Crossref Citations
This article has been cited by the following publications. This list is generated based on data provided by
Crossref.
Sheridan, Susan Rich
2005.
A theory of marks and mind: the effect of notational systems on hominid brain evolution and child development with an emphasis on exchanges between mothers and children.
Medical Hypotheses,
Vol. 64,
Issue. 2,
p.
417.
Núñez, Rafael E.
and
Sweetser, Eve
2006.
With the Future Behind Them: Convergent Evidence From Aymara Language and Gesture in the Crosslinguistic Comparison of Spatial Construals of Time.
Cognitive Science,
Vol. 30,
Issue. 3,
p.
401.
Goldin‐Meadow, Susan
2007.
Handbook of Child Psychology.
Low, Jason
2010.
Preschoolers’ Implicit and Explicit False‐Belief Understanding: Relations With Complex Syntactical Mastery.
Child Development,
Vol. 81,
Issue. 2,
p.
597.
Cutica, Ilaria
and
Bucciarelli, Monica
2011.
“The More You Gesture, the Less I Gesture”: Co-Speech Gestures as a Measure of Mental Model Quality.
Journal of Nonverbal Behavior,
Vol. 35,
Issue. 3,
p.
173.
Goldin‐Meadow, Susan
2015.
Handbook of Child Psychology and Developmental Science.
p.
1.
Kang, Seokmin
and
Tversky, Barbara
2016.
From hands to minds: Gestures promote understanding.
Cognitive Research: Principles and Implications,
Vol. 1,
Issue. 1,
Ianì, Francesco
and
Bucciarelli, Monica
2017.
Mechanisms underlying the beneficial effect of a speaker’s gestures on the listener.
Journal of Memory and Language,
Vol. 96,
Issue. ,
p.
110.
Zebrowski, Robin L.
2020.
Fear of a Bot Planet: Anthropomorphism, Humanoid Embodiment, and Machine Consciousness.
Journal of Artificial Intelligence and Consciousness,
Vol. 07,
Issue. 01,
p.
119.