Hostname: page-component-cd9895bd7-mkpzs Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-12-28T03:30:46.114Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

An Anthropologist Fails to Become a Fish: Multispecies Sensing in the Anthropocene

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  26 September 2024

Matthew Buttacavoli*
Affiliation:
The Cairns Institute, College of Arts, Society and Education, James Cook University, Cairns, Queensland, Australia Department of Anthropology, Aarhus University, Aarhus, Denmark
Rights & Permissions [Opens in a new window]

Abstract

How well do we know how non-humans experience environmental stressors and how do we communicate that knowledge as educators? This paper addresses these questions by way of an auto-ethnographic account of the author’s experience of attempting to listen to the Great Barrier Reef, off the Queensland coast. Through a series of methodological failures and roadblocks, this paper discusses the difficulties in understanding non-human sensory worlds. Following the auto-ethnographic account, the paper explores how anthropological pedagogies can contribute to environmental education of non-human experiences more broadly. The paper uses anthropological pedagogy to draw an analogy between ethnocentrism/cultural relativism and anthropocentrism/ecocentrism. Utilising practices of “third place” then demonstrates how the latter terms of these relationships are correctives to the former terms rather than oppositions. This paper concludes by suggesting ways in which the lessons learned can be applied to environmental education. It recommends creating a third space environmental curriculum which defamiliarises human experience and creates a zone of contact between humans and non-humans. The use of mediating technologies and artistic practice in conjunction with scientific education is recommended to maintain a critical perspective of human knowledge and biological limitations in creating experiential relationships with the environment.

Type
Article
Creative Commons
Creative Common License - CCCreative Common License - BY
This is an Open Access article, distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution licence (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/), which permits unrestricted re-use, distribution and reproduction, provided the original article is properly cited.
Copyright
© The Author(s), 2024. Published by Cambridge University Press on behalf of Australian Association for Environmental Education

Introduction

Environmental education in the Anthropocene faces significant pedagogical challenges in teaching about the effects that environmental changes have on non-human worlds. In this article, I utilise an auto-ethnographic approach based on fieldwork on the Great Barrier Reef to explore the boundaries of multispecies relationality. In my failure to listen like a fish, I will critique standard common approaches to environmental teaching and research. Finally, I draw on anthropological pedagogy (among others) to suggest new pathways for educators.

Environmental educators have found it important to shift education from an anthropocentric model to an ecocentric one which decentres humans and acknowledges the intrinsic value of beings in the ecosystem (Bonnett, Reference Bonnett2019; Cocks & Simpson, Reference Cocks and Simpson2015). As a part of this shift, environmental education pedagogy and research have incorporated more (posthuman) phenomenology and radical relation-making (Bonnett, Reference Bonnett2019; Davies, Reference Davies2021; Rautio et al., Reference Rautio, Hohti, Tammi and Ylirisku2022a), which also draws from a longer tradition of aesthetics and environmentalism (Harries-Jones, Reference Harries-Jones2005; Smith & Smith, Reference Smith and Smith1970). Significantly, these posthuman practices deviate from traditional phenomenology by moving away from practices of observation and towards practices of multispecies and multi-material entanglements (Barad, Reference Barad2007).

Such changes are necessary to comprehend the ways in which human action escape human intent, which Tsing, Deger, Keleman Saxena, and Zhou (Reference Tsing, Deger, Keleman Saxena and Zhou2021) call feral entities. Not to be confused with feral animals, “feral entities” refers to the unexpected by-products of human action which “assumes a trajectory beyond human control” (Tsing et al., Reference Tsing, Deger, Keleman Saxena and Zhou2021, “Introduction to Feral Atlas” section). While feral entities may include non-human species such as cane toads or kudzu which are moved around the globe due to human industry, it also includes non-living entities such as microplastics, radioactive fallout and induced mud volcanoes. Feral entities can escape because they are often able to proliferate outside of human experiences. How well do environmental education practices hold up when feral entities escape human perception?

Rautio et al. (Reference Rautio, Tammi, Aivelo, Hohti, Kervinen and Saari2022b) argue for a posthumanist environmental education which “seeks to consider the experiences of all beings, both human and nonhuman” (p.774), and so address those experiences which escape us humans. But as the CitiRats project demonstrates, students struggle with shedding their sense of the world in order to inhabit the world of non-human animals (Kervinen et al., Reference Kervinen, Hohti, Rautio, Saari, Tammi and Aivelo2024; Rautio et al., Reference Rautio, Tammi, Aivelo, Hohti, Kervinen and Saari2022b). As Despret (Reference Despret2013) explains, humans shedding their sense of the world is also a persistent issue in animal studies research. Jukes and Reeves (Reference Jukes and Reeves2020) and Stewart (Reference Stewart2018) both explore more-than-human relationality via extension through assemblages in their place-responsive pedagogies, although both note the friction in thinking through other bodies. This suggests to me a fundamental epistemological issue in how our perceptual biases influence our interpretations of non-human experiences. It is the same issue I ran into during my own research. In attempting to use traditional phenomenological techniques, I quickly ran into my human boundaries. When I shifted to more posthuman thinking about my research practices, I became more enmeshed in human-animal-technical networks. Still, I struggled with perceiving non-human worlds. It is in this struggle that I came to know the limits of phenomenological practice and the challenges of communicating posthuman ontologies.

Writing as method

Before exploring feral entities and perception, I would like to take a moment to elaborate on the methodology used in this article: auto-ethnography. Firstly, I use auto-ethnography because I am trained as an anthropologist, and ethnography is core to our discipline’s practice of communication. Ethnography is our way of presenting and discussing a culture as well as to structure and examine our own thoughts. Ethnography, though, is often writing about other people where the anthropologist acts as a bit player to enliven a scene. I, on the other hand, am seeking to explore a subjective experience, so I am employing auto-ethnography. Through auto-ethnography, I can communicate about events centred on my own participation and use those stories to interrogate the cultural phenomena that structure my experience and way of knowing. Posthuman and multispecies anthropologists are particularly fond of using this methodology (in whole or in part) because it allows them to probe the nature of their intertwined relationships with materials and lifeforms (Helmreich, Reference Helmreich2007; Tsing, Reference Tsing2015).

