No CrossRef data available.
Article contents
Psychology and computational architecture
Published online by Cambridge University Press: 04 February 2010
Abstract
An abstract is not available for this content so a preview has been provided. Please use the Get access link above for information on how to access this content.
- Type
- Open Peer Commentary
- Information
- Copyright
- Copyright © Cambridge University Press 1980
References
Anderson, J. R. (1978) Arguments concerning representations for mental imagery. Psychological Review 85:249–277. [JRM, ZP]Google Scholar
Anderson, P.; Cross, G. N.; Lomo, T.; and Green, O. (1969) In: Brazier, M. (ed.) The Interneuron. Los Angeles: Univ. of California Press. [SG]Google Scholar
Arbib, M., and Caplan, D. (in press) Neurolinguistics must be computational. Behavioral and Brain Sciences. [ZP]Google Scholar
Armstrong, D. M. (1973) Belief, truth and knowledge, New York: Cambridge University Press. [JHe]CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Atkinson, H. C., and Shiffrin, R. M. (1968) Human memory: a proposed system and its control processes. In: Spence, K. W. and Spence, J. T. (eds.) Advances in the psychology of learning and motivation, research and theory, Vol. II, New York: Academic Press. [SG]Google Scholar
Atkinson, H. C., and Shiffrin, R. M. (1971) The control of short term memory. Scientific American 225:82–90. [SG]CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Austin, H. (12, 1974) A computational view of the skill of juggling. AI memo 330, Artificial Intelligence Laboratory, M.I.T. [ZP]Google Scholar
Backus, J. (1978) Can programming be liberated from the von Neumann style? A functional style and its algebra of programs. 21:613–641. [ZP]CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Baddeley, A. D. (1980) Cognitive psychology and psychometric theory. In: Freidman, M., Das, J. P., and O'Connor, M. (eds.) Intelligence and learning. New York: Plenum Press. [HEK]Google Scholar
Baddeley, A. D. and Warrington, E. K. (1970) Amnesia and the distinction between long- and short-term memory. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior 9:176–189. [SG]Google Scholar
Bernstein, N. (1967) The coordination and regulation of movements. New York: Pergamon. [ZP]Google Scholar
Bloomfield, T. M., (1969) Behavioral contrast and the peak shift. In: Gilbert, H. M., and Sutherland, N. S. (eds.) Animal discrimination learning. New York: Academic Press. [SG]Google Scholar
Bobrow, D. G., and Raphael, B. R. (1974) New programming languages for A.I. research. ACM Computer Surveys 6:153–174. [ZP]Google Scholar
Brentano, F. (1960) The distinction between mental and physical phenomena, Trans. Terrell, D. B.. In: Chisholm, R. (ed.) Realism and the background of phenomenology. New York: The Free Press. [JHe]Google Scholar
Brewer, W. F. (1974) There is no convincing evidence for operant or classical conditioning in adult humans. In: Weiner, W. B., and Palermo, D. S. (eds.) Cognition and the symbolic processes. Hillsdale, N.J.: Lawrence Erlbaum. [ZP]Google Scholar
Brouwer, L. E. J. (1964) Intuitionism and formalism. In: Benacerraf, P., and Putnam, H. (eds.) Philosophy of Mathematics, Englewood Cliffs, N.J.: Prentice-Hall. [ZP]Google Scholar
Burner, J. S. (1957) On perceptual readiness. Psychological Review 64:123–152. [ZP]CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Buell, S. J., and Coleman, P. D. (1979) Dendritic growth in the aged human brain and failure of growth in senile dementia. Science 206:854. [PMC]Google Scholar
Butterfield, H. (1957) The origins of modern science: 1300–1800. Toronto: Clark Irwin & Co. [ZP]Google Scholar
