Hostname: page-component-586b7cd67f-dsjbd Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-11-24T02:55:19.680Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Observing and conditioned reinforcement

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  04 February 2010

James A. Dinsmoor
Affiliation:
Department of Psychology, Indiana University, Bloomington, Ind. 47405

Abstract

When experimenters require their subjects to perform some readily recorded response to gain access to discriminative stimuli but do not permit this behavior to alter the schedule of reinforcement, the response is classified, by analogy, as an “observing” response. Observing responses have been used not only to analyze discrimination learning but also to substantiate the concept of conditioned reinforcement and to measure the reinforcing effect of stimuli serving other behavioral functions. A controversy, however, centers around the puzzling question of how observing can be sustained when the resulting stimuli are not associated with any increase in the frequency of primary reinforcement. Two possible answers have been advanced: (a) that differential preparatory responses to these stimuli as conditional stimuli make both the receipt and the nonreceipt of unconditional stimuli more reinforcing; and (b) that information concerning biologically significant events is inherently reinforcing. It appears, however, that the stimulus associated with the less desirable outcome is not reinforcing. The maintenance of observing can be reconciled with the traditional theory that the acquisition of reinforcing properties proceeds according to the same rules as those for Pavlovian conditioning if it is recognized that the subject is selective in what it observes and procures a greater than proportionate exposure to the stimulus associated with the more desirable outcome. As a result of this selection, the overall frequency of primary reinforcement increases in the presence of the observed stimuli and declines in the presence of the nondifferential stimuli that prevail when the subject is not observing.

Type
Target Article
Copyright
Copyright © Cambridge University Press 1983

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

Abbott, B. & Badia, P. (1981) Observing as a function of the probability of shock and shock-free components. Paper presented at the meeting of the Midwestern Psychological Association, Detroit. [PB]Google Scholar
Amsel, A. (1958) The role of frustrative nonreward in noncontinuous reward situations. Psychological Bulletin 55:102–19. [LEL]Google Scholar
Amsel, A.(1968) secondary reinforcement and frustration. Psychological Bulletin 69:278. [LEL]Google Scholar
Arabian, J. M. & Desiderato, O. (1975) Preference for signaled shock: A test of two hypotheses. Animal Learning and Behavior 3:191–95. [PB]CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Astley, S. L. & Perkins, C. C. (in preparation) The effects of context and stimulus duration on the conditioned reinforcing value of stimuli. [CCP]Google Scholar
Auge, R. J. (1974) Context, observing behavior, and conditioned reinforcement. journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior 22:525–33. [taJAD]CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Auge, R. J. (1977) Stimulus functions within a fixed-interval clock schedule: Reinforcement, punishment, and discriminative stimulus control. Animal Learning and Behavior 5:117–23. [taJAD]Google Scholar
Autor, S. M. (1969) The strength of conditioned reinforcers as a function of frequency and probability of reinforcement. In: Conditioned reinforcement, ed. Hendry, D. P., pp. 127–62. Dorsey Press. [tarJAD]Google Scholar
Ayres, J. J. B. (1966) Conditioned suppression and the information hypothesis. journal of Comparative and Physiological Psychology 62:2125. [JM]Google Scholar
Badia, P. & Culbertson, S. (1972) The relative aversiveness of signaled vs. unsignaled escapable and inescapable shock. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior 17:462–71. [PB]Google Scholar
Badia, P., Culbertson, S. & Lewis, P. (1971) The relative aversiveness of signaled vs. unsignaled avoidance. journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior 16:113–21. [PB]CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Badia, P., Harsh, J. & Abbott, B. (1979) Choosing between predictable and unpredictable shock conditions: Data and theory. Psychological Bulletin 86:1107–31. [PB, HD, tarJAD]Google Scholar
Badia, P., Harsh, J., Coker, C. C. & Abbott, B. (1976) Choice and the dependability of stimuli that predict shock and safety. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior 26: 95111. [PB]Google Scholar
