. (1980b) The category effect in visual search: Identification versus localization factors. Canadian Journal of Psychology 34:238–47. [BEB]CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Butler, B. E. (1981) Identification and localization in tachistoscopic recognition: The effects of data- and resource-limitations. Canadian Journal of Psychology 35:38–51. [BEB]CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Butler, B. E. & Currie, A. M. (1986) On the nature of perceptual limits in vision: A new look at lateral masking. Psychological Research 48:201–10. [BEB]CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Butler, B. E., Mewhort, D. J. K. & Tramer, S. (1987) Location errors in tachistoscopic recognition: Guesses, probe errors, or spatial confusions? Canadian Journal of Psychology 41:339–50. [BEB]CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Butler, B. E. & Morrison, I. R. (1984) Do letter features migrate?: A note of caution. Psychological Research 46:223–36. [BEB]CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Caudill, M. & Butler, C. (1987) Proceedings of the Institute of Electrical and Electronic Engineers First Annual International Conference on Neural Networks, Piscataway, NJ: IEEE Service Center. [aGWS]Google Scholar
Chastain, G. (1982) Feature mislocalizations and misjudgments of intercharacter distance. Psychological Research 44:51–66. [WP]CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cohen, A. & Ivry, R. B. (in press) Illusory conjunctions as a function of distance between objects inside and outside the focus of attention. journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance ]WP]Google Scholar
Cohen, M. A. & Grossberg, S. (1983) Absolute stability of global pattern formation and parallel memory storage by competitive neural networks. IEEE Transactions on Systems. Man and Cybernetics 13:815–26. [JKK]Google Scholar
Cohen, N. J. & Squire, L. R. (1980) Preserved learning and retention of patter-analyzing skill in amnesia: Dissociation of knowing how and knowing that. Science 210:207–10. [aGWS]CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Colonnier, M. (1981) The electron-microscopic analysis of the neuronal organization of the cerebral cortex. In: The organization of the cerebral cortex, ed. Schmitt, F. O., Worden, F. G., Adelman, G. & Dennis, S. G.. MIT Press. [JF]Google Scholar
Coren, S. & Hoenig, P. (1972) Effect of non-target stimuli on the length of voluntary saccades. Perceptual and Motor Skills 34:499–508. [JF]CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Crick, F. (1984) Function of the thalamic reticular complex: The searchlight hypothesis. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America 81:4586–90. [arGWS, JD, HJM, DMP, PAS]CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Davidson, M. L., Fox, M. J. & Dick, A. O. (1973) Effect of eye movements on backward masking and perceived location. Perception & Psychophysics 14:110–16. [LEK]CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Dawson, M. R. W. (1987) Moving contexts do affect the perceived direction of apparent motion in motion competition displays. Vision Research 27:799–809. [MRWD]CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Dawson, M. R. W. (1988) The cooperative application of multiple natural constraints to the motion correspondence problem. Proceedings of the Seventh Canadian Conference on Artifitcial Intelligence. [MRWD]Google Scholar
Dawson, M. R. W. (submitted) Image matching in motion correspondence: The roles of geometry and topology. [MRWD]Google Scholar
Dawson, M. R. W. & Pylyshyn, Z. W. (1988) Natural constraints on apparent motion. In: Computational processes in human vision, ed. Pylyshyn, Z.. Ablex. [MRWD]Google Scholar
Desimone, R., Schein, S. J., Moran, J. & Ungerleider, L. G. (1985) Contour, color, and shape analysis beyond the striate cortex. Vision Research 25:441–52. [DJ]CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Deubel, H., Findlay, J. M., Jacobs, A. M. & Brogan, D. (1988) Saccadic eye movements to targets defined by structure differences. In: Eye movement research: Physiological and psychological aspects, ed. Lass, C. LüerU. & Shallo-Hoffman, J. S. J.. Hogrefe, C. J.. [JF]Google Scholar
Deubel, H., Wolf, W. & Hauske, G. (1984) The evaluation of the oculomotor error signal. In: Theoretical and applied aspects of eye movement research, ed. Gale, A. C. & Johnson, F. W.. North-Holland. [JF]Google Scholar
Dick, A. O. (1970) Visual processing and the use of redundant information in tachistoscopic recognition. Canadian journal of Psychology 24:133–41. [BEB]CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Dixon, P. & Shedden, J. M. (1987) Conceptual and physical differences in the category effect. Perception & Psychophysics 42:457–64. [BEB]CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Dodwell, P. C. (1971) On perceptual clarity. Psychological Review 78:275- 89. [LEK]CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Duncan, J. (1980) The locus of interference in the perception of simultaneous stimuli. Psychological Review 87:272–300. [JD]CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Duncan, J. (1983a) Category effects in visual search: A failure to replicate the “oh-zero” phenomenon. Perception & Psychophysics 34:221–32. [BEB]CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Duncan, J. (1983b) Perceptual selection based on alphanumeric class: Evidence from partial reports. Perception & Psychophysics 33:533–47. [BEB]CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Duncan, J. (1984) Selective attention and the organization of visual information. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General 113:501–17. [JD]CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Duncan, J. (1987) Attention and reading: Wholes and parts in shape recognition: A tutorial review. In: Attention and performance XII: The psychology of reading, ed. Coltheart, M.. Erlbaum. [HJM]Google Scholar
Duncan, J. & Humphreys, G. W. (in press) Visual search and stimulus similarity. Psychological Review. [rGWS, JD, HJM]Google Scholar
Eccles, J. C. (1984) The cerebral neocortex: A theory of its operation. In: Cerebral cortex, vol. 2, Functional properties of cortical cells, ed. Jones, E. G. & Peters, A.. Plenum Press. [aGWS]Google Scholar
Edelman, G. M., Call, W. E. & Cowan, W. M. (1984) Dynamic aspects of neocortical function. Wiley. [MPY]Google Scholar
Edelman, G. M. & Reeke, G. N. (1982) Selective networks capable of representative transformation, limited generalizations, and associative memory. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, USA 79:2091- 95. [JKK]CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Enns, J. T. (1988) Some three-dimensional objects “pop out” in visual search. Bulletin of the Psychonomic Society 26:514. [JW]Google Scholar
