Hostname: page-component-6587cd75c8-9kljr Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2025-04-24T09:16:00.817Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Historical myths are believed because audiences are socially motivated

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  02 January 2025

Shuai Shao*
Affiliation:
Department of Psychology, University of California, San Diego, CA, USA [email protected] https://www.shuaishao.org/
Michael Barlev
Affiliation:
Department of Psychology, Arizona State University, Tempe, AZ, USA [email protected]://sites.google.com/view/michaelbarlev
*
*Corresponding author.

Abstract

Do people believe in historical myths because they are manipulated by coalitional recruiters, or because it is in their interests to do so? The target article gives somewhat conflicting explanations. We propose that the audiences of historical myths are socially rather than epistemically motivated – they believe and propagate historical myths as a way of signaling their coalitional commitments.

Type
Open Peer Commentary
Copyright
Copyright © The Author(s), 2025. Published by Cambridge University Press

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

Article purchase

Temporarily unavailable

References

Barlev, M., & Neuberg, S. L. (2024). Rational reasons for irrational beliefs. American Psychologist. Advance online publication. https://doi.org/10.1037/amp0001321CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cordonier, L., Nettles, T., & Rochat, P. (2018). Strong and strategic conformity understanding by 3- and 5-year-old children. British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 36(3), 438451.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Delton, A. W., Cosmides, L., Guemo, M., Robertson, T. E., & Tooby, J. (2012). The psychosemantics of free riding: Dissecting the architecture of a moral concept. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 102(6), 12521270.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Higham, J. P. (2014). How does honest costly signaling work? Behavioral Ecology, 25(1), 811.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kurzban, R., & Christner, J. (2011). Are supernatural beliefs commitment devices for intergroup conflict? In Forgas, J. P., Kruglanski, A. W. & Williams, K. D. (Eds.), The psychology of social conflict and aggression (pp. 285300). Psychology Press.Google Scholar
Shao, S., Huang, J., Zhao, L., & Heyman, G. D. (2023). The profit motive: Implications for children's reasoning about merit-based resource distribution. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 226, 105563.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Tomasello, M. (2016). Cultural learning redux. Child Development, 87(3), 643653.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Watson-Jones, R. E., & Legare, C. H. (2016). The social functions of group rituals. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 25(1), 4246.CrossRefGoogle Scholar