Badia, P., Ryan, K. & Harsh, J. (1981) Choosing schedules of signaled appetitive events over schedules of unsignaled ones. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior 35:187–95. ‘PB, rJAD’CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Baron, A. & Galizio, M. (1976) Clock control of human performance on avoidance and fixed-interval schedules. journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior 26:165–80. ‘taJAD’Google Scholar
Baum, W. M. & Rachlin, H. C. (1969) Choice as time allocation. journal the Experimental Analysis of Behavior 12:861–74. ‘taJAD’Google Scholar
Berlyne, D. E. (1957) Uncertainty and conflict: A point of contact between information-theory and behavior-theory concepts. Psychological Review 64:329–39. ‘taJAD’Google Scholar
Berlyne, D. E. (1960) Conflict, arousal, and curiosity. McGraw-Hill. ‘tarJAD, RRM’Google Scholar
Blanchard, R. (1975) The effect of S– on observing behavior. Learning and Motivation 6:110. ‘taJAD’Google Scholar
Bloonsfield, T. M. (1972) Reinforcement schedules: Contingency or contiguity? In: Reinforcement: Behavioral analysis, ed. Gilbert, R. M. & Millenson, J. B., pp. 165208. Academic Press. ‘taJAD, RKRT’CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bolles, R. C. (1967) Theory of motivation. Harper & Bow. ‘LEL’Google Scholar
Bolles, R. C. (1979) Learning theory. Holt, Rinehart & Winston. ‘taJAD’Google Scholar
Bowe, C. A. & Dinsmoor, J. A. (1981) Temporal vs. spatial information as reinforcer of observing. Bulletin of the Psychonomic Society 17:3336. ‘rJAD’CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bowe, C. A. & Dinsmoor, J. A. (1983) Spatial and temporal relations in conditioned reinforcement and observing behavior. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior 39:227–40. ‘rJAD’CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bower, C., McLean, J. & Meacham, J. (1966) value of knowing when reinforcement is due. journal of Comparative and Physiological Psychology 62:184–92. ‘rJAD, JAN’Google Scholar
Branch, M. N. (1970) The distribution of observing responses during two VI schedules. Psychonomic Science 20:56. ‘rJAD’CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Branch, M. N. (1973) Observing responses in pigeons: Effects of schedule component duration and schedule value. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior 20:417–28. ‘MNB, tarJAD, EF’CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Browne, M. P. & Dinsmoor, J. A. (1972) Selective observing of discriminative stimuli. Proceedings of the 80th Annual Convention of the American Psychological Association 7:745–46. (Summary) ‘taJAD’Google Scholar
Browne, M. P. & Dinsmoor, J. A. (1974) Wyckoffs observing response: Pigeons learn to observe stimuli for free food but not stimuli for extinction. Learning and Motivation 5:165–73. ‘PB, tarJAD’CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Browne, M. P., Wasserman, E. A. & Dinsmoor, J. A. (1971). Selective observing during auditory discrimination learning. Paper presented at meeting of the Psychonomic Society, St. Louis. ‘taJAD’Google Scholar
Bugelski, B. R. (1956). The psychology of learning. Holt. ‘LEL’Google Scholar
Case, D. A. & Fantino, E. (1981) The delay-reduction hypothesis of conditioned reinforcement and punishment: Observing behavior. journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior 35:93108. ‘rJAD, EF’Google Scholar
Collier, A. C. (1977) Preference for shock signals as a function of the temporal accuracy of the signals. Learning and Motivation 8:159–70. ‘PB’Google Scholar
Cronin, P. B. (1980) Reinstatement of postresponse stimuli prior to reward in delayed-reward discrimination learning by pigeons. Animal Learning and Behavior 8:352–58. ‘CCP’CrossRefGoogle Scholar
D'Amato, M. R., Etkin, M. & Fazzaro, J. (1968) Cue-producing behavior the capuchin monkey during reversal, extinction, acquisition, and overtraining. journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior 11:425–33, ‘MRD'A, rJAD’Google Scholar
D'Amato, M. R. & Safarjan, W. R. (1979) Preference for information about shock duration in rats. Animal Learning and Behavior 7:8994. ‘MRD'A, rJAD’Google Scholar
Daniels, P. V. (1972) The reinforcing value of information. Doctoral dissertation, Columbia University. ‘rJAD, JAN’Google Scholar
Davis, H. & Levine, S. (1982) Predictability, control and the pituitary-adrenal response in rats. journal of Comparative and Physiological Psychology 96:393404. ‘HD’Google Scholar