Secondly, I use auto-ethnography because I believe that it can be a tremendous tool for academic communication in environmental education. As educators, we are often at the centre of experiments of our own design. We are both the subject and observer of our educational interventions, and we cannot be removed from the data we collect. As Barad (Reference Barad2007) argues, we are “not outside [the world] looking in” (p.6); we are making and being remade by our interactions with the world. Auto-ethnography allows the researcher to include themselves in the analysis and acknowledge their own experiences as valid data.

Diving in

I began my fieldwork in 2017 onboard Passions of Paradise III, a dive boat running day trips out of Cairns to several sites on the outer Great Barrier Reef. My intent was to study how recreational and professional divers related to the Reef and its denizens through—among other things—sound, and how this contributed to their understanding of underwater noise pollution. I also started diving for my own phenomenological investigation. I had the sense that if I were to write about the Reef, I should immerse myself in it. What does it sound like and feel like to be in the marine environment?

Phenomenology forms the basis of many methodologies of sound studies. Schafer’s (Reference Schafer1977) soundwalks, which require the practitioner to move through an environment while listening closely to the changes in sound, have been an inspiration for many sound studies. Another commonly employed auditory method is the practice of deep listening, as developed by composer Pauline Oliveros. According to Oliveros (Reference Oliveros1998), “deep listening is listening in every possible way to everything possible to hear no matter what you are doing” (p. 3). The practitioner listens intently and tries to identify the various qualities of the soundscape and the effect the environment has on those qualities. Even Helmreich (Reference Helmreich2007), the most science focused of the sound anthropologists, relies on phenomenological techniques when discussing sound in submersibles, although he does place more emphasis on the role of technological mediation in perception.

Armed with these methods, I attempted a phenomenological approach to listening. Phenomenological methods in anthropology (particularly in the anthropology of sound) focus on using the body as instrument to sense, record and analyse experiences to explore how body and culture shaped those experiences (in the tradition of Schutz [Reference Schutz1962]) (Bernard, Reference Bernard2006). I chose to start off with some soundswims which combines Schafer’s (Reference Schafer1997) soundwalks with Oliveros’ (Reference Oliveros1998) deep listening. I would participate in a dive and take notes on sound experiences and changes in the soundscape in a waterproof notebook.

Helmreich’s (Reference Helmreich2007) work on the anthropology of sound and ocean space points towards the need to recognise the (cyborgic) relationship between humans and technology:

attend to the earache, to imbalance, to all the embodied capacitances of the ethnographer—and to the work necessary to place oneself in particular networks, machinic and social … pay attention to impedance and resistance in cyborgic circuits, to the work that needs to be done so that signals can link machines and people together, at a range of scales, from the private to the public. (emphasis mine, p. 633)

Although enmeshed in cyborgic circuits, the ethnographer is still engaged in phenomenological work. Based in posthumanism, cyborg theory emphasises the embodied relationship between humans and technology (Haraway, Reference Haraway1991) and therefore encourages sensual exploration. Helmreich (Reference Helmreich2007) shifts attention to how phenological experience emerges from material relationalities that reconfigure interrelated subjects. Importantly, posthuman anthropology values phenomenology as being fundamental to our methodology (something environmental educators should take into consideration when using anthropological works) (Howard, Reference Howard2017).

Although scuba may offer the possibility of a new ear, in practice, the equipment does little to amplify the sensorial experience and can even encumber auditory sensing. As Picken and Ferguson (Reference Picken and Ferguson2014) note in their sensual ethnography of novice divers, most listening is oriented towards monitoring internal states rather than external sources. Most new divers concern themselves with monitoring their breathing rates to best conserve air and ensure that the supply system is working correctly. The internal soundscapes of a novice diver are so powerful that they mask environmental soundscapes. New divers are unlikely to be aware of what a healthy reef soundscape should sound like and are probably not going to learn it through diving.

The crew of Passions fare better at perceiving the Reef’s soundscape. Through training and experience, they have learned to listen beyond their internal states. Sound, for them, has become another tool for monitoring their surroundings and for navigation. An experienced diver can begin to recognise the sizzle of rain hitting the surface, the munching of parrot fish and triggerfish or the respirations of another diver just outside of the field of view. The divemaster may even signal these audio cues to the rest of the group, but it is unclear if the other divers actually hear it.

Intriguingly, one sonic experience was independently reported to me by multiple divers. A diver is underwater when they suddenly experience a strong increase in noise and a strong vibration. The diver is unnerved enough by the noise that they surface only to notice that a large ship was passing by several kilometres away! Even though this story was not uncommon, what was striking was that almost none of the divers I worked with connected that experience to underwater noise pollution. Shipping was visibly occurring within the Reef and those ships were most likely making noise, but no one was making a link between that noise and the health of the ecosystem. The frequency and location of these experiences might be partially responsible since they were rarely reported occurring at tourist dive sites on the Reef.

Living in

In the process of conducting my fieldwork, it became clear that the divers’ experiences with underwater noise were not reflecting the experience of marine creatures. While divers were regularly visiting the reefs, they did not know what it was like to live in the reefy environment.