Carroll, , Lewis, (1956) “What the Tortoise Said to Achilles,” Mind 4, 1895; reprinted inGoogle Scholar
Newman, , James, R., The World of Mathematics, pp. 2402–2405. New York: Simon and Schuster. [GR]Google Scholar
Churchland, P. M. (1979) Scientific realism and the plasticity of mind, ch. 5. New York: Cambridge Univ. Press. [PMC]Google Scholar
Churchland, P. M. (forthcoming) review of Persons and minds, by Margolis, J.. Dialogue. [PSC]Google Scholar
Colby, K. M. (1978) Mind models: an overview of current work. Mathematical Biosciences 39:159–185. [KMC]Google Scholar
Collins, A. M., and Loftus, E. L. (1975) A spreading activation theory of semantic processing. Psychological Review 82:407–428. [JRM]Google Scholar
Creutzfeldt, O. (1976) The brain as a functional entity. In: Corner, M. A., and Swaab, D. F. (eds.) Perspective in brain research: progress in brain research, Vol. 45, Amsterdam: Elsevier/North Holland Biomedical Press. [SG]Google Scholar
Davidson, D. (1970) Mental events. In: Foster, L., and Swanson, V. W. (eds.) Experience and theory. Amherst: Univ. of Massachusetts Press. (1970) [WBW, ZP]Google Scholar
Dennett, D. C. (in press) True believers: the intentional strategy and why it works. To appear in a volume of the 1979 Herbert Spencer Lectures on Scientific Explanation, Oxford Univ. Press. [ZP]Google Scholar
Donders, F. C. (1969) On the speed of mental processes (1868–1869). Acta Psychologica 30:412–431. [ZP]Google Scholar
Fahlman, S. E. (1979) NETL: A system for representing and using real world knowledge. Cambridge, Mass.: MIT Press. [ZP]Google Scholar
Favreau, O. (1976) Interference in color-contingent motion after-effects. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology 28:553–560. [SWZ]Google Scholar
Feigenbaum, E. A., and Simon, H. A. (1962) A theory of the serial position effect. British Journal of Psychology 53:307–320. [SG]Google Scholar
Feigenbaum, E. A., and Simon, H. A. (1963) Brief notes on the EPAM theory of verbal learning. In: Cofer, C. N., and Musgrave, B. S. (eds.) Verbal behavior and learning. New York: McGraw-Hill. [SG]Google Scholar
Finke, R. A. (in press) Levels of equivalence in imagery and perception. Psychological Review. [SP]Google Scholar
Fodor, J. A. (1975) The Language of Thought. New York: Thomas Y. Crowell. [PMC, J.He, FK, GR, ZP]Google Scholar
Fodor, J. A. (1978) Tom Swift and his procedural grandmother. Cognition 6:229–247. [HEK, ZP]CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Fortescue, M. D. (1979) Why the “language of thought” is not a language: some inconsistencies of the computational analogy of thought. Journal of Pragmatics 3:67–80. [MF]Google Scholar
Funt, B. V. (1977) Whisper: a problem-solving system utilizing diagrams. Proceedings of the Fifth International Joint Conference on Artificial Intelligence, (IJCAI-77), pp. 459–464. Department of Computer Science, Carnegie-Mellon University. [ZP]Google Scholar
Gibson, J. J. (1979) The ecological approach to visual perception. Boston: Houghton Mifflin Co. [JHe, ZP]Google Scholar
Gold, E. (1967) Language identification in the limit. Information and Control 16:447–474. [SWZ]Google Scholar
Grossberg, S. (1969) On the serial learning of lists. Mathematical Biosciences 4:201–253. [SG]Google Scholar
Grossberg, S.Contour enhancement, short term memory, and constancies in reverberating neural networks. Studies in Applied Math 52:217–257. [SG]Google Scholar
Grossberg, S. (1974) Classical and instrumental learning by neural networks. In: Rosen, R., and Snell, F. (eds.) Progress in Theoretical Biology, Vol. 3. New York: Academic Press. [SG]Google Scholar