Davis, H. & Memmott, J. (1982) Counting behavior in animals: A critical evaluation. Psychological Bulletin 92:547–71. ‘HD’CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Davis, H. & Memmott, J. (1983) Autocontingencies: Rats count to three to predict safety from shock. Animal Learning and Behavior 11:95100. ‘HD’CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Davison, M. (1969) Preference for mixed-interval versus fixed-interval schedules. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior 12:247–52. ‘EF’CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Dinsmoor, J. A. (1968) Escape from shock as a conditioning technique. In: Miami synsposium on the prediction of behavior, 1967: Aversive stimulation, ed. Jones, M. H., pp. 3375. University of Miami Press. ‘rJAD’Google Scholar
Dinsmoor, J. A. (1970) Operant conditioning: An experimental analysis of behavior. William C. Brown Co. ‘rJAD’Google Scholar
Dinsmoor, J. A. (1973) A theory of discrimination learning. Proceedings of the 81st Annual Convention of the American Psychological Association 8:861–62. (Summary) [tarJAD]Google Scholar
Dinsmoor, J. A. (1977) Escape, avoidance, punishment: Where do we stand? Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior 28:8395. ‘rJAD’Google Scholar
Dinsmoor, J. A. (1981) Some speculations concerning the role of observing and attention in establishing stimulus control. Presidential address, Midwestern Psychological Association, Detroit. ‘tarJAD’Google Scholar
Dinsmoor, J. A., Browne, M. P. & Lawrence, C. E. (1972) A test of the negative discriminative stimulus as a reinforcer of observing. journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior 18:7985. ‘tarJAD’CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Dinsmoor, J. A., Browne, M. P., Lawrence, C. E. & Wasserman, E. A. (1971) A new analysis of Wyckoffs observing response. Proceedings of the 79th Annual Convention of the American Psychological Association 6:679–80. (Summary) ‘taJAD, RRM’Google Scholar
Dinsmoor, J. A., Flint, C. A., Smith, R. F. & viemeister, N. F. (1969) Differential reinforcing effects of stimuli associated with the presence or absence of a schedule of punishment. In: Conditioned reinforcement. ed. Hendry, D. P., pp. 357–84. Dorsey Press. [PB, tarJAD]Google Scholar
Dinsmoor, J. A., Mueller, K. L., Martin, L. T. & Bowe, C. A. (1982) The acquisition of observing. journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior 38:249–63. [taJAD, RRM]Google Scholar
Dinsmoor, J. A., Mulvaney, D. E. & Jwaideh, A. R. (1981) Conditioned reinforcement as a function of duration of stimulus, journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior 36:4149. [tarJAD]Google Scholar
Dinsmoor, J. A-, Sears, C. W. & Dout, D. L. (1976) Observing as a function of stimulus difference. journal of Experimental Psychology: Animal Behavior Processes 2:154162. [PB, tarJAD]Google Scholar
Eckerman, D. A. (1973) Uncertainty reduction and conditioned reinforcement. Psychological Record 23:3947. [PB, taJAD]Google Scholar
Egger, M. D. & Miller, N. E. (1962) Secondary reinforcement in rats as a function of information value and reliability of the stimulus. Journal of Experimental Psychology 64:97104. [JM]Google Scholar
Egger, M.D. (1963) When is a reward reinforcing? An experimental study of the information hypothesis. journal of Comparative and Physiological Psychology 56:132–37. [JM]Google Scholar
Fanselow, M. S. (1979) Naloxone attenuates rat's preference for signaled shock. Physiological Psychology 7:7074. [rJAD]Google Scholar
Fanselow, M. S. (1980) Signaled shock-free periods and preference for signaled shock. journal of Experimental Psychology: Animal Behavior Processes 6:6580. [rJAD, RRM]Google Scholar
Fanselow, M. S. (1981) Naloxone and Pavlovian fear conditioning. Learning and Motivation 12:398419. [HD]Google Scholar
Fantino, E. (1967) Preference for mixed versus fixed-ratio schedules. journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior 10:3543. [EF]CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Fanselow, M. S. (1969) Choice and rate of reinforcement.journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior 12:723–30. [taJAD, JM, EFS]Google Scholar
Fanselow, M. S. (1977) Conditioned reinforcement: Choice and information. In: Handbook of operant behavior, ed, Honig, W. K. & StaddonK, J. E. B.. Prentice- Hall. [MNB, taJAD, EF, JM, JAN, CCP, RKRT]Google Scholar
Fanselow, M. S. (1981) Contiguity, response strength, and the delay-reduction hypothesis. In: Advances in analysis of behaviour, vol. 2, Predictability, correlation, and contiguity, ed. Harzem, P. & Zeiler, M. D., pp. 169201. Wiley. [EF]Google Scholar
Fantino, E. & Case, D. A. (in press) Human observing: Maintained by stimuli correlated with reinforcement but not extinction. journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior 40:193210. [tarJAD]Google Scholar
