Hostname: page-component-78c5997874-fbnjt Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-11-15T03:22:14.049Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Somatosensory processing subserving perception and action: Dissociations, interactions, and integration

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  20 August 2007

H. Chris Dijkerman
Affiliation:
Department of Experimental Psychology, Helmholtz Research Institute, Utrecht University, 3584 CS Utrecht, The Netherlands. [email protected]@uu.nl
Edward H. F. de Haan
Affiliation:
Department of Experimental Psychology, Helmholtz Research Institute, Utrecht University, 3584 CS Utrecht, The Netherlands. [email protected]@uu.nl

Abstract

The commentaries have raised important points regarding different aspects of our model. Some have queried the nature of the proposed dissociations, whereas others have requested and provided further details regarding aspects we had glossed over. Here we suggest that our approach to identify major processing streams based on the processing goal does not preclude interactions between them. We further specify details regarding body representations, haptic object recognition, and crossmodal processing, but are also aware that several features of the model require further filling in.

Type
Authors' Response
Copyright
Copyright © Cambridge University Press 2007

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

Anema, H. A., Kessels, R. P. & Dijkerman, C. (2006) Investigations of impairment in finger representation in a patient with Gerstmann's syndrome. Poster presented at the Second Meeting of the European Societies of Neuropsychology, Toulouse, France, October 18–20, 2006.Google Scholar
Anema, H. A., Overvliet, K., Smeets, J. B. & Dijkerman, H. C. (submitted a) Integration of tactile input across fingers in a patient with Gerstmann's syndrome.Google Scholar
Anema, H. A., Wolswijk, V. W. J., Ruis, C. & Dijkerman, H. C. (submitted b) Task dependent effects of Weber's illusion.Google Scholar
Arzy, S., Overney, L. S., Landis, T. & Blanke, O. (2006a) Neural mechanisms of embodiment: Asomatognosia due to premotor cortex damage. Archives of Neurology 63(7):1022–25.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Benedetti, F. (1985) Processing of tactile spatial information with crossed fingers. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance 11(4):517–25.Google ScholarPubMed
Benedetti, F. (1986) Spatial organization of the diplesthetic and nondiplesthetic areas of the fingers. Perception 15(3):285301.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Buxbaum, L. J. & Coslett, H. B. (2001) Specialised structural descriptions for human body parts: Evidence from autotopagnosia. Cognitive Neuropsychology 18(4):289306.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Buxbaum, L. J., Giovanetti, T. & Libon, D. (2000) The role of the dynamic body schema in praxis: Evidence from primary progressive apraxia. Brain 44(2):166–91.Google ScholarPubMed
Carey, D. P., Dijkerman, H. C., Murphy, K. J., Goodale, M. A. & Milner, A. D. (2006) Pointing to places and spaces in a patient with visual form agnosia. Neuropsychologia 44(9):1584–94.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Chaminade, T., Meltzoff, A. & Decety, J. (2005) An fMRI study of imitation: Action representation and body schema. Neuropsychologia 43(1):115–27.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Craig, A. D. (2002) How do you feel? Interoception: The sense of the physiological condition of the body. Nature Reviews: Neuroscience 3:655–66.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Culham, J. C., Danckert, S. L., DeSouza, J. F., Gati, J. S., Menon, R. S. & Goodale, M. A. (2003) Visually guided grasping produces fMRI activation in dorsal but not ventral stream brain areas. Experimental Brain Research 153(2):180–89.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Debowy, D. J., Ghosh, S., Ro, J. Y. & Gardner, E. P. (2001) Comparison of neuronal firing rates in somatosensory and posterior parietal cortex during prehension. Experimental Brain Research 137(3-4):269–91.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Dijkerman, H. C., Milner, A. D. & Carey, D. P. (1998) Grasping spatial relationships: Failure to demonstrate allocentric visual coding in a patient with visual form agnosia. Consciousness and Cognition 7(3):424–37.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Dijkerman, H. C., Webeling, M., Ter Wal, J. M., Groet, E. & Van Zandvoort, M.J. (2004) A long-lasting improvement of somatosensory function after prism adaptation: A case study. Neuropsychologia 42(12):1697–702.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ehrsson, H. H., Holmes, N. P. & Passingham, R. E. (2005a) Touching a rubber hand: Feeling of body ownership is associated with activity in multisensory brain areas. Journal of Neuroscience 25(45):10564–73.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ehrsson, H. H., Spence, C. & Passingham, R. E. (2004) That's my hand! Activity in premotor cortex reflects feeling of ownership of a limb. Science 305(5685):875–77.