For many creatures living in the waters of the Great Barrier Reef, from coral polyps to humpback whales, sound is an important medium for how they interpret their environment. From the moment the larvae of a variety of marine species including fish (Tolimieri et al., Reference Tolimieri, Jeffs and Montgomery2000), crustaceans (Montgomery et al., Reference Montgomery, Jeffs, Simpson, Meekan and Tindle2006) and coral polyps (Vermeij et al., Reference Vermeij, Marhaver, Huijbers, Nagelkerken and Simpson2010) decided to leave the miasma of the plankton and settle closer to shore, they begin to listen out for the sounds of the reef. All that sound is being produced by their aquatic kins as they search for food, fight for territory and maintain their schools, which crescendos each morning into the dawn chorus (McCauley & Cato, Reference McCauley and Cato2000). A reef’s health depends on the soniferous activity of its denizens (Rossi et al., Reference Rossi, Connell and Nagelkerken2016).

Unfortunately, the sound world of the reef is being eroded away by increased human activity. The ambient noise level of the world’s oceans has been increasing as much as 3 dB per decade (a doubling in volume) (Ross, Reference Ross1993) although that rate has slowed in recent years and varies by region (Miksis-Olds & Nichols, Reference Miksis-Olds and Nichols2016). Most of this increase in noise is coming from shipping traffic with other sources including naval sonar, petroleum exploration and coastal development (Ross, Reference Ross2005). Small vessel noise has also been indicated as a problem for reefs (Holles et al., Reference Holles, Simpson, Radford, Berten and Lecchini2013).

This increase in noise has a major impact on marine species. The long exposure of increased noise to fish and other marine animals has the possibility to greatly reduce the health of marine ecosystems (Slabbekoorn et al., Reference Slabbekoorn, Bouton, van Opzeeland, Coers, ten Cate and Popper2010). In fish, the increased noise has been shown to increase stress, resulting in impaired growth, lower reproductivity and impaired immune systems (Wysocki et al., Reference Wysocki, Dittami and Ladich2006). Noise can also impair predator avoidance responses, leading to increased fish mortality (Simpson et al., Reference Simpson, Radford, Nedelec, Ferrari, Chivers, McCormick and Meekan2016). Little is known about the effects of noise on invertebrates.

The divers aboard Passions had no experiential knowledge of these things and did not connect their experiences with those living on the reef. Although there might be sonic experiences that disturb human divers, the divers are often unable to translate that to the fishes’ experience. Nagel (Reference Nagel1974) has previously argued that the conscious experience of one species is not fully accessible to another. While others have worked to further develop the argument for non-human agency and how to identify that agency (Dawkins, Reference Dawkins2000; Morton, Reference Morton2017; Wadiwel, Reference Wadiwel2016), they still struggle with the sensual divide between species. The further away one gets from primates, mammals or terrestrial fauna, the harder it is for one to comprehend the life of another being which further limits one’s ability to imagine alternative ways of being. This provides opportunities for humans to ignore potentially harmful experiences which exist outside of our sensorium (Almiron, Reference Almiron2016; Puig de la Bellacasa, Reference Puig de la Bellacasa2017). For divers, it is easier to assume that the fish are deaf to most noise than it is to comprehend how a being can hear in a manner completely different to the human experience.

Multispecies listening

Given Nagel’s (Reference Nagel1974) quandary, would more marine education spark a sympathetic affect among divers? So far, I have outlined how divers might use sound and are affected by it in the practical activities of diving. These experiences build up a knowledge set of sounds for diving and indicate an aural phenomenological experience of diving. But when considering sound in relation to the experience of other beings (thus, detecting noise experiences beyond the human), does a human phenomenology overlap with a fish phenomenology?

The human animal is, fundamentally, terrible at hearing underwater. Our reduced hearing capabilities are a result of biology and physics (Hollien & Feinstein, Reference Hollien, Feinstein, Drew, Lythgoe and Woods1976). The structure of our ears is designed to channel the vibration of the air through the ear canal to the tympanic membrane and transmit it to the cochlea via the ossicle bones. Unfortunately, soft tissue of the human body is approximately the same density as seawater and acoustic waves mostly bypass the aural system (Hollien et al., Reference Hollien, Feinstein, Drew, Lythgoe and Woods1976). Instead, those waves are picked up by the bones of the skull and channelled to the cochlea in a process called bone conduction. Because of the rigidity of bone, hearing sensitivity and range are greatly reduced and favour lower frequencies. In addition, the speed at which sound travels through water (about five times faster than through air) disrupts the listener’s ability to tell from which direction the sound came. While some localisation is possible (Bovet et al., Reference Bovet, Drake, Bernmaschina and Savel1998), the human brain’s capacity to detect the delay between the two ears is greatly limited and decreases as the frequency rises.

While my frequency sensitivity and sense of directionality were deadened, I did start to sense the haptic qualities of sound. The whine of the dive tender as it passed overhead or the rumble from Passions’ bilge pumps when close to the hull would vibrate my entire body. Due to the higher density of water, sound could be felt more readily. For the divers who experience a cargo ship pass by, it was as much the bodily vibrations as the loud sound of the vessel that sent them to the surface. For me, the haptic qualities of sound were the first indication that I might do better approaching underwater sound and its haptic qualities’ relational potentiality by figuring it as acoustic energy. After all, it is these energetic properties of sound that also enable a fish to hear.

There is no one structure for the fish ear, but generally speaking, all fish sense sound through particle motion (Popper & Fay, Reference Popper and Fay2011). Since the fish’s flesh has a similar density to water, it moves in sync with the liquid as sound waves pass through it. Inside the fish’s head are small, hard objects called otoliths which are denser than the fish’s body and cause it to vibrate out of sync with the soft flesh. The otolith pushes and pulls on the sensory hairs inside the fish’s ear, causing the fish to hear. Fish can also sense particle motion through their lateral line, a string of sense organs running down a fish’s side.

Humans can only gain an inkling of how marine creatures experience sound. In this project, I had set out to understand how people grasped the sonic worlds of marine beings, but the phenomenological approach can only examine how people experience the sonic worlds containing marine beings. The cyborg circuits of the dive apparatus could only afford me to be immersed in the environment. Rather than a posthuman listening, diving was more peri-human – attending the experiences only at the border of the human body. It is unlikely that diving could ever allow a human to develop a complete empathy for non-human sonic experiences. I would need to find a new way of listening.