Fantino, E., Case, D. A. & Altus, D. (in press) Observing reward-informative and uninformative stimuli by normal children of different ages. journal of Experimental Child Psychology. [tarJAD]Google Scholar
Fantino, E., Dunn, B. & Meck, W. (1979) Percentage reinforcement and choice. journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior 32:335–40. [EF]Google Scholar
Fantino, E. & Logan, C. A. (1979) The experimental analysis of behavior: A biological perspective. Freeman, W. H.. [taJAD]Google Scholar
Gamzu, E. & Williams, D. R. (1973) Associative factors underlying the pigeon's key pecking in autoshaping procedures. journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior 19:225–32. [JAN]Google Scholar
Garfield, S. L. & Kurtz, R. (1976) Clinical psychologists in the 1970s. American Psychologist 31:19. [taJAD]CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gibbon, J. (1981) The contingency problem in autoshaping. In: Autoshaping and conditioning theory, ed. Locurto, C. M., Terrace, H. S. & Gibbon, J., pp. 285308. Academic Press. [rJAD]Google Scholar
Gibbon, J. & Balsam, P. (1981) Spreading association in time, In: Autoshaping and conditioning theory, ed. Locurto, C. M., Terrace, H. S. & Gibbon, J., pp. 219–53. Academic Press, [taJAD, JM, EFS]Google Scholar
Gibbon, J., Berryman, R. & Thompson, R. L. (1974) Contingency spaces and measures in classical and instrumental conditioning. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior 21:585605. [rJAD, JAN]Google Scholar
Gibson, J. J. (1979) The ecological approach to visual perception. Houghton Mifflin. [RKRT]Google Scholar
Godall, K. (1972) Shapers at work. Psychology Today 6 (6):5362, 132138. [taJAD]Google Scholar
Gormezano, J. & Coleman, S. R. (1973) The law of effect and CR contingent modification of the UCS. Conditional Reflex 8:4156. [CCP]Google Scholar
Hancock, R. A. (1982) Tests of conditioned reinforcement value of sequential stimuli in pigeons. Animal Learning and Behavior 10:4654. [CCP]CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Harsh, J. & Badia, P. (1974) A concurrent assessment of the positive and negative properties of a signaled shock schedule. Animal Learning and Behavior 2:169–72. [PB]Google Scholar
Harsh, J. & Badia, P. (1976) A temporal parameter influencing choice between signaled and unsigualed shock schedules. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior 25:327–33. [PB]Google Scholar
Hearst, E. & Franklin, S. R. (1977) Positive and negative relations between a signal and food: Approach-withdrawal behavior to the signal. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Animal Behavior Processes 3:3752. [taJAD]Google Scholar
Hendry, D. P., ed. (1969 a) Conditioned reinforcement. Dorsey Press. [DPH]Google Scholar
Hendry, D. P. (1969 b) Introduction. In: Conditioned reinforcement, ed Hendry, D. P., pp. 133. Dorsey Press. [taJAD]Google Scholar
Heisdry, D. P. & Dillow, P. V. (1966) Observing behavior during interval schedules. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior 9:337–49. [MNB, taJAD]Google Scholar
Herrnstein, R. J. (1964 a) Aperiodicity as a factor in choice. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior 7:179–82. [EF]Google Scholar
Herrnstein, R. J. (1964 b) Secondary reinforcement and rate of primary reinforcement. journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior 7:2736. [tarJAD]Google Scholar
Hershiser, D. & Trapold, M. A. (1971) Preference for unsignaled over signaled direct reinforcement in the rat. journal of Comparative and Physiological Psychology 77:323–28. [CCP]Google Scholar
Hirota, T. T. (1972) The Wyckoff observing response – a reappraisal. Journal of the Experisnental Analysis of Behavior 18:263–76. [taJAD]CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hovland, C. I. (1952) Clark Leonard Hull: 1884–1952. Psychological Review 59:347–50. [taJAD]Google Scholar
Hull, C. L. (1943) Principles of behavior: An introduction to behavior theory. Appleton-Century. [tarJADJ]Google Scholar
Hulse, S. H., Fowler, H. & Honig, W. K., eds. (1978) Cognitive processes in animal behavior. Lawrence Erlbaum Associates. [HD, taJAD, HST]Google Scholar
Hunter, W. S. (1913) The delayed reaction in animals. Behavior Monographs 2:6. [HST]Google Scholar
Hymowitz, N. (1979) Suppression of responding during signaled and unsignaled shock. Psychological Bulletin 86:175–90. [PB]Google Scholar
Imada, H., Mino, T., Sugioka, K. & Ohki, Y. (1981) Measurement of current flow through the rat under signaled and unsignaled grid-shock conditions. Animal Learning and Behavior 9:7579. [HD]Google Scholar
Imada, H. & Nageishi, Y. (1982) The concept of uncertainty in animal experiments using aversive stimulation. Psychological Bulletin 91:573–88. [MRD'A, rJAD]Google Scholar