Eriksen, C. & Hoffman, J. (1973) The extent of processing of noise elements during selective encoding from visual displays. Perception and Psychophysics 14:155–60. [WP]CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Eriksen, C. W. & St., James J. D. (1986) Visual attention within and around the field of focal attention: A zoom lens model. Perception & Psychophysics 40:255–40. [WP, CPW]CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Eriksen, C. W. & Yeh, Y.-Y. (1985) Allocation of attention in the visual field. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance 11:583–97. [CPW]Google ScholarPubMed
Feldman, J. A. (1981) Memory and change in connection networks. Computer Science Department Technical Report TR96. University of Rochester. [aGWS]Google Scholar
Feldman, J. A. (1985) Four frames suffice: A provisional model of vision and space. Behavioral and Brain Sciences 8:265–313. [aGWS, JD, DCP]CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Feldman, J. A. & Ballard, D. H. (1988) Connectionist models and their properties. In: Connectionist models and their implications: Readings from cognitive science, ed. Waltz, D. & Feldman, J. A.. Ablex. [aGWS]Google Scholar
Findlay, J. M. (1982) Global processing for saccadic eye movements. Vision Research 22:1033–45. [JF]CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Findlay, J. M. (1987) Visual computation and saccadic eye movements. Spatial Vision 2:175–89. [JF]CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Fischer, B. & Boch, R. (1985) Peripheral attention versus central fixation: Modulation of the visual activity of prelunate cortical cells of the rhesus monkey. Brain Research 345:111–23. [FA]CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Friedman, A. (1979) Framing pictures: The role of knowledge in automatized encoding and memory for gist. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General 108:316–55. [JW]CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Friedman, R. B. (1980) Identity without form: Abstract representations of letters. Perception & Psychophysics 28:53–60. [BEB]CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Fukada, Y. (1971) Receptive field organisation of the cat optic nerve with special reference to conduction velocity. Vision Research 11:209–26. [DMP]CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Fukushima, K. (1984) A hierarchical neural network model for associative memory. Biological Cybernetics 50:105–13. [MPY]CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Fukushima, K. (1986) A neural network model for selective attention in visual pattern recognition. Biological Cybernetics 55:5–15. [JKK]CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Georgopoulos, A. P., Caminiti, R., Kalaska, J. F. & Massey, J. T. (1983) Spatial coding of movement: A hypothesis concerning the coding of movement direction by motor cortical populations. In: Neural coding of motor perfonnance, ed. Massion, J., Paillard, J., Schultz, W. & Wiesendanger, M.. Springer-Verlag. [M PY]Google Scholar
Georgopoulos, A. P., Lurito, J. T., Petrides, M., Schwartz, A. B. & Massey, J. T. (1989) Mental rotation of the neuronal population vector. Science 243:234–46. [rGWS]CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Georgopoulos, A. P., Schwartz, A. B. & Kettner, R. E. (1986) Neuronal population coding of movement direction. Science 233:1416–19. [arCWS]CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gerstein, G. L., Perkel, D. H. & Dayhoff, J. E. (1985) Co-operative firing activity in simultaneously recorded populations of neurons: Detection and measurement. Journal of Neuroscience 5:881–89. [JF]CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gibson, J. J. (1979) The ecological approach to visual perception. Houghton Muffin. [aGWS, CF, JKK, LEK]Google Scholar
Goodale, M. A., Pelisson, D. & Prablanc, C. (1986) Large adjustments in visually guided reaching do not depend on vision of the hand or perception of target displacement. Nature 320:748–50. [BB]CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gouras, P. (1985) Oculomotor system. In: Principles of neural science, 2nd edition, ed. Kandel, E. R. & Schwartz, J. H.. Elsevier. [CF]Google Scholar
Graham, N. (1981) Psychophysics of spatial-frequency channels. In: Perceptual organization, ed. Kubovy, M. & Pomerantz, J.. Erlbaum. [LEK]Google Scholar
Graham, N., Kramer, P. & Haber, N. (1985) Attending to the spatial frequency and spatial position of near-threshold visual patterns. In: Attention & performance XI, ed. Posner, M. I. & Marin, O. S. M.. Erlbaum. [LEK]Google Scholar
Gray, C. M., Koenig, P., Engel, A. K. & Singer, W. (1988) Spatial temporal distribution of stimulus specific oscillations in cat visual cortex, part 1, Local interactions. Society for Neuroscience Abstracts 362.12:899. [MPY]Google Scholar
Gray, C. M. (1989) Oscillatory responses in cat visual cortex exhibit intercolumnar synchronization and reflect global properties of stimuli. Nature 338:334–37. [MPY]CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gray, C. M. & Singer, W. (1988) The role of nonlinear cooperativity and temporal synchrony in mediating neuronal responses in the primary visual cortex of the cat. Presented at the 11th European Neuroscience Association/20th European Brain and Behaviour Society Meeting, September, Zurich. [MPY]Google Scholar
Green, M. (1986) What determines correspondence strength in apparent motion? Vision Research 26:599–607. [MRWD]CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gross, C. G., Desimone, R., Albright, T. D. & Schwartz, E. L. (1985) Inferior temporal cortex and pattern recognition. In: Pattern recognition, ed. Chagas, C., Cattass, H. & Cross, C.. Springer-Verlag. [MPY]Google Scholar
Grossberg, S. (1980) How does a brain build a cognitive code. Psychological Review 87:1–51. [MPY]CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Grossberg, S. (1988) Nonlinear neural networks: Principles, mechanisms, and architectures. Neural Networks 1:17–61. [JKK]CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Haber, R. N. (1983) The impending demise of the icon: A critique of the concept of iconic storage in visual information processing. Behavioral and Brain Sciences 6:1–54. [LEK]CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Haenny, P. E., Maunsell, J. H. R. & Schiller, P. H. (1988) State dependent activity in monkey visual cortex. II. Retinal and extraretinal factors in V4. Experimental Brain Research 69:245–59. [FA]CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Heit, G., Smith, M. E. & Halgren, E. (1988) Neural encoding of individual words and faces in the human hippocanipus and amygdala. Nature 333:773–75. [MPY]CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hinton, G. E. (1981a) A parallel computation that assigns canonical object based frames of reference. In: Proceedings of the 7th International Joint Conference on Artificial Intelligence, Vancouver, B.C., Canada. [HJM, DCP]Google Scholar