Learning to hear

To overcome my physiological limitations, I turned to other technologies of listening. I had acquired a hydrophone—underwater microphone—and I listened in from the bow of the boat after dives. This device could pick up acoustic energy and transduce them into audible sound. The hydrophone forced me to consider the various mechanical energies interacting in the surrounding water including the reflection of acoustic waves on the surface and the movement of water particles against the instrument. Once I put on my headphones, a new sonic world opened. This cyborgic configuration more fully entangled me in a network of material relationships allowed for greater acoustic clarity.

Everything above 500 Hz in the audible range was dominated by the crackle of snapping shrimp. Occasionally, I had the opportunity to hear the grunts and chirps of fish. Or at least I thought I heard them. The primary deficit of using a hydrophone is that the listener is only partially transported underwater. While I could listen in, I rarely was able to see what I heard. Later, I learned that this was a perpetual problem in underwater bioacoustics. Unlike bird calls, there is no generally available resource to learn fish communication. There are limited databases of fish sounds and most focus on ecologies of the Northern temperate oceans or freshwater ecosystems. Without a functioning database, marine auditors must rely on visual confirmation to identify most sources. The only time I could be certain of the origin of a call was when a trio of batfish took interest in my hydrophone and auditioned before it.

Mostly, I would hear the sounds of human activity. The respirations of introductory divers, the whine of a buoyancy control devices (BCD) inflating, or the clang clang clang of a tank banger were common. Then there was the dive tender: a small rubber dinghy that would patrol the area and assist any passengers needing aid or occasionally tow divers to remote dive sites. The whine of the motor could be obnoxiously loud. I had experienced the noise while diving, but it was even worse with the hydrophone. Yet the fish did not seem to mind, at least as far as I could tell from listening in. On the few occasions when I did continuously hear fish grunting, the presence of the dive tender did not appear to affect their behaviour.

Apart from the dive tender, little anthropogenic noise pollution could be detected. It was not just the deadened sense of the divers; noise was not seeping into these dive sites. There was one exception: Towards the end of August, on Flynn Reef, one of the larger dive boats in the area left and came back to its mooring while I had my hydrophone in the water. Its departure from and return to the reef were gradual but noticeable above the typical reefy soundscape. Its manoeuvres to position itself at the mooring were the largest sources of noise. For each manoeuvre, the boat’s propellers would spring to life and explode with sound, producing noticeably higher sound levels in the lower frequencies.

Hearing does not have to be in the moment of listening in. Most of the time I was recording off the boat, I was too concerned about maintaining the equipment or monitoring what was going on around me. Many sounds I could not hear until I listened to the recordings again back at my desk. The day or so after a trip out to the Reef, I would replay my recordings using Adobe Audition. Not only would the acousmatic (sound without a visible source) listening allow me to focus on individual elements, but the computer program also generated a spectrogram, allowing me to see sound (Figure 1).

Figure 1. Spectrogram from recording of moving dive boat. Tim on top axis, frequency on right axis.

A spectrogram is a temporal graph with frequency on the vertical axis and duration on the horizontal. Each sound is colour-coded for intensity with purple being the least intense, reds and oranges in the middle and yellow the most intense. The low sounds of Passions form a thin yellow base to the spectrogram and the clicks from the snapping shrimp striate the graph in thin, broadband lines of varying colours. Upon this background, other sounds begin to appear. A BCD inflating takes the form of a gradually ascending line while tank bangers form decaying triangles. Most fish sounds produce a percussive series of dots near the bottom. Here, the effect of noise becomes visible. As anthropogenic noise increases, the fish chortles and grunts begin to blend in and become indistinct from the sounds of engines as they become masked.

Making others hear

Integrating sound into the tourist Reef experience is a constant challenge. Among all the other stressors to the ecosystem and the demands from tourists, noise does not seem so important. The average Reef visitor has a superficial level of knowledge about the Reef ecosystem mostly drawn from popular documentaries, news reports and guidebooks, few of which seriously consider the soundscape. Noise, to most visitors, blends into all the other bits of information about the aquatic environment. It was less notable because it was less noticeable, say compared to coral bleaching. The crew must cater to the educational needs of the passengers and so they focus on more approachable topics of identification and the life cycles of popular creatures.

Heading home from the Reef, the crew would hold their daily marine life talks. It is a brief lesson on coral, reef ecology, key reef species and conservation. As part of my agreement with Passions, I added my own segment about sound. This was the final attempt of the crew to drive home any environmental or educational message and my last opportunity to get the passengers to hear noise.

Using iPad tablets, one of the divemasters started to explain the size of the marine park, the anatomy of coral and how to identify various types. Using my own Surface tablet and a portable speaker, I introduced the audience to the sounds of the Reef, playing either that day’s recording or another recent one. Passengers were often shocked that the crackle of the Reef is not produced by foraging fish, but by little shrimp. They also enjoyed recordings of fish choruses and whale songs. Closing out the marine life talk, the instructor promoted Passions of Paradise’s conservation efforts and allied programmes.

Making things weird

Anthropologists are known for making things weird. It is central to our pedagogy. One of the first obstacles we must tackle in every introductory course is to dismantle our students’ ethnocentrism—their bias towards their own cultural practice—and introduce a doctrine of cultural relativism—a framework that states all cultures are qualitatively different and therefore cannot be ranked. We accomplish this, in part, by challenging our students’ sense of what is “normal” through a process of defamiliarisation, or “making the familiar strange” as the saying goes in our profession. Defamiliarisation coupled with exposure to other cultures guides our students towards evaluating cultural practices through a lens of cultural relativism.