Jenkins, H. M., Barnes, B. A. & Barrera, F. J. (1981) Why autoshaping depends on trial spacing. In: Autosha ping and conditioning theory, ed. Locurto, C. M., Terrace, H. S. & Gibbon, J., pp. 255–84. Academic Press. [taJAD, JM]Google Scholar
Jenkins, H. M. & Boakes, R. A. (1973) Observing stimulus sources that signal food or no food. journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior 20:197207. [taJAD, JAN]Google Scholar
Jenkins, H. M. & Shattuck, D. (1981) Contingency in fear conditioning: A reexamination. Bulletin of the Psychonomic Society 17:159–62. [taJAD, EFS]Google Scholar
Jwaideh, A. R. (1968) Stimulus functions in chained fixed-ratio schedules of reinforcement. Doctoral dissertation, Indiana University. [taJAD]Google Scholar
Jwaideh, A. R. & Mulvaney, D. E. (1976) Punishment of observing by a stimulus associated with the lower of two reinforcement densities. Learning and Motivation 7:211–22. [tarJAD]Google Scholar
Katz, H. N. (1976) A test of the reinforcing properties of stimuli correlated with nonreinforcement. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior 26:4556. [taJAD]Google Scholar
Kelleher, R. T., Riddle, W. C. & Cook, L. (1962) Observing responses in pigeons. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior 5:313. [MNB, rJAD]Google Scholar
Keller, F. S. & Schoenfeld, W. N. (1951) Principles of psychology. AppletonCentury-Crofts. [taJAD]Google Scholar
Kendall, S. B. (1972) Some effects of response-dependent clock stimuli in a fixed-interval schedule. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior 17:161–68. [MNB, taJAD]Google Scholar
Kendall, S. B. (1973 a) Effects of two procedures for varying information transmission on observing responses. journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior 20:7383. [tarJAD]Google Scholar
Kendall, S. B. (1973 b) Redundant information in an observing-response procedure. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior 19:8192. [PB, rJAD]CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kendall, S. B. (1974). Maintenance of observing response with the less highly valued stimulus in pigeons. Animal Learning and Behavior 2:210–14. [rJAD, JM]Google Scholar
Kendall, S. B. & Gibson, D. A. (1965) Effects of discriminative stimulus removal on observing behavior. Psychological Record 15:545–51. [taJAD]Google Scholar
Killeen, P. (1968) On the measurement of reinforcement frequency in the study of preference. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior 11:263–69. [EF]CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Killeen, P., Wald, B. & Cheney, C. D. (1980) Observing behavior and information. Psychological Record 30:181–90. [tarJAD]Google Scholar
Leahey, T. H. (1981) The revolution never happened: Information processing is behaviorism. Paper presented at the 52d annual meeting of the Eastern Psychological Association, New York. [HST]Google Scholar
Lewis, P. & Gardner, E. T. (1977) The reliability of preference for signaled shock. Bulletin of the Psychonomic Society 9:135–38. [PB]Google Scholar
Lieberman, D. A. (1972) Secondary reinforcement and information as determinants of observing behavior in monkeys (Macaca mulatta). Learning and Motivation 3:341–58. [taJAD, DPH, BKBT]CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lieberman, S. M. (1967) A study of secondary reinforcement. MA thesis, University of Southern California. [LEL]Google Scholar
Lipsey, M. W. (1974) Research and relevance: A survey of graduate students and faculty in psychology. American Psychologist 29:541–53. [taJAD]Google Scholar
Longstreth, L. E. (1964) An operational distinction between secondary reinforcement and frustration. American Psychologist 19:452 (abstract). [LEL]Google Scholar
Longstreth, L. E. (1966) Frustration and secondary reinforcement concepts as applied to human conditioning and extinction. Psychological Monographs 80:11, whole no. 619. [LEL]Google Scholar
Longstreth, L. E. (1972) A cognitive interpretation of secondary reinforcement. In: Nebraska symposium on motivation, 1971, ed. Cole, J. K.. University of Nebraska Press. [LEL]Google Scholar
Lutz, R. E. & Perkins, C. C. Jr, (1960) A time variable in the acquisition of observing responses. Journal of Comparative and Physiological Psychology 53:180–82. [PB, tarJAD, EF]Google Scholar
Mackintosh, N. J. (1974) The psychology of animal learning. Academic Press. [taJAD, HST]Google Scholar
McMichael, J. S., Lanzetta, J. T. & Driscoll, J. (1967) Infrequent reward facilitates observing responses in rats. Psychonomic Science 8:2324. [taJAD]CrossRefGoogle Scholar
