Hostname: page-component-cd9895bd7-gbm5v Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-12-26T21:50:20.711Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Social and linguistic input in low-income African American mother–child dyads from 1 month through 2 years: Relations to vocabulary development

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  13 October 2011

PRIYA M. SHIMPI*
Affiliation:
Mills College
ALICIA FEDEWA
Affiliation:
University of Kentucky
SYDNEY HANS
Affiliation:
University of Chicago
*
ADDRESS FOR CORRESPONDENCE Priya Mariana Shimpi, 5000 MacArthur Boulevard, Oakland, CA 94613. E-mail: [email protected]

Abstract

The relation of social and linguistic input measures to early vocabulary development was examined in 30 low-income African American mother–infant pairs. Observations were conducted when the child was 0 years, 1 month (0;1), 0;4, 0;8, 1;0, 1;6, and 2;0. Maternal input was coded for word types and tokens, contingent responsiveness, and directiveness. Children's outcome measures included productive vocabulary at 1;6 and 2;0. Patterns of social and linguistic input were highly consistent over time. Significant positive relations were found between linguistic input measures and child vocabulary development. Findings for social input measures included positive relations between directive input and child word types, which differs from previous research with European American middle-class samples.

Type
Articles
Copyright
Copyright © Cambridge University Press 2011

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

REFERENCES

Akhtar, N. (2005). Is joint attention necessary for early language learning? In Homer, B. D. & Tamis-LeMonda, C. S. (Eds.), The development of social cognition and communication (pp. 165179). Mahway, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Akhtar, N., Dunham, F., & Dunham, P. (1991). Directive interactions and early vocabulary development: The role of joint attentional focus. Journal of Child Language, 18, 4150.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Akhtar, N., & Gernsbacher, M. A. (2007). Joint attention and vocabulary development: A critical look. Language and Linguistics Compass, 1, 195207.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Akhtar, N., & Tomasello, M. (2000). The social nature of words and word learning. In Golinkoff, R. & Hirsh-Pasek, K. (Eds.), Becoming a word learner: A debate on lexical acquisition. Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Baldwin, D. (1991). Infants’ contribution to the achievement of joint reference. Child Development, 62, 875890.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Baldwin, D. (1993). Infants’ ability to consult the speaker for clues to word reference. Journal of Child Language, 20, 395418.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Baumwell, L., Tamis-LeMonda, C. S., & Bornstein, M. H. (1997). Maternal verbal sensitivity and child language comprehension. Infant Behavior and Development, 20, 247258.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bernstein, V. J., & Hans, S. L. (1994). Predicting the developmental outcome of two-year-old children born exposed to methadone: Impact of social-environmental risk factors. Journal of Clinical Child Psychology, 23, 349359.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bigelow, A., & Rochat, P. (2006). Two-month-old infants’ sensitivity to social contingency in mother–infant and stranger–infant interaction. Infancy, 9, 313325.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bornstein, M. H., Haynes, M. O., & Painter, K. M. (1998). Sources of child vocabulary competence: A multivariate model. Journal of Child Language, 25, 367393.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Brooks-Gunn, J., & Markman, L. (2005). The contribution of parenting to ethnic and racial gaps: In school readiness. Future of Children, 15, 139168.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Burns, A., & Radford, J. (2008). Parent–child interaction in Nigerian families: Conversation analysis, context and culture. Child Language Teaching and Therapy, 24, 193209.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Carpenter, M., Nagell, K., & Tomasello, M. (1998). Social cognition, joint attention, and communicative competence from 9 to 15 months of age. Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development, 63, 1142.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Chavajay, P., & Rogoff, B. (1999). Cultural variation in management of attention by children and their caregivers. Developmental Psychology, 35, 10791090.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Childers, J., Vaughan, J., & Burquest, D. (2007). Joint attention and word learning in Ngas-speaking toddlers. Journal of Child Language, 34, 199225.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gaskins, S. (1999). Children's daily lives in a Mayan village: A case study of culturally constructed roles and activities. In Göncü, A. (Ed.), Children's engagement in the world: Sociocultural perspectives (pp. 2560). New York: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Goldin-Meadow, S. (1999). The role of gesture in communication and thinking. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 3, 419429.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Goldin-Meadow, S., & Saltzman, J. (2000). The cultural bounds of maternal accommodation: How Chinese and American mothers communicate with deaf and hearing children. Psychological Science, 11, 307314.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Grattan, M., & Hans, S. (1996). Motor behavior in children exposed prenatally to drugs. Physical and Occupational Therapy in Pediatrics, 16, 89109.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