The practice of defamiliarisation has its limits. Anthropologists have acknowledged that ontological gaps between them and the cultural representatives persist due to varied life histories (Paleček & Risjord, Reference Paleček and Risjord2013). Hastrup (Reference Hastrup1995) identifies in her work with Icelanders that anthropological research happens at a “third space” in which the anthropologist and representative of the culture under investigation come in contact. The anthropologist cannot fully experience another culture, nor can a representative fully indoctrinate the anthropologist. The third space is a zone of contact where understanding is imperfect, but where understanding can occur. The third space in anthropological research exists because the dichotomy between ethnocentrism and cultural relativism is partly false. Given that the anthropologist is unable to fully remove themselves from their own culture, complete cultural relativism becomes impractical (Eriksen, Reference Eriksen1995). Anthropologists are trained to be consistently critical of their own subjective position in the contact zone, not just to defend against bias but to also to ensure that important domains of knowledge are not missed (Bernard, Reference Bernard2006).

Contact zones are also utilised in multispecies ethnographies (Helmreich, Reference Helmreich2009; Tsing, Reference Tsing2015), philosophy (Haraway, Reference Haraway2008; Isaacs & Otruba, Reference Isaacs and Otruba2019) and education (Lussier & Ruitenberg, Reference Lussier and Ruitenberg2022; Taylor & Pacini-Ketchabow Reference Taylor and Pacini-Ketchabaw2015) as a means of reorienting to an ecocentric perspective. Contact acts “as a lens for better appreciating the complexity of entanglement in our more-than-human world” (Isaacs & Otruba, Reference Isaacs and Otruba2019, p.700). The use of contact zones in environmental research and environmental education calls participants to check cultural and value biases (Payne & Wattchow, Reference Payne and Wattchow2009) thus correcting for the general view of anthropocentrism that is rightly decried by environmental scholarship (Kopnina et al, Reference Kopnina, Washington, Taylor and Piccolo2018; Kopnina, Reference Kopnina2012). This still does little to address the cognitive anthropocentrism which is “the tendency to reason about unfamiliar biological species or processes by analogy to humans” (Coley & Tanner, Reference Coley and Tanner2012, p.209) as participants are not asked to critically interrogate their innate human subjectivity. Additionally, as Haraway (Reference Haraway2008) points out, contact zones are not without their politics as humans often have the privilege of deciding where contact takes place and even who lives and dies within those zones. To further illustrate this point, let me provide one more ethnographic account.

In the winter of 2018, I was invited onto the crown-of-thorns starfish (COTS) control boat as part of a group at the Reef Restoration Symposium. COTS are a significant stressor on the Great Barrier Reef. The large, spiky sea stars eat coral polyps and can destroy entire reef ecosystems if their populations become too large (De’ath et al., Reference De’ath, Fabricius, Sweatman and Puotinen2012; Milne et al., Reference Milne, Anand and Bauch2023). The COTS Control Program is an attempt to reduce the number of mature COTS in the marine park to prevent a destructive outbreak. COTS Control divers inject each animal with a substance that causes an allergic reaction resulting in the sea star breaking apart. On that day, we were given a live demonstration of the process. One COTS diver removed a large specimen from its plastic container and then proceeded to inject the creature. My fellow guests watched in fascination, but I felt sickened by the execution. I was horrified for the sea star even if it did not know what was happening to it. Fortunately, we were spared the sight of the animal breaking apart, but I wondered what it would be like to slowly disintegrate.

I know I am anthropomorphising. The lived experience of a COTS is relatively simple. It does not have a centralised brain for higher-order thought, and it probably does not feel pain as we think of it (Smith et al., Reference Smith, Rotgans, Lang, Johnston, Wang, Suwansa-ard, Bose, Satoh, Egertova, Hall, Byrne, Elphick, Motti and Cummins2023). I know these things, but I still want to project onto it my own way of sensing. I have been warned against the dangers of anthropomorphising, yet I cannot fully prevent myself from doing it.

To be clear, anthropomorphism is a consequence of anthropocentrism. If the entire world is only understood through human experience, then radically other beings must be transformed into near-humans for us to make sense of their experiences. When uncritically engaged, these become ethically dubious practices which set up a faulty substitute for the organism itself. Rather than creating a representation of the creature, we have “humans in animal drag” (Toadvine, Reference Toadvine, Painter and Lotz2007, p. 39).

But we cannot seem to stop from anthropomorphising. Even among the most careful scientists, non-human experience is often described through analogy (Coley & Tanner, Reference Coley and Tanner2012). In part, it is a result of the shortcomings of human language to describe non-shared experience. But the issue is more deeply rooted. Even non-linguistic forms of communication transform non-human experience into anthropocentric formats. Whale songs are sped up to be made audible to human researchers (Payne, Reference Payne1995) and infrared light is made visible so that researchers might “see” a snake’s ability to perceive heat (Schraft et al., Reference Schraft, Bakken and Clark2019). Those who attempt to remove any trace of anthropomorphism through purely technical language threaten to succumb to mechanomorphism (Karlsson, Reference Karlsson2012), an equally dubious position. Some researchers, partially those advocating for animal welfare, suggest utilising anthropomorphism as a means to rethink animal research, participation and sources of harm (Hayward, Reference Hayward2010; Karlsson, Reference Karlsson2012; Morton et al., Reference Morton, Burghardt and Smith1990).

Anthropomorphism creates a double bind. Removing anthropomorphism risks creating a subject/object relationship while engaging with it risks ignoring vital differences between multispecies subjectivises. The third way out could be to engage in a critical anthropocentrism which recognises that all representations of non-human experiences are attempts to escape our own subjectivity which must continuously fail in some manner. Critical anthropocentrism in multispecies activities requires the practitioner to critically reflect on how their human subjectivity and capacity inform their experiences and how they are coming to know other subjectivities. Critical reflection is not limited to the directly experienced, but also examines how nonhuman experiences must be transformed for our understanding. It makes the human experience weird so as to better understand subjective differences in multispecies contact zones.

