No CrossRef data available.
Article contents
Preconscious semantic processing: Why and how?
Published online by Cambridge University Press: 04 February 2010
Abstract
An abstract is not available for this content so a preview has been provided. Please use the Get access link above for information on how to access this content.

- Type
- Continuing Commentary
- Information
- Copyright
- Copyright © Cambridge University Press 1987
References
Balota, D. A. (1983) Automatic semantic activation and episodic memory encoding. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Beahvior 22: 88–104. [DH, JS]CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Balota, D. A. (1986) Unconscious semantic processing: The pendulum keeps on swinging. Behavioral and Brain Sciences 9: 23–24. [JAG]CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Balota, D. A. & Chumley, J. I. (1984) Are lexical decisions a good measure of lexical access? The role of word frequency in the neglected decision stage. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance 10: 340–57. [DH]Google ScholarPubMed
Breitmeyer, B. G. (1984) Visual masking: An integrative approach. Oxford University Press. [DH]Google Scholar
Brown, J. W. (1982) Hierarchy and evolution in neurolinguistics. In: Neural models of language processes, ed. Arbib, M., Caplan, D. & Marshall, J. C.. Academic Press. [GK]Google Scholar
Brown, J. W. (1983) Rethinking the right hemisphere. In: Cognitive processing in the right hemisphere, ed. Perecman, E.. Academic Press. [GK]Google Scholar
Brown, W. P. (1961) Conceptions of perceptual defence. British Journal of Psychology. Monograph Supplement No. 35. [JAG]Google Scholar
Cheesman, J. & Merikle, P. M. (1985) Word recognition and consciousness. In: Reading research: Advances in theory and practice, vol. 5, ed. Besner, D., Waller, T. G. & MacKinnon, G. E.. Academic Press. [DH]Google Scholar
Cutler, A., Mehler, J., Norris, D. & Segui, J. (1986) The syllable's differing role in the segmentation of French and English. Journal of Memory and Language 25: 385–400. [DH]CrossRefGoogle Scholar
de Groot, A. M. B. (1983) The range of automatic spreading activation in word priming. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior 22: 417–36. [DH, JS]CrossRefGoogle Scholar
de Groot, A. M. B., Thomassen, A. J. W. M. & Hudson, P. T. W. (1982) Associative facilitation of word recognition as measured from a neutral prime. Memory and Cognition 10: 358–70. [JS]CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Dennis, M. (1980a) Capacity and strategy for syntactic comprehension after left or right hemideeortication. Brain and Language 10: 287–317. [DH]CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Dennis, M. (1980b) Language acquisition in a single hemisphere: Semantic organization. In: Biological studies of mental processes, ed. Caplan, D.. MIT Press. [DH]Google Scholar
Dennis, M., Lovett, M. & Wiegel-Crump, C. A. (1981) Written language acquisition after left or right hemideeortication in infancy. Brain and Language 12: 54–91. [DH]CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Dixon, N. F. (1986) On private events and brain events. Behavioral and Brain Sciences 9: 29–30. [DH, JAG]CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Erdelyi, M. H. (1986) Experimental indeterminacies in the dissociation paradigm of subliminal perception. Behavioral and Brain Sciences 9: 30–31. [DH]CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Eriksen, C. W. (1960) Discrimination and learning without awareness: A methodological survey and evaluation. Psychological Review 67: 279–300. [JAG]CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Evett, L. J., Humphreys, G. W. & Quinlan, P. T. (1986) Identification, masking, and priming: Clarifying the issues. Behavioral and Brain Sciences 9: 31–32. [JAG]CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Fodor, J. A. (1985) Precis of The modularity of mind. Behavioral and Brain Sciences 8: 1–42. [DH]CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Forster, K. I. (1979) Levels of processing and the structure of the language processor. In: Sentence processing: Psycholinguistic studies presented to Merrill Garrett, ed. Cooper, W. E. & Walker, E. C. T.. Erlbaum. [JS]Google Scholar
Fowler, C. A., Wolford, G., Slade, R. & Tassinary, L. (1981) Lexical access with and without awareness. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General 110: 341–62. [DH, JS]CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gazzaniga, M. S. (1983) Right hemisphere language following brain bisection. American Psychologist 38: 525–37. [DH]CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Green, M. & Odom, J. V. (1984) Comparison of monoptic and dichoptic masking by light. Perception and Psychophysics 35: 265–68. [DH]CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Groeger, J. A. (1984) Evidence of unconscious semantic processing from a forced error situation. British Journal of Psychology 75: 305–14. [DH, JAG]CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Groeger, J. A. (1986) Predominant and non-predominant analysis: Effects of level of presentation. British Journal of Psychology 77: 109–16. [DH, JAG]CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Henley, S. H. A. (1976) Responses to homophones as a function of cue words on the unattended channel. British Journal of Psychology 67: 559–67. [DH, JAG]CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Henley, S. H. A. & Dixon, N. F. (1974) Laterality differences in the effects of incidental stimuli upon evoked imagery. British Journal of Psychology 65: 529–36. [DH, JAG]CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Holender, D. (1986) Semantic activation without conscious identification in dichotic listening, parafoveal vision, and visual masking: A survey and appraisal. Behavioral and Brain Sciences 9: 1–66.