Grossberg, S. (1975) A neural model of attention, reinforcement, and discrimination learning. In: Pfeiffer, C. (ed.) International Review of Neurobiology 18:263–327. [SG]CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Grossberg, S. (1978a) Behavioral contrast in short term memory: serial binary memory models or parallel continuous memory models? Journal of Mathematical Psychology 17:199–219. [SG]Google Scholar
Grossberg, S. (1978b) A theory of human memory: self-organization and performance of sensory-motor codes, maps, and plans. In: Rosen, R., and Snell, F. (eds.) Progress in Theoretical Biology, Vol. 5, New York: Academic Press. [SG]Google Scholar
Grossberg, S. (1978c) Decisions, patterns, and oscillations in the dynamics of competitive systems with applications to Volterra-Lotka systems. Journal of Theoretical Biology 73:101–130. [SG]Google Scholar
Grossberg, S. (1978d) Competition, decision, and consensus. Journal of Mathematical Analyses and Applications 66:470–493. [SG]Google Scholar
Grossberg, S. (1980) How does a brain build a cognitive code? Psychological Review, 87:1–51. [SG]Google Scholar
Grossberg, S. and Pepe, J. (1971) Spiking threshold and overarousal effects in serial learning. Journal of Statistical Physics 3:95–125. [SG]CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Guzman, A. (1968) Computer recognition of three-dimensional objects in a visual scene. MIT Artificial Intelligence Laboratory Report AI-TR-228 (thesis). [ZP]Google Scholar
Hanson, N. R. (1965) Patterns of Discovery. Cambridge, England: Cambridge Univ. Press. [ZP]Google Scholar
Haugeland, J. (1978) The nature and plausibility of cognitivism. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 2, 215–260. [JHa, RCM, WES, ZP]Google Scholar
Hochberg, J. (1968) In the mind's eye. In: Haber, R. N. (ed.) Contemporary theory and research in visual perception. New York: Holt, Rinehardt & Winston. [ZP]Google Scholar
Hogan, R. M., and Hogan, M. M. (1975) Structural and transient components of memory. Memory and Cognition 3:210–215. [SG]Google Scholar
Howard, I. P. (1978) Recognition and knowledge of the water-level principle. Perception 7:151–160. [ZP]Google Scholar
Hunt, E. (1978) Imageful thought. In: Cotton, J. W., and Klatzky, R. W. (eds.) Semantic factors in cognition. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum. [WES]Google Scholar
Hunt, E. B. (1980a) The design of a robot mind: a theoretical approach to the issues in intelligence. In: Friedman, M., Das, J. P., and O'Connor, M. (eds.) Intelligence and learning. New York: Plenum Press. [EH]Google Scholar
Hunt, E. B. (1980b) Intelligence as an information processing concept. Bntish Journal of Psychology. [EH]Google Scholar
Klatzky, R. L., and Stoy, A. M. (1978) Semantic information and visual information processing. In: Cotton, J. W. and Klatzky, R. L. (eds.) Semantic factors in cognition. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum. [RLK]Google Scholar
Kolers, P. A., and Smythe, W. E. (1979a) Images, symbols, and skills. Canadian Journal of Psychology 33:158–184. [WES]Google Scholar
Kolers, P. A., and Smythe, W. E. (1979b) Aspects of picturing. In: Nodine, C. F., and Fisher, D. F. (eds.) Perception and pictorial representation. New York: Praeger Publishers. [WES]Google Scholar
Kosslyn, S. M. (in press) Image and mind. Cambridge. Mass.: Harvard Univ. Press. [SP, ZP]Google Scholar
Kosslyn, S. M. and Shwartz, S. P. (1977) A data-driven simulation of visual imagery. Cognitive Science 1:265–296. [ZP]Google Scholar
Kosslyn, S. M.; Pinker, S.; Smith, G. E.; and Schwartz, S. P. (in press, a) On the demystification of mental imagery. The Behavioral and Brain Sciences 2:535–581. (1979) [SP, ZP]Google Scholar