Without critical anthropocentrism, ecocentrism risks introducing unexamined biases into multispecies relationships, particularly for experiences outside of the human sensorium. From an environmental education perspective, inclusion of critical anthropocentrism in immersive and contact-oriented pedagogies may allow students to better examine the boundaries of the experience, understand more fully the impact of human action on the environment and become more critical of their own actions and interventions.

Mediating contact zones in environmental education

The struggle to understanding fishy sound worlds for the divers and for myself were the result of insufficient engagement in the contact zone of the Reef because engagement relied on the uncritical use of human-centred phenomenology. Once I critically examined my own bodily capacities, I was able to reorient myself in the contact zone so that fish and noisy machines could be heard. That reorientation required mediation. By mediation, I mean this in the strong sense of utilising technology and media. Mediation was also required to let others hear.

While there are strong arguments in environmental education for immersive experience (Riley & White, Reference Riley and White2019; Whitehouse et al., Reference Whitehouse, Taylor, Evans, Doyle, Sellwood and Zee2017), as I stated at the beginning of this article, the feral entities of the Anthropocene often escape our ability to perceive them. Where our embodied capacitances fail, we must then enrol other sensing technologies. In my research on underwater sound, I used hydrophones, speakers and specialised software (as well as fins, snorkel, mask and dive gear). These technologies extended my sensory capabilities by compensating for my physical limitations or enrolling new senses to assist when my ears reached their limit. To remain critical of our mediated experience we once again pay attention to the “cyborgic circuits” as Helmreich (Reference Helmreich2007) requests. In the case of underwater listening, paying attention to the hydrophone technology helps me to learn the differences between my hearing as a land-based mammal and that of a fish. Posthumanism asks us to reconfigure human/non-human relationality, but we may still need phenomenological practices to help us identify our own physical limitations and biases. While I can no longer create a one-to-one relation through shared experience, I learn that I cannot base my relationship with environmental stressors solely through my own sensory experience.

Those who study environmental sounds often turn to artistic practice to gain a better understanding of their field and the sense of difference between them and other beings (Bandt & Barclay, Reference Bandt and Barclay2017; Westerkamp, Reference Westerkamp2002). Interpreting scientific and environmental knowledge through art helps practitioners and observers to develop understanding through alternative routes such as through the affective domain (Friedman, Reference Friedman2013). Incorporating artistic practice into education activities in contact zones may assist students in analysing mediated experiences by engaging effective responses.

I encourage environmental educators to explore how critical anthropocentrism and mediation can be incorporated into curriculum to make feral entities more apparent. A mature environmental education pedagogy needs to pay more attention to the subjective differences between human and non-human experience. Curriculum that does not address experiences beyond the human sensorium risks allowing anthropogenic disturbances like underwater noise pollution to continue to propagate. In my failure to become a fish, I learned more about what it means to be a human trying to live in and with the Anthropocene.

Acknowledgements

I would like to thank the owners and crew of Passions of Paradise III for providing me space on their vessel to conduct my research and guide my dives, Geoff McPherson for teaching me how to use my hydrophone and to Charles St-Germain who taught me how to dive. I would also like to thank Hilary Whitehouse for graciously reviewing my early draft and helping me navigate the environmental education literature.

Financial support

The study discussed in this paper was funded by student research funds from James Cook University and Aarhus University.

Ethical standard

This research was conducted with ethical approval (H6927) from James Cook University.

Author biography

Matthew Buttacavoli (PhD) is an anthropologist focusing on science and technology. His doctoral thesis explores the sonic relationships between humans and non-humans as mitigated through audio technology. He has conducted ethnographic research in the Great Barrier Reef and in the Salish Sea. In addition to his scholarly research, he also creates sound art to further explore sonic relationships.