McMillan, J. C. (1974) Average uncertainty as a determinant of observing behavior. journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior 22:401–8. [taJAD]Google Scholar
Maki, W. S. (1981) Directed forgetting in animals. In: Information processing in animals: Memory mechanisms, ed. Spear, N. E. & Miller, B. B., pp. 199225. Lawrence Erlbaum Associates. [RKRT]Google Scholar
Maki, W. S., Gillund, C., Hauge, G. & Siders, W. (1977) Matching to sample after extinction of observing responses. journal of Experimental Psychology: Animal Behavior Processes 3:285–96. [RKRT]Google Scholar
Marlin, N. A., Berk, A. M. & Miller, R. R. (1978) Modification and avoidance of unmodiflable and unavoidable footshock. Bulletin of the Psychonomic Society 11:203–5. [RRM]Google Scholar
Marlin, N. A.Sullivan, J. M., Berk, A. M. & Miller, R. R. (1979) Preference for information about intensity of signaled tailshock. Learning and Motivation 10:8597. [MRD'A]CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Martin, G. & Pear, J. (1978) Behavior modification: What it is and how to do it. Prentice-Hall. [taJAD]Google Scholar
Mayer, D. J. & Price, D. D. (1976) Central nervous system mechanisms of analgesia. Pain 2:379404. [rJAD]Google Scholar
Miller, G. A. (1981) Review of Perspectives in cognitive science, ed. Norman, D. A.. Science 214:57. [HST]CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Miller, L. K. (1968) Escape from an effortful situation. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior 11:619–27. [rJAD, MP]CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Miller, R. R., Greco, C. & Vigorito, M. (1981) Classically conditioned tail flexion in rats: CR-contingent modification of US intensity as a test of the preparatory response hypothesis. Animal Learning and Behavior 9:8088. [CCP]Google Scholar
Miller, R. B., Creco, C., Vigorito, M. & Marlin, N. A. (1983) Signaled tailshock is perceived as similar to a stronger unsignaled tailshock: Implications for a functional analysis of classical conditioning. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Animal Behavior Processes 9:105–31. [RRM]Google Scholar
Mowrer, O. H. (1960) Learning theory and the symbolic processes. Wiley. [RRM, CCP]Google Scholar
Mueller, K. L. & Dinsmoor, J. A. (1984) Testing the reinforcing properties of S–: A replication of Lieberman's procedure. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior. [taJAD]Google Scholar
Mulvaney, D. E., Dinsmoor, J. A., Jwaideh, A. B. & Hughes, L. H. (1974) Punishment of observing by the negative discriminative stimulus. journal of the Experimental Analysis of Beharior 21:3744. [tarJAD, RRM, MP]Google Scholar
Mulvaney, D. E., Hughes, L. H., Jwaideh, A. H. & Dinsmoor, J. A. (1981) Differential production of positive and negative discriminative stimuli by normal and retarded children. journal of Experimental Child Psychology 32:389400. [taJAD, RKRT]Google Scholar
Murray, M. & Nevin, J. A. (1967) Some effects of correlation between response-contingent shock and reinforceimient. journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior 10:301–9. [JAN]CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Neisser, U. (1976) Cognition and reality. Freeman, W. H.. [HST]Google Scholar
Nevin, J. A. (1969) The maintenance of conditioned reinforcement. Transactions of the New York Academy of Sciences 31:686–96. [JAN]Google Scholar
Nevin, J. A. (1973) Conditioned reinforcement. In: The study of behacior: Learning, motication, emotion, and instinct, ed. Nevin, J. A., pp. 155–98. Scott, Foresman. [taJAD]Google Scholar
Nevin, J. A. & Mandell, C. (1978) Conditioned reinforcement and choice. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior 29:135–48. [JAN]Google Scholar
Pavlov, I. P. (1960) Conditioned reflexes. Translated by Anrep, C. V.. Dover. (Originally published 1927.) [taJAD, CCP]Google Scholar
Perkins, C. C. Jr, (1955) The stimulus conditions which follow learned responses. Psychological Renew 62:341–48. [HD, taJAD, RRM, CCP]Google ScholarPubMed
Perkins, C. C. (1968) An analysis of the concept of reinforcement. Psychological Review 75:155–72. [HD, taJAD, RRM, CCP]CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Perkins, C. C. (1971) Reinforcement in classical conditioning. In: Essays in neobehaciorism: A memorial colume to Kenneth W. Spence, ed. Kendler, H. H. & Spence, J. T., pp. 113–36. Appleton-Century-Crofts. [HD, taJAD, CCP]Google Scholar
(in press) The analysis of performance. In: Advances in analysis of behavior, vol. 3, Biological factors in learning, ed. Zeiler, M. D. & Harzem, P.. Wiley. [CCP]Google Scholar