The Hanen Centre. (n.d.). Research summary: It Takes Two to Talk™. Retrieved from http://www.hanen.org/web/Home/HanenPrograms/ItTakesTwotoTalk/Research/tabid/87/Default.aspxGoogle Scholar
Harris, M., Jones, D., Brookes, S., & Grant, J. (1986). Relations between the non-verbal context of maternal speech and rate of language development. British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 4, 261268.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hart, B., & Risley, T. (1995). Meaningful differences in the everyday experiences of young American children. Baltimore, MD: Brookes.Google Scholar
Heath, S. B. (1983). Ways with words: Language, life, and work in communities and classrooms. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hoff, E. (2003a). Causes and consequences of SES-related differences in parent-to-child speech. In Bornstein, M. & Bradley, R. (Eds.), Socioeconomic status, parenting, and child development (pp. 147160). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Hoff, E. (2003b). The specificity of environmental influence: Socioeconomic status affects early vocabulary development via maternal speech. Child Development, 74, 13681378.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hoff, E., & Naigles, L. (2002). How children use input to acquire a lexicon. Child Development, 73, 418433.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hoff-Ginsberg, E. (1991). Mother–child conversation in different social classes and communicative settings. Child Development, 62, 782796.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hoff-Ginsberg, E. (1994). Influences of mother and child on maternal talkativeness. Discourse Processes, 18, 105117.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hollich, G. J., Hirsh-Pasek, K., & Golinkoff, R. M. (2000). Breaking the language barrier: An emergentist coalition model for the origins of word learning. Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development, 65(3, Serial No. 262).Google ScholarPubMed
Hollingshead, A. B., & Redlich, F. C. (1958). Social class and mental illness: A community study. New York: Wiley.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Huttenlocher, J., Haight, W., Bryk, A., Seltzer, M., & Lyons, T. (1991). Early vocabulary growth: Relation to language input and gender. Developmental Psychology, 27, 236248.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Johnston, J. R., & Wong, M. (2002). Cultural differences in beliefs and practices concerning talk to children. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 45, 916926.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Laakso, M. L., Poikkeus, A. M., Eklund, K., & Lyytinen, P. (1999). Social interactional behaviors and symbolic play competence as predictors of language development and their associations with maternal attention-directing strategies. Infant Behavior and Development, 22, 541556.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lawrence, V. W., & Shipley, E. F. (1996). Parental speech to middle-and working-class children from two racial groups in three settings. Applied Psycholinguistics, 17, 233255.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Manolson, A. (1992). It Takes Two to Talk. Toronto: Hanen Centre.Google Scholar
Masur, E. (1982). Mothers’ responses to infants’ object-related gestures: Influences on lexical development. Journal of Child Language, 9, 2330.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Masur, E., Flynn, V., & Eichorst, D. (2005). Maternal responsive and directive behaviors and utterances as predictors of children's lexical development. Journal of Child Language, 32, 6391.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Naigles, L. R., & Hoff-Ginsberg, E. (1998). Why are some verbs learned before other verbs? Effects of input frequency and structure on children's early verb use. Journal of Child Language, 25, 95120.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ochs, E., & Schieffelin, B. B. (1984). Language acquisition and socialization: Three developmental stories. In Shweder, R. & LeVine, R. (Eds.), Culture theory: Essays on mind, self and emotion (pp. 276320). New York: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Pan, B., Rowe, M., Singer, J., & Snow, C. (2005). Maternal correlates of growth in toddler vocabulary production in low-income families. Child Development, 76, 763782.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Pan, B., Rowe, M., Spier, E., & Tamis-LeMonda, T. (2004). Measuring productive vocabulary of toddlers in low-income families: concurrent and predictive validity of three sources of data. Journal of Child Language, 31, 587608.Google ScholarPubMed