Kosslyn, S. M.; Pinker, S.; Smith, G. E.; and Schwartz, S. P. (in press, b) The how, what, and why of mental imagery. The Behavioral and Brain Sciences 2:570–579. (1979) [SP]Google Scholar
Kripke, S. (1972) Naming and necessity. In: Davidson, D. and Harman, G. (eds.) Semantics of natural language. Dordrecht: Reidel. [GR]Google Scholar
Kripke, S. (1975) Wittgenstein on rules and private language. Transcript of a talk presented at the Wittgenstein Colloquium, University of Western Ontario, London, Canada (mimeo). [ZP]Google Scholar
Kyburg, H. (1974) The logical foundations of statistical Inference. Dordrecht: Reidel. [HEK]CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lee, C. L., and Eates, W. K. (1977) Order and position in primary memory for letter strings. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior 16:395–418. [SG]Google Scholar
Llinas, R., and Pellionisz, A. (1979) Brain modeling by tensor network theory and computer simulation. The cerebellum: distributed processor for predictive coordination. Neuroscience 4. [PSC]CrossRefGoogle Scholar
MacLeod, C. M.: Dekaban, A. S.; and Hunt, E. (1978) Memory impairment in epileptic patients: selective effects of phenobarbital concentration. Science 202:1102–1104. [EH]Google Scholar
Marcus, M. (1979) A theory of syntactic recognition for natural language. Cambridge, Mass.: MIT Press. [ZP, SWZ]Google Scholar
Marr, D. (1975) Approaches to biological information processing. Science 190:875–876. [ZP]CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Marr, D. (1977) Artificial intelligence – a personal view. Artificial Intelligence 9:37–48. [SWZ]Google Scholar
Marr, D. (1979) Representing visual information. In: Hanson, M. A., and Riseman, E. (eds.) Computer Vision Systems. New York: Academic Press. [ZP]Google Scholar
Marr, D. and Nishihara, H. K. (1977) Representation and recognition of the spatial organization of three-dimensional shapes. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B 200:269–294. [ZP]Google Scholar
McCarthy, J., and Hayes, P. (1969) Some philosophical problems from the standpoint of artificial intelligence. In: Meltzer, B., and Michie, D. (eds.) Machine intelligence 4. Edinburg: Edinburgh Univ. Press. [ZP]Google Scholar
Miller, G. A.; Galanter, E.; and Pribram, K. (1960) Plans and the structure of behavior. N.Y.: Holt, Rinehart and Winston. [ZP]Google Scholar
Morton, J. A. (1970) A functional model of memory. In: Norman, D. A. (ed.) Models of human memory. New York: Academic Press. [ZP]Google Scholar
Mountcastle, , Vernon, B. (1978) Brain mechanisms for directed attention. Journal of the Royal Society of Medicine 71. [PSC]Google Scholar
Newell, A. (1973) Production systems: models of control structures. In: Chase, W. C. (ed.) Visual information processing. N.Y.: Academic. [ZP, SWZ]Google Scholar
Newell, A. (in press-a) The physical symbol system hypothesis. In: Norman, D. A. (ed.) Perspectives on Cognitive Science: Talks from the La Jolla Conference. [ZP]Google Scholar
Newell, A. (in press-b) Harpy, production systems and human cognition. In: Cole, R. A. (ed.) Perception and Production of Fluent Speech. Hillsdale, N.J.: Lawrence Erlbaum Press. [ZP]Google Scholar
Newell, A. and Simon, H. A. (1972) Human problem solving. Englewood Cliffs, N.J.: Prentice-Hall. [ZP]Google Scholar
Nicod, J. (1970) Geometry and induction. Berkeley, Calif.: Univ. of California Press. [ZP]Google Scholar
Norman, D. A. (1968) Toward a theory of memory and attention. Psychological Review 75:522–536. [ZP]Google Scholar
Norman, D. A. (1969) Memory and attention: an introduction to human Information processing. N.Y.: Wiley. [ZP]Google Scholar