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Humphreys, G. W., Evett, L. J. & Taylor, D. E. (1982) Automatic phonological priming in visual word recognition. Memory and Cognition 10: 576–90. [JAG]CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kinsbourne, M. (1985) Parallel processing explains modular informational encapsulation. Behavioral and Brain Sciences 8: 23. [DH]CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Levelt, W. J. M. (1966) The alternation process in binocular rivalry. British Journal of Psychology 57: 225–38. [DH]CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Macmillan, N. A. (1986) The psychophysics of subliminal perception. Behavioral and Brain Sciences 9: 38–39. [DH]CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Marcel, A. J. (1983) Conscious and unconscious perception: Experiments on visual masking and word recognition. Cognitive Psychology 15: 197–237. [JAG, DH, JS]CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Marcel, A. J. (1983) Conscious and unconscious perception: An approach to the relation between phenomenal experience and perceptual processes. Cognitive Psychology, 15, 238–300. [JAG]CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Marcel, A. J. (1986) Consciousness and processing: Choosing and testing a null hypothesis. Behavioral and Brain Sciences 9: 40–41. [DH]CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Merikle, P. M. (1982) Unconscious perception revised. Perception and Psychophysics 31: 298–301. [JAG]CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Merikle, P. M. & Cheesman, J. (1986) Consciousness is a “subjective” state. Behavioral and Brain Sciences 9: 42. [JAG]CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Moray, N. (1959) Attention in dichotic listening: Affective cues and the influence of instructions. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psycholog 11: 56–60. [JMW]CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Moseovitch, M. (1983) The linguistic and emotional functions of the normal right hemisphere. In: Cognitive processing in the right hemisphere, ed. Perecman, E.. Academic Press. [GK]Google Scholar
Mykel, N. & Daves, W. F. (1979) Emergence of unreported stimuli into imagery as a function of laterality of presentation: A replication and extension of research by Henley and Dixon. British Journal of Psychology 70: 253–58. [JAG, DH]CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Philpott, A. & Wilding, J. (1979) Semantic interference from subliminal stimuli in a dichoptic viewing situation. British Journal of Psychology 70: 559–63. [DH, JMW]CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Posner, M. I. & Snyder, C. R. (1975a) Attention and cognitive control. In: Information processing and cognition: The Loyola Symposium, ed. Solso, R. L.. Erlbaum. [JS]Google Scholar
Posner, M. I. & Snyder, C. R. (1975b) Facilitation and inhibition in the processing of signals. In: Attention and Performance V, ed. Rabbitt, P. M. A. & Dornic, S.. Academic Press. [JS]Google Scholar
Reicher, G. M. (1969) Perceptual recognition as a function of meaningfulness of stimulus material. Journal of Experimental Psychology 81: 275–80. [DH]CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Smith, G. J. W., Spence, D. P. & Klein, G. S. (1959) Subliminal effects of verbal stimuli. Journal of Abnormal and Social Psychology 59: 167–76. [DH]CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Somekh, D. E. & Wilding, J. M. (1973) Perception without awareness in a dichoptic viewing situation. British Journal of Psychology 64: 339–49. [JAG, DH, JMW]CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Stanovich, K. E. & Purcell, D. G. (1986) Priming without awareness: What was all the fuss about? Behavioral and Brain Sciences 9: 47–48. [DH]CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Treisman, A. & Geffen, G. (1967) Selective attention: Perception or response? Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology 19: 1–17. [JMW]CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Turvey, M. T. (1973) On peripheral and central processes in vision: Interference from an information-processing analysis of masking with patterned stimuli. Psychological Review 80: 1–52. [DH]CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Underwood, G. & Thwaites, S. (1982) Automatic phonological coding of unattended printed words. Memory and Cognition 10: 434–42. [JAG]CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Wilding, J. M. & Farrell, J. M. (1970) Selective attention: Superior detection of word targets compared with sound targets in a prose passage while shadowing another passage. Psychonomic Science 19: 123–24. [DH, JMW]CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Walker, P. (1978) Binocular rivalry: Central or peripheral selective processes? Psychological Bulletin 85: 376–89. [DH]CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Wolfe, J. M. (1986) Stereopsis and binocular rivalry. Psychological Review 93: 269–82. [DH]CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Zimba, L. D. & Blake, R. (1983) Binocular rivalry and semantic processing: Out of sight, out of mind. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance 9: 807–15. [DH, JMW]Google Scholar