Hinton, G. E. (1981b) Shape representation in parallel systems. In: Proceedings of the 7th International Joint Conference on Artificial Intelligence, Vancouver, B.C., Canada. [DCP]Google Scholar
Hinton, G. E. & Lang, K. J. (1985) Shape recognition and illusory conjunctions. Proceedings of the International Joint Conference on Artiflcial Intelligence. Morgan Kaufmann. [aGWS, DCP]Google Scholar
Hinton, G. E., McClelland, J. L. & Rumelhart, D. E. (1986) Distributed representations. In: Parallel distributed processing, ed. Rumelhart, D. E. & McClelland, J. L.. MIT Press. [JKK]Google Scholar
Hinton, G. E. & Parsons, L. M. (1988) Scene-based and viewer-centered representations for comparing shapes. Cognition 30:1–35. [DCP]CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hinton, G. E. & Sejnowski, T. J. (1986) Learning and relearning in Boltzmann machines. In: Parallel distributed processing: Explorations in the microstructure of cognition, vol. 1, ed. Rumelhart, D. E. & McClelland, J. L.. MIT Press. [aGWS, JKIC]Google Scholar
Hoffman, D. D. & Richards, W. A. (1984) Parts of recognition. Cognition 18:65–96. [JW]CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hoffman, J. E. & Nelson, B. (1981) Spatial selectivity in visual search. Perception & Psychophysics 30:283–90. [CPW]CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Holland, J. H. (1975) Adaptation in natural and artificial systems. University of Michigan Press. [arGWS]Google Scholar
Holland, J. H. (1986) Escaping brittleness: The possibilities of general-purpose learning algorithms applied to parallel rule-based systems. In: Machine learning: An artificial intelligence approach, vol. 2, ed. Carbonell, J. G. & Mitchell, T. M.. Morgan Kaufman. [arGWS]Google Scholar
Holland, J. H. (1987) Genetic algorithms and classifier systems: Foundations and future directions. In: Genetic algorithms and their applications: Proceedings of the Second International Conference on Genetic Algorithms, ed. Grefenstette, J. J.. Erlbaum. [rOWS]Google Scholar
Holland, J. H., Holyoak, K. J., Nisbett, R. E. & Thagard, P. R. (1987) Induction: Processes of inference, learning, and discovery. MIT Press. [aGWS]Google Scholar
Hopfield, J. J. (1982) Neural networks and physical systems with emergent collective computational abilities. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, USA 79:2554–58. [JKK]CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hopfield, J. J. (1984) Neurons with graded response have collective computational properties like those of two-state neurons. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, USA 81:3088–92. [JKK]CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hopfield, J. J., Feinstein, D. I. & Palmer, R. G. (1983) “Unlearning” has a stabilizing effect in collective memories. Nature 304:158–59. [JKK]CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hrechanyk, L. M. & Ballard, D. H. (1983) Viewframes: Aconnectionist model of form perception. In: Proceedings of the 14th Annual Image Understanding Workshop. Science Applications. [arGWS]Google Scholar
Hubel, D. H. & Livingstone, M. S. (1985) Complex-unoriented cells in a subregion of primate area 18. Nature 315:325–27. [aGWS]CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hubel, D. H. & Wiesel, T. N. (1962) Receptive fields, binocular interaction, and functional architecture in the cat's visual cortex. Journal of Physiology 160:106–54. [LEK]CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Humphreys, G. W. (in press) Connectionism and psychology. In: Parallel distributed processing: Implications for psychology and neuroscience, ed. Morris, R. C.. Oxford University Press. [HJM]Google Scholar
Humphreys, G. W., Quinlan, P. T. & Riddoch, M. J. (in press) Grouping processes in visual search: A case for parallel distributed feature- combination. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General. [HIM]Google Scholar
Hurlbert, A. & Poggio, T. (1985) Spotlight on attention. Trends in Neuroscience, 07:309–11. [aGWS]CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ikeuchi, K. (1987) Generating and interpretation tree from a CAD model for 3D-object recognition in bin-picking tasks. International Journal of Computer Vision 1:145–65. [DCP]CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ingling, N. W. (1972) Categorization: A mechanism for rapid information processing. Journal of Experimental Psychology 94:239–43. [BEB]CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Irwin, D. E., Brown, J. S. & Sun, J.-S. (1988) Visual masking and visual integration across saccadic eye movements. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General 117:276–87. [LEK]CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Johannesma, P., Aertsen, A., van, den Boogaard H., Eggermont, J. & Epping, W. (1986) From synchrony to harmony: Ideas on the function of neural assemblies and on the interpretation of neural synchrony. In: Brain theory, ed. Palm, G. & Aertsen, A.. Springer-Verlag. [JF]Google Scholar
Johnson, G. (1988) Artificial brain again seen as a guide to the mind. The New York Times, 08 16. [aGWS]Google Scholar
Johnson, M. (1987) The body in the mind: The bodily basis of meaning, imagination, and reason. University of Chicago Press. [aGWS]CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Jonides, J. (1983) Further toward a model of the mind's eye movement. Bulletin of the Psychonomic Society 21:247–50. [aGWS, CPW]CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Jonides, J. & Gleitman, H. (1972) A conceptual category effect in visual search: O as letter or as digit. Perception & Psychophysics 12: 457–60. [BEB]CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Jonides, J. (1976) The benefit of categorization in visual search: Target location without identification. Perception & Psychophysics 20:289–98. [BEB]CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Julesz, B. (1980) Spatial-frequency channels in one-, two-, and three- dimensional vision: Variations on an auditory theme by Bekesy. In: Visual coding and adaptability, ed. Harris, C. S.. Erlbaum. [LEK]Google Scholar
Kahneman, D. & Henik, A. (1981) Perceptual organization and attention. In: Perceptual organization, ed. Kubovy, M. & Pomerantz, J. R.. Erlbaum. [JD]Google Scholar
Kandel, E. R. (1985) Processing of form and movement in the visual system. In: Principles of neural science, 2nd edition, ed Kandel, E. R. & Schwartz, J. H.. Elsevier. [CF]Google Scholar