Perkins, C. C. Jr, Seymann, R. C., Levis, D. J. & Spencer, H. H. Jr. (1966) Factors affecting preference for signal-shock over shock-signal. journal of Experimental Psychology 72:190–96. [taJAD]CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Perone, M. & Baron, A. (1980) Reinforcement of human observing behavior by a stimulus correlated with extinction or increased effort. journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior 34:239–61. [tarJAD, JM, MP]Google Scholar
Powell, D. R. & Perkins, C. C. (1957) Strength of secondary reinforcement as a determiner of the effects of duration of the goal response on learning. Journal of Experimental Psychology 53:106–12. [CCP]Google Scholar
Premack, D. & Collier, G. (1966) Duration of looking and number of brief looks as dependent variables. Psychonoinic Science 4:8182. [rJAD]Google Scholar
Prokasy, W. F. Jr. (1956) The acquisition of observing responses in the absence of differential external reinforcement. Journal of Coin parative and Physiological Psychology 49:131–34. [tarJAD, DPH, LEL, JAN]Google Scholar
Raehlin, H. (1976) Behavior and learning. Freeman, W. H.. [taJAD]Google Scholar
Rand, J. F. (1977) Behaviors observed during S– in a simple discrimination learning task. Journal of the Erperi mental Analysis of Behavior 27:103–17. [taJAD]Google Scholar
Rescorla, R. A. (1966) Predictability and number of pairings in Pavlovian fear conditioning. Psychonomic Science 4:383–84. [JAN]Google Scholar
Rescorla, R. A. (1968) Probability of shock in the presence and absence of CS in fear conditioning. Journal of Comparative and Physiological Psychology 66:15. [LJM]Google Scholar
Rilling, M., Kramer, T. J. & Richards, R. W. (1973) Aversive properties of the negative stimulus during learning with and without errors. Learning and Motivation 4:110. [MP]Google Scholar
Roitblat, H. L. (1980) Codes and coding processes in pigeon short-term memory. Animal Learning and Behavior 8:341–51. [HST]Google Scholar
Roitblat, H. L. (1982) The meaning of representation in animal memory. Behavioral and Brain Sciences 5:353406. [MNB, HST]Google Scholar
Roitblat, H. L., Bever, T. C. & Terrace, H. S., eds. (1983, in press) Animal cognition. Lawrence Erlbaum Associates. [MRD'A]Google Scholar
Ruja, H. (1956) Productive psychologists. American Psychologist 11:148/-49. [taJAD]Google Scholar
Safarjan, W. R. & D'Amato, M. R. (1978) variables affecting preference for signaled shock in a symmetrical changeover design. Learning and Motication 9:314–31. [PB]Google Scholar
Saltzman, I. J. (1949) Maze learning in the absence of primary reinforcement: A study of secondary reinforcement. Journal of Comparative and Physiological Psychology 42:161–73. [LEL]CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Sands, S. F. & Wright, A. A. (1980) Serial probe recognition performance by a rhesus money amid a human with 10- and 20-item lists. journal of Experimental Psychology: Animal Behavior Processes 6:386–96. [HST]Google Scholar
Schaub, R. E. (1969) Response-cue contingency and cue effectiveness. In: Conditioned reinforcement, ed. Hendry, D. P., pp. 342–56. Dorsey Press. [taJAD]Google Scholar
Schaub, B. E. & Honig, W. K. (1967) Reinforcement of behavior with cues correlated with extinction. Psychononuic Science 7:1516. [taJAD]Google Scholar
Schrier, A.M., Thompson, C. R. & Spector, N. R. (1980) Observing behavior in monkeys (Macaca arctoides): Support for the information hypothesis. Learning and Motivation 11:355–65. [taJAD]CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Schrier, A. M. & Wing, T. C. (1973) Eye movements of monkeys during brightness discrimination and discrimination reversal. Animal Learning and Behavior 1:145–50. [rJAD]Google Scholar
Seligman, M. E. P. (1966) CS redundancy and secondary punishment. journal of Experimental Psychology 72:546–50. [JM]Google Scholar
Seligman, M. E. P. (1968) Chronic fear produced by unpredictable shock. Journal of Comparative and Physiological Psychology 66:402–11. [RRM]Google Scholar
Shannon, C. E. & Weaver, V. (1949) The mathematical theory of communication. University of Illinois Press. [IaJAD]Google Scholar
Sheffield, F. D. (1965) Relation between classical conditioning and instrumental learning. In: Classical conditioning: A symposium, ed. Prokasy, W. F.. Appleton-Cemstury-Crofts. [CCP]Google Scholar
Skinner, B. F. (1938) The behavior of organisms. Appleton- Century. [taJAD]Google Scholar
Skinner, B. F. (1950) Are theories of learning necessary? Psychological Review 57:193216. [tarjAD]Google Scholar
Skinner, B. F. (1953) Science and human behavior. Macmillan. [taJAD, HST]Google Scholar
Skinner, B. F. (1957) Verbal behavior. Prentice-Hall. [MP]Google Scholar
Skinner, B. F. (1977) Why I am not a cognitive psychologist. Behaviorism 5:110. [HST]Google Scholar