Poulson, C. L. (1983). Differential reinforcement of other-than-vocalization as a control procedure in the conditioning of infant vocalization rate. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 36, 471489.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Prorok, E. M. S. (1980). Mother–child verbal exchange: A descriptive study of young children's verbal behavior. Journal of Psycholinguistic Research, 9, 451471.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Rogoff, B. (2003). The cultural nature of human development. Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Rogoff, B., Malkin, C., & Gillbride, K. (1984). Interaction with babies as guidance in development. New Directions for Child Development, 23, 3144.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Rogoff, B., Mistry, J., Göncü, A., & Mosier, C. (1993). Guided participation in cultural activity by toddlers and caregivers. Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development, 58, 1174.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rollins, P. R. (2003). Caregivers contingent comments to 9-month-old infants: Relationships with later language. Applied Psycholinguistics, 24, 221234.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Rollins, P. R., & Snow, C. (1998). Shared attention and grammatical development in typical children and children with autism. Journal of Child Language, 25, 653673.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rooparine, J., Fouts, H., Lamb, M., & Lewis-Elligan, T. (2005). Mothers’ and fathers’ behavior toward their 3- to 4-month-old infants in lower, middle, and upper socioeconomic African American families. Developmental Psychology, 41, 723732.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Rowe, M. (2000). Pointing and talk by low-income mothers and their 14-month-old children. First Language, 20, 305330.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Saxon, T., & Reilly, J. (1998). Language competence and joint attention in mother–toddler dyads. Early Child Development & Care, 142, 3342.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Scheffner Hammer, C., & Weiss, A. (1999). Guiding language development: How African American mothers and their infants structure play interactions. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 42, 12191233.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Scheffner Hammer, C., & Weiss, A. (2000). African American mothers’ views of their infants’ language development and language-learning environment. American Journal of Speech–Language Pathology, 9, 126140.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Shimpi, P., & Huttenlocher, J. (2007). Redirective labels and early vocabulary development. Journal of Child Language, 34, 115.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Shneidman, L., Buresh, J., Shimpi, P., Knight-Schwartz, J., & Woodward, A. L. (2009). Word learning in an overhearing paradigm: Relations with social experience and attention patterns. Language Learning and Development, 5, 266281.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Snow, C. E. (1977). The development of conversation between mothers and babies. Journal of Child Language, 4, 122.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Snow, C. E., Arlman-Rupp, A., Hassing, Y., Jobse, J., Joosten, J., & Vorster, J. (1976). Mothers’ speech in three social classes. Journal of Psycholinguistic Research, 5, 120.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Tamis-LeMonda, C. S., Bornstein, M. H., & Baumwell, L. (2001). Maternal responsiveness and children's achievement of language milestones. Child Development, 72, 748767.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Tamis-LeMonda, C. S., & Bornstein, M. H. (1994). Specificity in mother–toddler language play: Relations across the second year. Developmental Psychology, 30, 283292.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Tamis-LeMonda, C. S., Bornstein, M. H., Kahana-Kalman, R., Baumwell, L., & Cyphers, L. (1998). Predicting variation in the timing of language milestones in the second year: An events history approach. Journal of Child Language, 675–700.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Tomasello, M., & Farrar, M. (1986). Joint attention and early language. Child Development, 57, 14541463.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Tomasello, M., Strosberg, R., & Akhtar, N. (1996). Eighteen-month-old children learn words in non-ostensive contexts. Journal of Child Language, 23, 157176.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Tomasello, M., & Todd, J. (1983). Joint attention and lexical acquisition style. First Language, 4, 197212.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Van Kleeck, A. (1994). Potential cultural bias in training parents as conversational partners with their children who have delays in language development. American Journal of Speech–Language Pathology, 3, 6778.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Vigil, D. (2002). Cultural variations in attention regulation: A comparative analysis of British and Chinese-immigrant populations. International Journal of Language and Communication Disorders 37, 433458.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Vigil, D., Tylar, A., & Ross, S. (2006). Cultural differences in learning novel words in an attention-following versus attention-directing style. Journal of Multilingual Communication Disorders, 4, 5970.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Vigil, D. C., & Hwa-Froelich, D. A. (2004). Interaction styles in minority caregivers: Implications for intervention. Communication Disorders Quarterly, 25, 119126.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Wing, C., Kohnert, K., Pham, G., Cordero, K. N., Ebert, K. D., Kan, P. F., et al. (2007). Culturally consistent treatment for late talkers. Communication Disorders Quarterly, 29, 2027.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Yoder, P. J., & Warren, S. F. (1999). Maternal responsivity mediates the relationship between prelinguistic intentional communication and later language. Journal of Early Intervention, 22, 126136.CrossRefGoogle Scholar