Kandel, E. R. & Schwartz, J. H. (1981) Principles of neural science. Elsevier/North-Holland. [aGWS]Google Scholar
Kaplan, S., Weaver, M. & French, R. (in press) Active symbols and internal models: Toward a cognitive connectionism. Al and Society. [RML]Google Scholar
Kawamoto, A. H. & Anderson, J. A. (1985) A neural network model of multistable perception. Acta Psychologica 59:35–65. [JKK]CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Keele, S. W., Cohen, A., Ivry, R., Liotti, M. & Yee, P. (1988) Tests of a temporal theory of attentional binding. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance 14:444–52. [HJM, WP]Google ScholarPubMed
Koch, C., Poggio, T. & Torre, V. (1983) Nonlinear interactions in a dendritic tree: Localization, timing, and role in information processing. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences USA 80:2799–2802, [aGWS]CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Koch, C. & Ullman, S. (1985) Shifts in selective visual attention: Towards the underlying circuitry. Human Neurobiology 4:219–27. [aGWS]Google Scholar
Koendennk, J. J. & van, Doom A. J. (1986) Dynamic shape. Biological Cybernetics 53:383–96. [JW]CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kolers, P. A. (1983) Some features of visual form. Computer vision, graphics, and image processing 23:15–41. [MRWD]CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kolers, P. & Green, M. (1984) Color logic of apparent motion. Perception 13:149–54. [MRWD]CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kolers, P. & Pomerantz, J. R. (1971) Figural change in apparent motion. Journal of Experimental Psychology 87:99–108. [MRWD]CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kolers, P. & von, Grunau M. (1976) Shape and colour in apparent motion. Vision Research 16:329–35. [MRWD]CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Korn, H. & Faber, D. S. (1979) Electrical interactions between vertebrate neurons: Field effects and electrotonic coupling. In: The Neurosciences Fourth Study Program, ed. Schmitt, F. O. & Wordern, F. G.. MIT Press. [aGWS]Google Scholar
Kosslyn, S. M. (1978) Measuring the visual angle of the mind's eye. Cognitive Psychology 10:356–89. [aGWS]CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kosslyn, S. M. (1988) Aspects of a cognitive neuroscience of mental imagery. Science 240:1621–26. [aCWS]CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Krüger, J. & Aiple, F. (1988) Multimicroelectrode investigation of monkey striate cortex: Spike train correlations in the infragranular layers. Journal of Neurophysiology 60:798–827. [FA]CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Krueger, L. E. (1984) The category effect in visual search depends on physical rather than conceptual differences. Perception & Psychophysics 35:558–64. [BEB]CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Krumhansl, C. L. (1984) Independent processing of visual form and motion. Perception 13:535–46. [MBWD]CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kuipers, B. (1982) The “map in the head” metaphor. Environment and Behavior 14:202–20. [aGWS]CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kutas, M. & Hillyard, S. A. (1980) Reading senseless sentences: Brain potentials reflect semantic incongruity. Science 207:203–5. [MPY]CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lakoff, G. (1987) Women, fire, and dangerous things: What categories reveal about the mind. University of Chicago Press. [aCWS]CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lennie, P. & D'Zmura, M. (1988) Mechanisms of color vision. CRC Critical Reviews in Neurobiology 3:333–400. [CLH]Google ScholarPubMed
Livingstone, M. S. (1988) Art, illusion, and the visual system. Scientific American 258(1):78–85. [arCWS]CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Livingstone, M. S. & Hubel, D. H. (1985) Spatial relationship and extrafoveal vision. Nature 315:285. [arCWS]CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Livingstone, M. S. (1987) Psychophysical evidence for separate channels for the perception of form, color, movement, and depth. Journal of Neuroscience 7:3416–68. [MPY]CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Loftus, G. R. & Mackworth, N. H. (1978) Cognitive determinants of fixation location during picture viewing. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance 4:565–72. [JW]Google ScholarPubMed
Lowe, D. G. (1987) Perceptual organization and visual recognition. Kluwer Academic. [DCP]Google Scholar
Mackworth, N. H. & Morandi, A. J. (1967) The gaze selects informative details within pictures. Perception & Psychophysics 2:547–51. [JW]CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Marr, D. (1982) Vision: A computational investigation into the human representation and processing of visual information. W. H. Freeman. [MRWD, JKK, LEK, DCP, DMP, WP, RS, JW]Google Scholar
Marr, D. & Nishihara, H. K. (1978) Representation and recognition of the spatial organization of three-dimensional shapes. Proceedings of the Royal Society of London, Series B 200:269–94. [DCP]Google ScholarPubMed
Maunsell, J. H. R. & Newsome, W. T. (1987) Visual processing in monkey extrastriate cortex. Annual Review of Neuroscience 10:363–401. [JD, CF]CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
May, R. M. (1976) Simple mathematical models with very complicated dynamics. Nature 261:459–67. [MPY]CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
McCallum, W. C., Zappoli, R. & Denoth, F., eds. (1986) Cerebral psychophysiology: Studies in event-related potentials. Supplement 38 to Electroencephalography and Clinical Neurophysiology. Elsevier. [MPY]Google Scholar
McClelland, J. L. (1985) Putting knowledge in its place. Cognitive Science 9:1113–46. [PAS]CrossRefGoogle Scholar
McClelland, J. L. (1986) The programmable blackboard model of reading. In: Parallel distributed processing: Explorations in the microstructure of cognition, vol. 2, ed. McClelland, J. L. & Rumelhart, D. E.. Bradford Books/MIT Press. [HJM]Google Scholar
McCollough, C. (1965) Color adaptation of edge-detectors in the human visual system. Science 149:1115–16. [LEK]CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
McDermott, D. & Davis, E. (1984) Planning routes through uncertain territory. Artificial intelligence 22:107–56. [aCWS]CrossRefGoogle Scholar
McLean, J. P., Broadbent, D. E. & Broadbent, M. H. P. (1982) Combining attributes in rapid serial visual presentation tasks. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology 35A:171–86. [HJM, WP]CrossRefGoogle Scholar