Palmer, S. F. (1978) Fundamental aspects of cognitive representation. In: Rosch, E. H., and Lloyd, B. B. (eds.) Cognition and categorization. Hillsdale, N.J.: Erlbaum. [ZP]Google Scholar
Pike, K. L. (1967) Language in relation to a unified theory of the structure of human behavior. The Hague: Mouton. [ZP]CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Poltrock, S. (1977) Models of temporal order discrimination. University of Washington Doctoral Dissertation. [EH]Google Scholar
Poltrock, S. and Hunt, E. B. (1977) Individual differences in phonological fusion and separation errors. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 3:62–74. [EH]Google Scholar
Posner, M. I., and Snyder, C. R. (1975) Attention and cognitive control. In: Solso, R. L. (ed.) Information processing and cognition. Hillsdale, N.J.: Lawrence Erlbaum. [ZP]Google Scholar
Powers, W. T. (1979) A cybernetic model for research in human development. In: M., Ozer (ed.) A cybernetic approach to the assessment of children; toward a more humane use of human beings. Boulder, Colo.: Westview Press [WTP]Google Scholar
Pribram, K. (1971) Languages of the brain. Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice- Hall. [WBW]Google Scholar
Pribram, K. (1980) Reflections on the place of brain in the ecology of mind. In: Weimer, W. B., and Palermo, D. S. (eds.) Cognition and the symbolic processes, Vol. II. Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Eribaum Associates. [WBW]Google Scholar
Price, H. H. (1946) Thinking and representation. Proceedings of the British Academy 32:1–40. [JHe]Google Scholar
Putnam, H. (1975) The meaning of meaning. In: Putnam, H. (ed.) Mind, language, and reality. Cambridge: and New York: Cambridge Univ. Press. [GR]CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Putnam, H. (1978) Meaning and the moral sciences, London: Routledge and Kegan Paul. [ZP]Google Scholar
Pylyshyn, Z. (1973) What the mind's eye tells the mind's brain: a critique of mental imagery. Psychological Bulletin 80:1–24. [ZP]Google Scholar
Putnam, H. (1976) Le role des theories de la competence en psychobogie cognitive. In: Sarazin, R. (ed.) Psycholinguistique theoretique et expéimentale. Quebec: Université de Quebec Press. [ZP]Google Scholar
Putnam, H. (1978a) Computational models and empirical constraints. Behavioral and Brain Sciences 1:93–99. [ZP]Google Scholar
Putnam, H. (1978b) When is attribution of beliefs justified? Behavioral and Brain Sciences 1:592–593. [ZP]Google Scholar
Putnam, H. (1978c) Imagery and artificial intelligence. In: Savage, W. (ed.) Perception and cognition: issues in the foundations of psychology. Minneapolis:Univ. of Minnesota Press. [ZP]Google Scholar
Putnam, H. (1979a) The rate of “mental rotation” of images: a test of a holistic analogue hypothesis. Memory and Cognition 7:19–28. [RCM, ZP]Google Scholar
Putnam, H. (1979b) Validating computational models: a critique of Anderson's indeterminacy of representation claim. Psychological Review 86:383–394. [ZP]Google Scholar
Putnam, H. (1979c) The imagery debate: analogue media or tacit knowledge? Psychological Review (1980) [ZP]Google Scholar
Putnam, H. (1979d) Brain and consciousness. Invited talk, Psychology and Philosophy Conference, New York (mimeo). [ZP]Google Scholar
Putnam, H. (forthcoming) The imagery debate: Analogue media or tacit knowledge? (submitted). [ZP]Google Scholar
Pylyshyn, Z.; Elcock, E. W.; Marmor, M.; and Sander, P. (1978) Explorations in perceptual-motor spaces. In: Proceedings of the Second International Conference of the Canadian Society for Computational Studies of Intelligence, Toronto: Univ. of Toronto. Department of Computer Science. [ZP]Google Scholar