References

Almiron, N. (2016). Beyond anthropocentrism: Critical animal studies and the political economy of communication. The Political Economy of Communication, 4(2), 472.Google Scholar
Bandt, R., & Barclay, L. (2017). Sonic activism in the antipodes. n.paradoxa, 40, 94103.Google Scholar
Barad, K. (2007). Meeting the universe halfway: Quantum physics and the entanglement of matter and meaning. Duke University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bernard, H.R. (2006). Research methods in anthropology: Qualitative and quantitative approaches (4th ed,). AltaMira Press.Google Scholar
Bonnett, M. (2019). Interpreting the idea of the Anthropocene, and its relevance to education. On Education. Journal for Research and Debate, 2(4). DOI: 10.17899/on_ed.2019.4.2.Google Scholar
Bovet, P., Drake, C., Bernmaschina, F., & Savel, S. (1998). Underwater sound localization: Role of interaural time and intensity differences. The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 103(5), 849850. DOI: 10.1121/1.421509.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Cocks, S., & Simpson, S. (2015). Anthropocentric and ecocentric: An application of environmental philosophy to outdoor recreation and environmental education. Journal of Environmental Education, 38(3), 216227. DOI: 10.1177/1053825915571750.Google Scholar
Coley, J., & Tanner, K. (2012). Common origins of diverse misconceptions: Cognitive principles and the development of biology thinking. CBE Life Sciences Education, 11(3), 209215. DOI: 10.1187/cbe.12-06-0074.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Davies, B. (2021). Moving beyond (id)entities, toward emergent becomings of the world and its mattering. Australian Journal of Environmental Education, 38(3-4), 311327. DOI: 10.1017/aee.2021.20.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Dawkins, M.S. (2000). Animal minds and animal emotions. American Zoologist, 40(6), 883888. DOI: 10.1093/icb/40.6.883.Google Scholar
De’ath, G., Fabricius, K., Sweatman, H., & Puotinen, M. (2012). The 27-year decline of coral cover on the Great Barrier Reef and its causes. Proceedings of The National Academy of Sciences of The United States of America, 109(44), 1799517999. DOI: 10.1073/pnas.1208909109.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Despret, V. (2013). Responding bodies and partial affinities in human-animal worlds. Theory, Culture & Society, 7(8), 5176. DOI: 10.1177/0263276413496852.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Eriksen, T.H. (1995). Small places, large issues: An introduction to social and cultural anthropology (2nd ed.). Pluto Press.Google Scholar
Friedman, A. (2013). Reflections on communicating science through art. Curator: The Museum Journal, 56(1), 39. DOI: 10.1111/cura.12001.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Haraway, D. (1991). Simians, cyborgs, and women: The reinvention of nature. Routledge.Google Scholar
Haraway, D. (2008). When species meet. University of Minnesota Press.Google Scholar
Harries-Jones, P. (2005). Understanding ecological aesthetics: The challenge of Bateson. Cybernetics and Human Knowledge, 12(1-2), 6174.Google Scholar
Hastrup, K. (1995). A passage to anthropology: Between experience and theory. Routledge.Google Scholar
Hayward, E. (2010). FINGERYEYES: Impressions of cup corals. Cultural Anthropology, 25(4), 577599. DOI: 10.1111/j.1548-1360.2010.01070.x.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Helmreich, S. (2007). An anthropologist underwater: Immersive soundscapes, submarine cyborgs, and transductive ethnography. American Ethnologist, 34(4), 621641. DOI: 10.1525/ae.2007.34.4.621.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Helmreich, S. (2009). Alien ocean: Anthropological voyages in microbial seas. University of California Press.Google Scholar
Holles, S., Simpson, S., Radford, A., Berten, L., & Lecchini, D. (2013). Boat noise disrupts orientation behaviour in coral reef fish. Marine Ecology Progress Series, 485, 295300. DOI: 10.3354/meps10346.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hollien, H.J., & Feinstein, H.S. (1976). Hearing in divers. In Drew, E.A., Lythgoe, J.N. & Woods, J.D. (Eds.), Underwater research (pp. 81138). Academic Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Howard, C. (2017). Posthuman anthropology? Facing up to planetary conviviality in the Anthropocene. Language Choices, 6(1), 2025.Google Scholar
Isaacs, J., & Otruba, A. (2019). Guest introduction: More-than-human contact zones. ENE: Nature and Space, 2 (4), 697711. DOI: 10.1177/2514848619855369.Google Scholar
Jukes, S., & Reeves, Y. (2020). More-than-human stories: Experimental co-productions in outdoor environmental education pedagogy. Environmental Education Research, 26(9-10), 12941312. DOI: 10.1080/13504622.2019.1699027.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Karlsson, F. (2012). Critical anthropomorphism and animal ethics. Journal of Agricultural and Environmental Ethics, 25(5), 707720. DOI: 10.1007/s10806-011-9349-8.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kervinen, A., Hohti, R., Rautio, R., Saari, M., Tammi, T., & Aivelo, T. (2024). Ratty places – unsettling human-centeredness in ecological inquiry with young people. Environmental Education Research, 118. DOI: 10.1080/13504622.2024.2314037.Google Scholar
Kopnina, H. (2012). Towards conservational anthropology: Addressing anthropocentric bias in anthropology. Dialectic Anthropology, 36(1-2), 127146. DOI: 10.1007/s10624-012-9265-y.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kopnina, H., Washington, H., Taylor, B., & Piccolo, J. (2018). Anthropocentrism: More than just a misunderstood problem. Journal of Agricultural and Environmental Ethics, 31(1), 109127. DOI: 10.1007/s10806-018-9711-1.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lussier, J., & Ruitenberg, C. (2022). Touch points: Educative experiences in multispecies contact zones. Philosophy of Education, 78(2), 3751. DOI: 10.47925/78.2.037.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
McCauley, R., & Cato, R. (2000). Patterns of fish calling in the nearshore environment in the Great Barrier Reef. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B, 355(1401), 12891293. DOI: 10.1098/rstb.2000.0686.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Miksis-Olds, J., & Nichols, S. (2016). Is low frequency ocean sound increasing globally? Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 129(1), 501511. DOI: 10.1121/1.4938237.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Milne, R., Anand, M., & Bauch, C.T. (2023). Preparing for and managing crown-of-thorns starfish outbreaks on reefs under threat from interacting anthropogenic stressors. Ecological Modelling, 484, 110443. DOI: 10.1016/j.ecolmodel.2023.110443.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Montgomery, J., Jeffs, A., Simpson, S., Meekan, M., & Tindle, C. (2006). Sound as an orientation cue for the pelagic larvae of reef fishes and decapod crustaceans. Advances in Marine Biology, 51, 143196. DOI: 10.1016/S0065-2881(06)51003-X.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Morton, D., Burghardt, G., & Smith, J. (1990). Animals, science, and ethics -- Section III. Critical anthropomorphism, animal suffering, and the ecological context. Hastings Center Report, 20(3), S139.Google Scholar