McLeod, P., Driver, J. & Crisp, J. (1988) Visual search for a conjunction of movement and form is parallel. Nature (London) 332:154–55. [JD]CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Meredith, M. A. & Stein, B. E. (1983) Interactions among converging sensory inputs in the superior colliculus. Science 221:389–91. [aGWS]CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Meredith, M. A. (1985) Descending efferents from the superior colliculus relay integrated multisensory information. Science 227:657–59. [aGWS]CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Merikle, P. M. (1980) Selection from visual persistence by perceptuals groups and category membership. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Ceneral 109:279–95. [BEB]CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Mewhort, D. J. K., Butler, B. E., Feldman-Stewart, D. & Tramer, S. (1988) “Iconic memory,” location information, and the bar-probe task: A reply to Chow (1986). Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception & Performance 14:729–37. [BEB]Google Scholar
Mewhort, D. J. K., Campbell, A. J., Marchetti, F. M. & Campbell, J. I. D. (1981) Identification, localization, and “iconic memory”: An evaluation of the bar-probe task. Memory & Cognition 9:50–67. [BEB]CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Meyer, R. A., Raja, S. N. & Campbell, J. N. (1985) Coupling of action potential activity between unmyelinated fibers in the peripheral nerve of monkey. Science 227:184–87. [aGWS]CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Miller, G. A. (1956) The magical number seven, plus or minus two: Some limits on our capacity for processing information. Psychological Review 63:81–97. [rGWS]CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Mishkin, M., Ungerleider, L. C. & Macko, K. A. (1983) Object vision and spatial vision: Two cortical pathways. Trends in Neurosciences 6:414–17. [aGWS, BB]CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Moar, I. & Bower, G. H. (1983) Inconsistency in spatial knowledge. Memory and Cognition 11:107–13. [aGWS]CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Mountcastle, V. B. (1978) Brain mechanisms for directed attention. Journal of the Royal Society of Medicine 71:14–27. [aGWS]CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Monutcastle, V. B. (1979) An organizing principle for cerebral function: The unit module and the distributed system. In: The Neurosciences Fourth Study Program, ed. Schmitt, F. O. & Wordern, F. G.. MIT Press. [arGWS]Google Scholar
Mountcastle, V. B., Lynch, J. C., Ceorgopoulos, A.Sakata, H. & Acuna, C. (1975) Posterior parietal association cortex of the monkey: Command functions for operations within extrapersonal space. Journal of Neurophysiology 38:871–908. [DCP]CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Mozer, M. C. (1983) Letter migration in word perception. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance 9:531- 46. [HJM]Google ScholarPubMed
Mozer, M. C. (1988) A connectionist model of selective attention us visual perception. Proceedings of the Tenth Annual Conference of the Cognitive Science Society. [PAS]Google Scholar
Müller, H. J. & Findlay, J. M. (1987) Sensitivity and criterion effects in the spatial cuing of visual attention. Perception & Psychophysics 42:383–99. [HJM]CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Murphy, T. D. & Eriksen, C. W. (1987) Temporal changes in the distribution of attention in the visual field in response to precucs. Perception & Psychophysics 42:576–86. [CPW]CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Nakayama, K. & Silverman, C. H. (1986) Serial and parallel processing of visual feature conjunctions. Nature (London) 320:264–65. [JD, WP]CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Navon, D. (1976) Irrelevance of figural identity for resolving ambiguities in apparent motion. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance 2:130–38. [MRWD]Google ScholarPubMed
Navov, D. (1977) Forest before trees: The precedence of global features in visual perception. Cognitive Psychology 9:353–63. [LEK, DMP]Google Scholar
Navon, D. & Norman, J. (1983) Does global precedence really depend on visual angle? Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance 9:955–65. [DMP]Google ScholarPubMed
Neisser, U. & Becklen, R. (1975) Selective looking: Attending to visually specified events. Cognitive Psychology 7:480–94. [JD]CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Nelson, W. N. & Loftus, G. R. (1980) The functional visual field during picture viewing. Journal of Experimental Psychology: human Perception and Performance 6:391–99. [JW[]Google ScholarPubMed
Neumann, O., van, der Heijden A. H. C. & Allport, D. A. (1986) Visual selective attention: Introductory remarks. Psychological Research 48: 185–88. [BEB]CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Nissen, M. J. (1985) Accessing features and objects: Is location special? In: Attention and performance Xl, ed Posner, M. I. & Marin, O. S. M.. Erlbaum. [JD, WP]Google Scholar
O'Keefe, J. & Nadel, L. (1978) The hippocampus as a cognitive map. Clarendon Press. [aGWS]Google Scholar
O'Rourke, J. & Badler, N. I. (1980) Model-based image analysis of human motion using constraint propagation. IEEE Transactions on Pattern Analysis and Machine Intelligence PAMI-2:522–36. [aGWS]CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ottes, F. P., van, Gisbergen J. A. M. & Eggermont, J. J. (1984) Metrics of saccadic responses to double stimuli: Two different modes. Vision Research 24:1169–79. [JF]CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Paillard, J. (1987) Cognitive versus sensorimotor encoding of spatial information. In: Cognitive processes and spatial orientation in animal and man, ed. Ellen, P. & Thinus-Blanc, C.. Martinus Nijhoff. [BB]Google Scholar
Palacios, J. M., Cortes, R. & Probst, A. (1987) Receptor plasticity in the human brain: Some autoradiographic studies. Journal of Receptor Research 7:581–97. [aGWS]CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Palmer, S. E. (1975a) Visual perception and world knowledge: Notes on a model of sensory-cognitive interaction. In: Exploratioro in cognition, ed. Norman, D. A., Rumeihart, D. E. & the LRN Research Group. W. H. Freeman. [DCP]Google Scholar
Palmer, S. E. (1975b) The effects of contextual scenes on the identification of objects. Memory and Cognition 3:519–26. [DCP]CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Palmer, S. E. (1977) Hierarchical structure in perceptual representation. Cognitive Psychology 9:441–74. [DCP]CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Parker, D. M. (1980) Simple reaction times to the onset, offset and contrast reversal of sinusoidal grating stimuli. Perception and Psychophysics 28:365–68. [DMP]CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Parker, D. M. & Dutch, S. (1987) Perceptual latency and spatial frequency. Vision Research 27:1279–83. [DMP]CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Pashler, H. & Badgio, P. C. (1987) Attentional issues in the identification of alphanumeric characters. In: Attention and Performance XII, ed. Coitheart, M.. Erlbaum. [JD]Google Scholar