Spence, K. W. (1936) The nature of discrimination learning in animals. Psychological Review 43:427–49. [taJAD]Google Scholar
Spence, K. W. (1940) Continuous versus non-continuous interpretations of discrimination learning. Psychological Review 47:271–88. [tarJAD]Google Scholar
Spence, K. W. (1947) The role of secondary reinforcement in delayed reward learning. Psychological Review 54:114. [CCP]Google Scholar
Spence, K. W. (1952) Clark Leonard Hull: 1884–1952. American Journal of Psychology 65:639–46. [tajAD]Google Scholar
Squires, N., Norborg, J. & Fantino, E. (1975) Second-order schedules: Discrimination of components. journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior 24:157–71. [taJAD]Google Scholar
Steiner, J. (1970) Observing responses and uncertainty reduction: II. The effect of varying the probability of reinforcement. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology 22:592–99. [taJAD]Google Scholar
Straub, R. O. & Terrace, H. S. (1981) Ceneralization of serial learning in the pigeon. Animal Learning and Behavior 9:454–68. [HST]Google Scholar
Terrace, H. S. (1983a) Animal cognition. In: Animal cognition, ed. Roitblat, H. L., Bever, T. C. & Terrace, H. S.. Lawrence Erlbaum Associates, in press. [HST]Google Scholar
Terrace, H. S. (1983b) Simultaneous chaining: The problem it poses for traditional chaining theory. In: Quantitative analyses of behavior: Acquisition, ed. Commons, M. L., Herrnstein, R. J. & Wagner, A. R.. Ballinger, in press. [HST]Google Scholar
Thomas, D. R., Berman, D. L., Serednesky, C. E. & Lyons, J. (1968) Information value amid stimulus configuring as factors in conditioned reinforcement. Journal of Experimental Psychology 76:181–89. [JM]Google Scholar
Thompson, D. M. (1964) Escape from SD associated with fixed-ratio reinforcement. Journal of time Experimental Analysis of Behavior 7:18. [MP]Google Scholar
Thompson, R. K. R. & Herman, L. M. (1981) Auditory delayed discriminations by the dolphin: Nonequivalence with delayed-matching performance. Animal Learning and Behavior 9:915. [RKRT]Google Scholar
Tolman, E. C. (1959) Principles of purposive behavior. In: Psychology: A study of a science, ed. Koch, S., pp. 92157. McCraw-Hill. [taJAD]Google Scholar
Turner, M. B. (1971) Realism and the exploitation of behavior. Appleton-Century-Crofts. [EFS]Google Scholar
Ullmann, L. P. & Krasner, L., eds. (1965) Case studies in behavior modification. Holt, Rinehart & Winston. [taJAD]Google Scholar
Wald, B. A. & Dukich, T. D. (1978) Observing behavior: Redundant stimuli and time since information. Animal Learning and Behavior 6:380–84. [taJAD]Google Scholar
Wasserman, E. A. (1981) Comparative psychology returmis: A review of Hulse, Fowler, and Honig's Cognitive processes in animal behavior. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior 35:243–57. [taJAD]Google Scholar
Wasserman, E. A., Franklin, S. B. & Hearst, E. (1974) Pavloviami appetitive contingencies and approach versus withdrawal to conditioned stimuli in pigeons. Journal of Comparative and Physiological Psychology 86:616–27. [taJAD]Google Scholar
Weiss, J. M. (1970) Somatic effects of predictable and unpredictable shock. Psychosomatic Medicine 32:397408. [HD]Google Scholar
Wilton, R. N. & Clements, R.O. (1971) The role of information in the emission of observing responses: A test of two hypotheses. journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior 16:161–66. [taJAD]Google Scholar
Winter, J. & Perkins, C. C. (1982) Ceneralization amid backchaining of conditioned reinforcement in delayed reward learning by pigeons. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior 38:169–79. [CCP]CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Wright, A. A., Santiago, H. C., Sands, S. F. & Urcuioli, P. J. (1983) Monkey and pigeon serial probe recognition performance: Effects of item pool size on proactive interference and item-specific associations. In: Animal cognition, ed. Roitbiat, H. L., Bever, T. C. & Terrace, H. S.. Lawrence Erlbaum Associates, in press. [HST]Google Scholar
Wright, C. D. (1970) A further note on ranking the important psychologists. American Psychologist 25:650–51. [taJAD]CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Wyckoff, L. B. Jr. (1952) The role of observing responses in discrimination learning. Part 1. Psychological Review 59:431–42. [taJAD, HST]Google Scholar
Wyckoff, L. B. (1959) Toward a quantitative theory of secondary reinforcement. Psychological Review 66:6878. [tarJAD, LEL]CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Wyckoff, L. B. (1969) The role of observing responses in discrimination learning. In: Conditioned reinforcement. ed. Hendry, D. P., pp. 237–60. Dorsey Press. [taJAD]Google Scholar