Morton, T. (2017). Humankind: Solidarity with nonhuman people. Verso.Google Scholar
Nagel, T. (1974). What is it like to be a bat? The Philosophical Review, 83(4), 435450.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Oliveros, P. (1998). The root of the moment. Drogue Press.Google Scholar
Paleček, M., & Risjord, M. (2013). Relativism and the ontological turn within anthropology. Philosophy of the Social Sciences, 43(1), 323. DOI: 10.1177/0048393112463335.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Payne, P.G., & Wattchow, B. (2009). Phenomenological deconstruction, slow pedagogy and the corporeal turn in wild environmental/outdoor education. Canadian Journal of Environmental Education, 14(1), 1532. DOI: 10.1007/BF03401021.Google Scholar
Payne, R. (1995). Among whales. Scribner.Google Scholar
Picken, F., & Ferguson, T. (2014). Diving with Donna Haraway and the promise of a blue planet. Environmental and Planning D: Society and Space, 32(2), 329341. DOI: 10.1068/d13016p.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Popper, A., & Fay, R. (2011). Rethinking sound detection by fishes. Hearing Research, 273(1-2), 2536. DOI: 10.1016/j.heares.2009.12.023.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Puig de la Bellacasa, M. (2017). Matters of care: Speculative ethics in more than human worlds. University of Minnesota Press.Google Scholar
Rautio, P., Hohti, R., Tammi, T., & Ylirisku, H. (2022a). Multiple worlds and strange objects: Environmental education research as an additive practice. Australian Journal of Environmental Education, 38(3-4), 214226. DOI: 10.1017/aee.2022.29.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Rautio, P., Tammi, T., Aivelo, T., Hohti, R., Kervinen, A., & Saari, M. (2022b). For whom? By whom”: Critical perspectives of participation in ecological citizen science. Cultural Studies of Science Education, 17(3), 765793. DOI: 10.1007/s11422-021-10099-9.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Riley, K., & White, P. (2019). Attuning-with, affect, and assemblages of relations in a transdisciplinary environmental education. Australian Journal of Environmental Education, 35(3), 262272. DOI: 10.1017/aee.2019.30.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ross, D. (1993). On ocean underwater ambient noise. Acoustic Bulletin, 18, 58.Google Scholar
Ross, D. (2005). Ship sources of ambient noise. IEEE Journal of Oceanic Engineering, 30(2), 257261. DOI: 10.1109/JOE.2005.850879.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Rossi, T., Connell, S., & Nagelkerken, I. (2016). Silent oceans: Ocean acidification impoverishes natural soundscapes by altering production of the world’s noisiest marine invertebrate. Proceedings of the Royal Society B, 283, 20153046. DOI: 10.1098/rspb.2015.3046.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Schafer, R.M. (1977). The tuning of the world. Alfred A. Knopf.Google Scholar
Schraft, H., Bakken, G., & Clark, R. (2019). Infrared-sensing snakes select ambush orientation based on thermal backgrounds. Scientific Reports, 9 (1), 3950. DOI: 10.1038/s41598-019-40466-0.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Schutz, A. (1962). Collected papers I: The problem of social reality. Martinus Nijhoff.Google Scholar
Simpson, S., Radford, A., Nedelec, S., Ferrari, M., Chivers, D., McCormick, M., & Meekan, M. (2016). Anthropogenic noise increases fish mortality by predation. Nature Communications, 7(10544), 17. DOI: 10.1038/ncomms10544.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Slabbekoorn, H., Bouton, N., van Opzeeland, I., Coers, A., ten Cate, C., & Popper, A. (2010). A noisy spring: The impact of globally rising underwater sound levels on fish. Trends in Ecology and Evolution, 25(7), 419427. DOI: 10.1016/j.tree.2010.04.005.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Smith, M.K., Rotgans, B.A., Lang, T., Johnston, R., Wang, T., Suwansa-ard, S., Bose, U., Satoh, N., Egertova, M., Hall, M.R., Byrne, M., Elphick, M.R., Motti, C.A., Cummins, S.F. (2023). Structure and proteomic analysis of the crown-of-thorns starfish (Acanthaster sp.) radial nerve cord. Scientific Reports, 13(1), 3349. DOI: 10.1038/s41598-023-30425-1.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Smith, R.A., & Smith, C.M. (1970). Aesthetics and environmental education. The Journal of Aesthetic Education, 4(4), 125140.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Stewart, A.J. (2018). A Murray Cod assemblage: Re/considering riverScape pedagogy. The Journal of Environmental Education, 49(2), 130141. DOI: 10.1080/00958964.2017.1417224.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Taylor, A., & Pacini-Ketchabaw, V. (2015). Learning with children, ants, and worms in the Anthropocene: Towards a common world pedagogy of multispecies vulnerability. Pedagogy, Culture & Society, 23(4), 507529. DOI: 10.1080/14681366.2015.1039050.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Toadvine, T. (2007). How not to be a jellyfish: Human exceptionalism and the ontology of reflection. In Painter, C. & Lotz, C. (Eds.), Phenomenology and the non-human animal: At the limits of experience (pp. 3955). Springer.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Tolimieri, N., Jeffs, A., & Montgomery, J. (2000). Ambient sound as a cue for navigation by the pelagic larvae of reef fishes. Marine Ecology Progress Series, 207, 219224. DOI: 10.3354/MEPS207219.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Tsing, A.L. (2015). The mushroom at the end of the world: On the possibility of life in capitalist ruins. Princeton University Press.Google Scholar
Tsing, A.L., Deger, J., Keleman Saxena, A., & Zhou, F. (2021). Feral atlas: The more-than-human Anthropocene. Stanford University Press. DOI: 10.21627/2020fa Google Scholar
Vermeij, M., Marhaver, K., Huijbers, C., Nagelkerken, I., & Simpson, S. (2010). Coral larvae move towards reef sounds. PLoS One, 5(5), e10660. DOI: 10.1371/journal.pone.0010660.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Wadiwel, D.J. (2016). Do fish resist? Cultural Studies Review, 22(1), 196242. DOI: 10.5130/csr.v22i1.4363.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Westerkamp, H. (2002). Linking soundscape composition and acoustic ecology. Organised Sound, 7(1), 5156. DOI: 10.1017/S1355771802001085.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Whitehouse, H., Taylor, M., Evans, N.S., Doyle, T., Sellwood, J., & Zee, R. (2017). A Sea Country learning partnership in times of Anthropocenic risk: Offshore coral reef education and our story of practice. Australian Journal of Environmental Education, 22(3), 160170. DOI: 10.1017/aee.2017.28.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Wysocki, L., Dittami, J., & Ladich, F. (2006). Ship noise and cortisol secretion in European freshwater fishes. Biological Conservation, 128(4), 501508. DOI: 10.1016/j.biocon.2005.10.020.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Figure 0

Figure 1. Spectrogram from recording of moving dive boat. Tim on top axis, frequency on right axis.