Pearson, J. C., Finkel, L. H. & Edelman, G. M. (1987) Plasticity in the organization of adult cerebral cortical maps: A computer simulation based on neuronal group selection. Journal of Neuroscience 7(12):4209–23. [MPY]CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Pellionisz, A. & Llinas, R. (1985) Tensor network theory of the metaorganization of functional geometrics in the central nervous system. Neuroscience 16:245–73. [JF]CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Perrett, D. I., Smith, P. A. J., Potter, D. D., Mistlin, A. J., Head, A. S., Milner, A. D. & Jeeves, M. A. (1984) Neurones responsive to faces in the temporal cortex: Studies of functional organization, sensitivity to identity, and relation to perception. Human Neurobiology 3:197–208.Google ScholarPubMed
Pinker, S. (1985) Visual cognition: An introduction. In: Visual cognition, ed. Pinker, S.. MIT Press. [DCP]Google Scholar
Pollatsek, A. & Digman, L. (1977) Dependent spatial channels in visual processing. Cognitive Psychology 9:326–52. [BEB]CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Posner, M. I. (1980) Orienting of attention. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology 32:3–25. [CPW]CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Posner, M. I., Cohen, Y. & Rafal, R. D. (1982) Neural systems control of spatial orienting. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society of London B 298:187–98. [aGWS]Google ScholarPubMed
Posner, M. I., Nissen, M. J. & Ogden, W. C. (1978) Attended and unattended processing modes: The role of set for spatial location. In: Modes of perception, ed. Pick, H. J. & Saltzman, I. J.. Erlbaum. [KBC]Google Scholar
Posner, M. I., Snyder, C. R. R. & Davidson, B. J. (1980) Attention and the detection of signals. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General 109:160–74. [KRC, CPW]CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Posner, M. I., Walker, J. A., Friedrich, F. J. & Rafal, R. D. (1984) Effects of parietal injury on covert orienting of attention. Journal of Neuroscience 4:1863–74. [FA]CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Post, W. M., Travis, C. C. & DeAngelis, D. L. (1985) Mutualism, limited competition and positive feedback. In: The biology of mutualism: Ecology and evolution, ed. Boucher, D. H.. Oxford University Press. [aGWS]Google Scholar
Prinzmetal, W. (1981) Principles of feature integration in visual perception. Perception and Psychophysics 30:330–40. [JD, LEK]CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Prinzmetal, W. & Keysar, B. (in press) A functional theory of illusory conjunctions and neon colors. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General. [WP]Google Scholar
Prinzmetal, W. & Millis-Wright, M. (1984) Cognitive and linguistic factors affect visual feature integration. Cognitive Psychology 16:305–40. [JD, WP]CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Prinzmetal, W., Presti, D. E. & Posner, M. I. (1986) Does attention affect visual feature integration? Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance 12:361–69. [CPW]Google ScholarPubMed
Prinzmetal, W., Treiman, R. & Rho, S. H. (1986) How to see a reading unit. Journal of Memory and Language 25:461–75. [WP]CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Pylyshyn, Z. W. & Storm, B. (1988) Tracking multiple independent targets: Evidence for a parallel tracking mechanism. Spatial Vision 3:1–19. [MRWD]CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Quinlan, P. T. (1987) Theoretical notes on “Parallel Models of Associative Memory. ” Cognitive Neuropsychology 4:333–64. [HJM]CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Rakic, P. (1988) Specification of cerebral cortical areas. Science 241:170–76. [aGWS]CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ramachandran, V. S., Ginsburg, A. P. & Anstis, S. (1983) Low spatial frequencies dominate apparent motion. Perception 12:457–61. [MRWD]CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rayner, K. (1984) Visual selection in reading, picture perception and visual search: A tutorial review. In: Attention and performance, ed. Bouman, H. & Bouwhuis, D. G.. Erlbaum. [JW]Google Scholar
Remington, R. W. & Pierce, L. (1984) Moving attention: Evidence for time- invariant shifts of visual selective attention. Perception & Psychophysics 35:393–99. [CPW]CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Richmond, B. J. & Goldberg, M. E. (1985) On computer science, visual science, and the physiological utility of models. Behavioral and Brain Sciences 8:300–301. [aGWS]CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Richmond, B. J. & Optican, L. M. (1987) Temporal encoding of two-dimensional patterns by single units in primate inferior temporal cortex. Journal of Neurophysiology 57(1): 132–78. [RS]CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Robinson, D. L., Goldberg, M. E. & Stanton, G. B. (1978) Parietal association area in the primate: Sensory mechanisms and behavioral modulations. Journal of Neurophysiology 41:910–32. [aGWS]CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Rock, I. & Gutman, D. (1981) The effect of inattention on form perception. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance 7:275–85. [HJM]Google ScholarPubMed
Rosch, E. (1978) Principles of categorization. In: Cognition and categorization, ed. Bosch, E. & Lloyd, B. B.. Erlbaum. [aGWS]Google Scholar
Rosenblatt, F. (1962) Principles of neurodynamics, perceptrons and the theory of brain mechanisms. Spartan Books. [aGWS]Google Scholar
Rumelhart, D. E., Hinton, G. E. & Williams, R. J. (1986) Learning internal representations by error propagation. In: Parallel distributed processing: Explorations in the microstructure of cognition, vol. 1, Foundations, ed melhart, D. E. Rum & McClelland, J. L.. MIT Press. [aCWS]CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Rumelhart, D. E. & McClelland, J. L. (1986) Parallel distributed processing: Explorations in the microstmucture of cognition, vols. 1 and 2. MIT Press. [aGWS]CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Rumelhart, D. E., Smolensky, P., McClelland, J. L. & Hinton, G. E. (1986) Schemata and sequential thought processes in PDP models. In: Parallel distributed processing, vol. 2, ed. McClelland, J. L. & Rumelhart, D. E., MIT Press. [JKK]CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Sagi, D. & Julesz, B. (1985) “Where” and “what” in vision. Science 228:1217–19. [aGWS, PAS, RS]CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Sandon, P. A. & Uhr, L. M. (1988) An adaptive model for viewpoint-invariant object recognition. Proceedings of the Tenth Annual Conference of the Cognitive Science Society. [PAS]Google Scholar