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gallagher, S. (2005) How the body shapes the mind. Oxford University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gardner, E. P., Ro, J. Y., Debowy, D. & Ghosh, S. (1999) Facilitation of neuronal activity in somatosensory and posterior parietal cortex during prehension. Experimental Brain Research 127(4):329–54.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gibson, J. J. (1962) Observations on active touch. Psychological Review 69:477–91.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Goldenberg, G. (1996) Defective imitation of gestures in patients with damage in the left or the right hemisphere. Journal of Neurology, Neurosurgery and Neuropsychiatry 61(2):176–80.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Goodale, M. A., Jakobson, L. S. & Keillor, J. M. (1994) Differences in the visual control of pantomimed and natural grasping movements. Neuropsychologia 32:1159–78.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
James, T. W., Culham, J., Humphrey, G. K., Milner, A. D. & Goodale, M. A. (2003) Ventral occipital lesions impair object recognition but not object-directed grasping: An fMRI study. Brain 126(Pt 11):2463–75.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kammers, M. P. M., De Vignemont, F., Verhagen, L. & Dijkerman, H. C. (submitted) The non-erasable rubber hand illusion: The effect of the rubber hand illusion is dependent on the task at hand.Google Scholar
Kammers, M. P. M., van der Ham, I. J. M. & Dijkerman, H. C. (2006) Dissociating body representations in healthy individuals: Differential effects of a kinaesthetic illusion depending on body representations. Neuropsychologia 44(12):2430–36.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lederman, S. J. & Klatzky, R. L. (1990) Haptic classification of common objects: Knowledge-driven exploration. Cognitive Psychology 22(4):421–59.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Marcel, A. (2003) The sense of agency: Awareness and ownership of action. In: Agency and self-awareness, ed. Roessler, J. & Eilan, N.. Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Milner, A. D., Dijkerman, H. C., McIntosh, R. D., Rossetti, Y. & Pisella, L. (2003) Delayed reaching and grasping in patients with optic ataxia. Progress in Brain Research 142:225–42.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Milner, A. D. & Goodale, M. A. (1995) The visual brain in action. Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Murphy, K. J., Carey, D. P. & Goodale, M. A. (1998) The perception of spatial relationships in a patient with visual form agnosia. Cognitive Neuropsychology 15:705–22.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Newell, F. N., Ernst, M. O., Tjan, B. J. & Bülthoff, H. H. (2001) Viewpoint dependence in visual and haptic object recognition. Psychological Science 12(1):3742.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Olausson, H., Lamarre, Y., Backlund, H., Morin, C., Wallin, B. G., Starck, G., Ekholm, S., Strigo, I., Worsley, K., Vallbo, A. B. & Bushnell, M. C. (2002) Unmyelinated tactile afferents signal touch and project to insular cortex. Nature Neuroscience 5(9):900904.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Paillard, J. (1999) Body schema and body image: A double dissociation in deafferented patients. In: Motor control, today and tomorrow, ed. Gantchev, G. N., Mori, S. & Massion, J., pp. 197214. Academic Publishing House.Google Scholar
Pelisson, D., Prablanc, C., Goodale, M. A. & Jeannerod, M. (1986) Visual control of reaching movements without vision of the limb: II. Evidence of fast unconscious processes correcting the trajectory of the hand to the final position of a double-step stimulus. Experimental Brain Research 62(2):303–11.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ro, J. Y., Debowy, D., Ghosh, S. & Gardner, E. P. (2000) Depression of neuronal firing rates in somatosensory and posterior parietal cortex during object acquisition in a prehension task. Experimental Brain Research 135(1):111.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Rossetti, Y., Rode, G. & Boisson, D. (2001) Numbsense: A case study and implications. In: Out of mind: Varieties of unconscious processing, ed. de Gelder, B., De Haan, E. H. F. & Heywood, C. A.. Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Sathian, K. (2005) Visual cortical activity during tactile perception in the sighted and visually deprived. Developmental Psychobiology 46(3):279–86.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Schenk, T. (2006) An allocentric rather than perceptual deficit in patient D.F. Nature Neuroscience 9(11):1369–70.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Schwoebel, J. & Coslett, H. B. (2005) Evidence for multiple, distinct representations of the human body. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience 17(4):543–53.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Tsakiris, M., Prabhu, G. & Haggard, P. (2006) Having a body versus moving your body: How agency structures body-ownership. Consciousness and Cognition 15(2):423–32.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Westwood, D. A. & Goodale, M. A. (2003) A haptic size-contrast illusion affects size perception but not grasping. Experimental Brain Research 153:253–59.CrossRefGoogle Scholar