Quine, W. V. Q. (1976) “Truth By Convention,” first published in 1936; reprinted in his The Ways of Paradox and Other Essays, Cambridge, Mass.: Harvard Univ. Press. [GR]Google Scholar
Quine, W. V. Q. (1977) Natural kinds. In: Schwartz, S. P. (ed.) Naming, necessity, and natural kinds. Ithaca: Cornell Univ. Press. [ZP]Google Scholar
Rosch, E. (1973) On the internal structure of perceptual and semantic categories. In: Moore, T. E. (ed.) Cognitive development and the acquisition of language. New York: Academic Press. [ZP]Google Scholar
Ruiz-Marcos, A., and Valverde, F. (1969) The temporal evolution of the distribution of dendritic spines in the visual cortex of normal and dark raised mice. Experimental Brain Research 8:284–294. [PMC]Google Scholar
Schneider, W., and Shiffrin, R. M. (1976) Automatic and controlled information processing in vision. In: LaBarge, D., and Samuels, S. J. (eds.) Basic processes in reading: perception and comprehension. Hillsdale, N.J.: L. Eribaum. [SG]Google Scholar
Schneider, W., and Shiffrin, R. M. (1977) Controlled and automatic human information processing: I Detection, search and attention. Psychological Review 84:1–66. [ZP]Google Scholar
Scott, D. S., and Strachey, C. (1971) Towards a mathematical semantics for computer languages. Proc. Symposium on Computers and Automata, Fox, J. (ed). New York: Polytechnic Instit. of Brooklyn Press. [ZP]Google Scholar
Segall, M.; Campbell, D. T.; and Herskovits, M. J. (1963) Cultural differences in the perception of geometrical illusions. Science 139:769–771. [ZP]Google Scholar
Shepard, R. N. (1964) Attention and the metrical structure of the similarity space. Journal of Mathematical Psychology (1964) 1:54–87. [ZP]Google Scholar
Shepard, R. N. and Feng, C. (1972) A chronometric study of mental paper folding. Cognitive Psychology 3:228–243. [ZP]Google Scholar
Singer, J. L. (1979) Imagery and affect in psychotherapy: elaborating private scripts and generating contexts. In: Sheikh, A. A. and Shaffer, J. T. (eds.) The potential of fantasy and imagination. New York: Brandon. [RLK]Google Scholar
Smythe, W. E. (04 13, 1979) The analoglcal/propositlonal debate about mental representation: a Good manian analysis. Paper presented at the 5th annual meeting of the Society for Philosophy and Psychology. NYU Medical Center. [WES]Google Scholar
Sperling, G., and Reeves, A. (11 11. 1976) Reaction time of an unobservable response, unpublished address. Psychonomic Society, St. Louis. [SG]Google Scholar
Sternberg, S. (1969) The discovery of processing stages: extensions of Donders' method. Acta Psychologica 30:276–315. [ZP]Google Scholar
Stich, S. (1978) Beliefs and subdoxastic states. Philosophy of Science 45(4):499–518. [ZP]Google Scholar
Turing, A. M. (1936) On computable numbers, with an application to the Entscheidungsproblem. Proceedings of the London Mathematical Society 42:230–265. [ZP]Google Scholar
Ullman, S. (1979) The interpretation of visual motion. Cambridge: MIT Press. [ZP, SWZ]Google Scholar
Wallach, H., and O'Connell, D. N. (1953) The kinetic depth effect. Journal of Experimental Psychology 45:205–217. [ZP]Google Scholar
Weimer, W. B. (1977) A conceptual framework for cognitive psychology: motor theories of the mind. In: Shaw, R., and Bransford, J. D. (eds.) Perceiving, acting, and knowing. Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates. [WBW]Google Scholar
Young, R. K. (1968) Serial learning. In: Dixon, T. R., and Horton, D. L. (eds.) Verbal behavior and general behavior theory. Englewood Cliffs, N.J.: Prentice-Hall. [SG]Google Scholar
Zinman, J. (1979) Reliable knowledge. An exploration of the grounds for belief in science. New York: Cambridge Univ. Press. [KMC]Google Scholar