Schiffinan, H. R. (1982) Sensation and perception: An integrated approach, 2nd ed. Wiley. [LEK]Google Scholar
Schiller, P. H., Malpeli, J. G. & Schein, S. J. (1979) Composition of geniculostriate input to superior colliculus of the rhesus monkey. journal of Neurophysiology 42(4):1124–33. [rGWS]CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Schmitt, F. O. (1979) The role of structural, electrical, and chemical circuitry in brain function. In: The Neurosciences Fourth Study Program, ed. Schmitt, F. O. & Wordern, F. G.. MIT Press. [aGWS]Google Scholar
Schmitt, F. O., Worden, F. C., Adelman, G. & Dennis, S. G. (1981) The organization of the cerebral cortex. MIT Press. [rGWS]Google Scholar
Schneider, G. E. (1967) Contrasting visuomotor functions of tectum and cortex in golden hamster. Psycholgische Forschung 31:52–68. [BB]CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Schneider, W. (1988) Structure and controlling subsymbolic processing. Behavioral and Brain Sciences 11:15–52. [aGWS]CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Schoner, G. & Kelso, J. A. S. (1988) Dynamic pattern generation in behavioral and neural systems. Science 239:1513–20. [aGWS]CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Schreter, Z. (1988) Sequential processing by overlap and fistigue of memories. Neural Networks 1(Suppl. 1):218. [aGWS, JKK]CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Seidenberg, M. S. (1987) Sublexical structures in visual word recognition: Access units or orthographic redundancy? In: Attention and Performance XII: The psychology of reading, ed. Coltheart, M.. Erlbaum. [JHM]Google Scholar
Sejnowski, T. J. (1977) Storing covariance with nonlinearly interacting neurons. Journal of Mathematical Biology 4:303–21. [aGWS]CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Sejnowski, T. J. (1981) Skeleton filters in the brain. In: Parallel models of associative memory, ed. Hinton, G. E.. Erlbaum. [aGWS]Google Scholar
Sejnowski, T. J., Koch, C. & Churchland, P. S. (1988) Computational neuroscience. Science 241:1299–1306. [aGWS]CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Selverston, A. & Moulin, M. (1987) The crustacean stomatogastric system. Springer-Verlag. [RS]CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Shastri, L. (1988) A connectionist approach to knowledge representation and limited inference. Cognitive Science 12:331–92. [JRL]CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Shepard, R. N. (1984) Ecological constraints on internal representation: Resonant kinematics of perceiving, imagining, thinking, and dreaming. Psychological Review 91(4):417–47. [rGWS]CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Shepherd, M., Findlay, J. M. & Hockey, R. J. (1986) The relationship between eye movements and spatial attention. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology 38A:475–91. [CPW]CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Shulmsn, G. L., Wilson, J. & Sheehy, J. B. (1985) Spatial determinants of the distribution of attention. Perception & Psychophysics 37:59–65. [CPW]CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Smolensky, P. (1986) Information processing in dynamical systems: Foundations of harmony theory. In: Parallel distributed processing: Explorations in the microslructure of cognition, vol. 1, Foundations, ed. Rumelhart, D. E. & MeClelland, J. L.. MIT Press. [aGWS, JKK]Google Scholar
Smolensky, P. (1988) On the proper treatment of connectionism. Behavioral and Brain Sciences 11:1–74. [aGWS, MPY]CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Snyder, C. R. R. (1972) Selection, inspection and naming in visual search. Journal of Experimental Psychology 92:428–31. [BEB, WP]CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Sparks, D. L. & Jay, M. (1987) The role of the primate superior colliculus in sensorimotor integration. In: Vision, brain, and cooperative computation, ed. Arbib, M. A. & Hanson, A. R.. MIT Press. [aGWS, RS]Google Scholar
Sperling, G. (1960) The information available in brief visual presentation. Psychological Monographs 74:11, Whole No. 498. [BEB]CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Sperling, G. (1967) Successive approximations to a model for short-term memory. In: Attention and performance I, ed. Sanders, A. F.. North-Holland. [JD]Google Scholar
Strong, G. W. & Whitehead, B. A. (1987) A factored architecture to solve illusory conjunctions in neural networks. In: Proceedings of the first annual international conference on neural networks, ed Caudill, M. & Butler, C.. IEEE Service Center. [aGWS]Google Scholar
Strong, G. W. (in preparation) The problem of population turnover in genetic neural models. [rGWS]Google Scholar
Sun, R., Marder, E. & Waltz, D. (1988) The modeling of lobster stomatogastric ganglions. Technical Report CS-88–134. Computer Science Department, Brandeis University [RS]Google Scholar
Tolman, E. C. (1948) Cognitive maps in rats and men. Psychological Review 55:189–208. [rGWS]CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Touretzky, D. S. & Hinton, G. E. (1988) A distributed connectionist production system. Cognitive Science 12:423–66. [JRL]CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Treisman, A. (1985) Preattentive processing in vision. Computer Vision, Graphics and Image Processing 31:156–77. [rGWS]CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Treisman, A. (1988) Features and objects: The fourteenth Bartlett memorial lecture. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology 40A:201–37. [JD]CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Treisman, A. M. (1982) Perceptual grouping and attention in visual search for features and for objects. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance 8:194–214. [CPW]Google ScholarPubMed
Treisman, A. M. (1986) Features and objects in visual processing. Scientific American 255(5):106–15. [aGWS, CLH]CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Treisman, A. M. & Gelade, G. (1980) A feature-integration theory of attention. Cognitive Psychology 12:97–136. [aGWS, BEB, KRC, JD, CLH, PAS, CPW]CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Treisman, A. M. & Gormican, S. (1988) Feature analysis in early vision: Evidence from search asymmetries. Psychological Review 95:15–48. [aGWS, LEK, CPW]CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Treisman, A. M. & Schmidt, H. (1982) Illusory conjunctions in the perception of objects. Cognitive Psychology 14:107–41. [BEB, KRC, HJM, CPW]CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Treisman, A. M. & Souther, J. (1985) Search asymmetry: A diagnostic for preattentive processing of separable features. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General 114:285–310. [JD, HJM]CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Treisman, A. M. (1986) Illusory words: The roles of attention and of top-down constraints in conjoining letters to ftrm words. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception & Performance 12:3–17. [BEB]Google Scholar
Trevarthen, C. (1968) Two mechanisms of vision in primates. Psychologische Forschung 31:299–337. [BB]CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Trevarthen, C. (1978) Modes of perceiving and modes of acting. In: Modes of perceiving and processing information, ed. Pick, H. L. Jr, & Saltzman, E.. Erlbaum. [LEK]Google Scholar
Tsal, Y. (1983) Movements of attention across the visual field. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance 9:523- 30. [CPW]Google ScholarPubMed
Tsotsos, J. K. (1987) Representational axes and temporal cooperative processes. In: Vision, brain, and cooperative computation, ed. Arbib, M. A. & Hanson, A. R.. MIT Press.[aGWS]Google Scholar
Tsotsos, J. K. (1987a) Analyzing vision at the complexity level. Technical Report RCVBTR-87–20. University of Toronto. [PAS]Google Scholar
Uhr, L. M. (1980) Psychological motivation and underlying concepts. In: Structured computer vision, ed. Tanimoto, S. L. & Klinger, A.. Academic Press. [PAS]Google Scholar
Ullman, S. (1979) The interpretation of visual motion. MIT Press. [rGWS, MRWD]CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ullman, S. (1980) The effect of similarity between line segments on the correspondence strength in apparent motion. Perception 9:617–26. [MRWD]CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ungerleider, L. C. & Mishkin, M. (1982) In: Analysis of visual behavior, ed Ingle, D. J., Goodale, M. A., & Mansfield, R. J. W.. MIT Press. [aGWS]Google Scholar
Van, Essen D. C. & Maunsell, J. H. R. (1983) Hierarchical organization and functional streams in the visual cortex. Trends in Nec rosclence 6:370–75. [PAS]Google Scholar
Vaughan, H. C.Jr, Costa, L. & Gilden, L. (1966) The functional relation of visual evoked response and reaction time to stimulus intensity. Vision Research 6:645–56. [DMP]CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Verfaillie, K. & Wagemans, J. (1987) What eye movements can tell about functional vision in Usher syndrome. Paper presented at the 9th Conference of the International Associotion for the Education of the Deaf/Blind, Poitiers, France. [JW]Google Scholar
Virzi, B. A. & Egeth, H. E. (1984) Is meaning implicated in illusory conjunctions? Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance 10:573–80. [HIM, CPW]Google ScholarPubMed
von, der Malsburg C. (1975) Nervous structures with dynamical links. Berichte Bunsenges Physikalische Chemie 89:703–9. [sGWS]Google Scholar
von, der Malsburg C. & Cowan, J. D. (1982) Outline of a theory for the ontogenesis of iso-orientation domains in visual cortex. Biological Cybernetics 45:49–56. [aGWS]Google Scholar
von, Holst E. (1954) Relations between the central nervous system and the peripheral organs. British Journal of Animal Behaviour 2:89–94. [LEK]Google Scholar
von, Wright J. M. (1968) Selection in visual immediate memory. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology 20:62–68. [BEB]Google Scholar
von, Wright J. M. (1970) On selection in visual immediate memory. Ada Psychologia 33:280–92. [BEB]Google Scholar
Warren, R. M. & Warren, R. P. (1968) Helmholtz on perception: Its physiology and development. Wiley. [JD]Google Scholar
Watt, R. J. (1987) Scanning from coarse to fine spatial in the human visual system after the onset of a stimulus. Journal of the Optical Society of America A4:2006–21 [CPW]CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Weiskrantz, L. (1987) Residual vision in a scotoma. A follow-up study of ‘form’ discrimination. Brain 110:77–92. [BB]CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Weisstein, N. & Harris, C. S. (1974) Visual detection of line segmeists: An object-superiority effect. Science 186:752–55. [JW]CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
White, C. W. (1976) Visual masking during smooth pursuit eye movements. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance 2:469–78. [aGWS]Google Scholar
Whitehead, B. A. & Strong, G. W. (1982) Spatial image computations required for a semantic network. In: A general survey of systems methodology, ed. Troncale, L.. Proceedings of the 26th North American Meeting of the Society for General Systems Research. Louisville: Society fur General Systems Research. [rGWS]Google Scholar
Whitehead, B. A. (1988) A neural network to register invariants in object-relative position under observer locomotion. In: Abstracts of the International Neural Network Society First Annual Meeting, Boston. Neural Networks 1(Suppl. 1):530. [arGWS]Google Scholar
Wickelgren, W. A. (1969) Context-sensitive coding, associative memory, and serial order in (speech) behavior. Psychological Review 76:1–15. [JD]CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Wickelgren, W. A. (1979) Chunking and consolidation: A theoretical synthesis of semantic networks, configuring in conditioning, S-R versus cognitive learning, normal forgetting, the amnesic syndrome, and the hippocampal arousal system. Psychological Review 86:44–60. [aGWS]CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Wise, S. P. & Desimone, R. (1988) Behavioral neurophysiology: Insights into seeing and grasping. Science 242:736–41. [PAS]CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Wolfe, J. M., Cave, K. B. & Franzel, S. L. (in press) A modified feature integration model for visual search. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance. [JD]Google Scholar
Wolford, G. (1975) Perturbation model for letter identification. Psychological Review 82:184–99. [PAS]CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Wolford, G. & Shum, K. H. (1980) Evidence for feature perturbations. Perception & Psychophysics 27:409–20. [WP, PAS]CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Wong, E. & Mack, A. (1981) Saccadic programming and perceived location. Acta Psychologica 48:123–31. [BB]CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Wurtz, B. H., Goldberg, M. E. & Robinson, E. L. (1980) Behavioral modulation of visual responses in the monkey: Stimulus selection for attention and movement. Progress in Psychobiology and Physiological Psychology 9:43–83. [aGWS]Google Scholar
Zeki, S. & Shipp, S. (1988) The functional logic of cortical connections. Nature 335:311–17. [MPY]CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed