Hostname: page-component-78c5997874-xbtfd Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-11-05T10:38:51.124Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Referential place-holding in Chinese children's acquisition of English articles

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  28 November 2008

Kang Lee
Affiliation:
University of New Brunswick
Catherine Ann Cameron*
Affiliation:
University of New Brunswick
Murrary J. Linton
Affiliation:
University of New Brunswick
Anne K. Hunt
Affiliation:
University of New Brunswick
*
Catherine Ann Cameron, Department of Psychology, University of New Brunswick, Fredericton, New Brunswick E3B 6E4, Canada. E-mail: Cameron @ UNB.CA.Bitnet

Abstract

This longitudinal study examines the acquisition of English articles by three 6-year-old, second language learning children whose native tongue is Chinese, a language without articles. Brown's coding scheme and an extended coding scheme were used in scoring the corpora of children's responses to a Syntax Elicitation Task. Results revealed that the Chinese children's acquisition of the definite article differed from- what had been previously found using Brown's coding scheme with English as first language learners and second language learning children of other native language origins. Chinese children's use of the definite article developed through an unmarked phase, a referential place-holding phase, a marked phase, and a referential substitution phase before the definite article was fully acquired. The acquisition of the indefinite article, on the other hand, was similar to the acquisition pattern already reported for children learning English as a first language or as a second language. It is suggested that referential place-holding, as well as referential substitution, might not be a Chinese-specific second language learning phenomenon; rather, they might be derived from a universal referential strategy for learning articles.

Type
Articles
Copyright
Copyright © Cambridge University Press 1994

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

REFERENCES

Aaronson, D., & Ferres, S. (1987). The impact of language differences on language processing: An example from Chinese-English bilingualism. In Homel, P., Palij, M. & Aaronson, D. (Eds.), Childhood bilingualism: Aspects of linguistic, cognitive and social development (pp. 75119). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Bickerton, D. (1984). The language bioprogram hypothesis. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 7, 173188.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Brown, R. (1973). A first language: The early stages. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Chomsky, N. (1962). A transformational approach to syntax. Paper presented at the third conference on the problem of linguistic analysis of syntax, University of Texas, Austin.Google Scholar
Cziko, G. A. (1986). Testing the language bioprogram hypothesis: A review of children's acquisition of articles. Language, 62, 878898.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Garton, A. F. (1983). An approach to the study of determiners in early language development. Journal of Psycholinguistic Research, 12, 513525.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Grannis, O. C. (1972). The definite article conspiracy in English. Language Learning, 22, 275289.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hakuta, K. (1976). A case study of a Japanese child learning English as a second language. Language Learning. 26, 321351.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Huebner, T. (1985). System and variability in interlanguage syntax. Language Learning, 35, 141163.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Johnson, J. S., & Newport, E. L. (1989). Critical period effects in second language learning: The influence of maturational state on the acquisition of English as a second language. Cognitive Psychology, 21, 6099.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Karmiloff-Smith, A. (1979a). A functional approach to child language. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Karmiloff-Smith, A. (1979b). Micro- and macrodevelopmental changes in language acquisition and other representational systems. Cognitive Science, 3, 91118.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Karmiloff-Smith, A. (1985). Language and cognitive processes from a developmental perspective. Language and Cognitive Processes, 1, 6185.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Leopold, W. F. (1949). Speech development of a bilingual child (Vol. 4). New York: AMS Press.Google Scholar
Lyons, J. (1975). Deixis as the source of reference. In Keenan, E. L. (Ed.), Formal semantics of natural language (pp. 125143). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Lyons, J. (1977). Semantics 2. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Maratsos, M. P. (1976). The use of definite and indefinite reference in young children. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
McLaughlin, B. (1984). Second-language acquisition in childhood: Vol. 1. Preschool children. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
McLaughlin, B. (1987). Theories of second language learning. London: Edward Arnold.Google Scholar
Perlmutter, D. (1970). On the article in English. In Bierwisch, M. & Heidolph, K. E. (Eds.), Progress in linguistics (pp. 233248). The Hague: Mouton.Google Scholar
Power, R. J. D., & Dal, Martello M. F. (1986). The use of definite and indefinite articles by Italian preschool children. Journal of Child Language, 13, 145154.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Selinker, L., Swain, M. & Dumas, G. (1975). The interlanguage hypothesis extended to children. Language Learning, 25, 139191.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Slobin, D. I. (1973). Cognitive prerequisites for the development of grammar. In Ferguson, C. & Slobin, D. I. (Eds.), Studies in child language development (pp. 175208). New York: Holt, Rinehart & Winston.Google Scholar
Sommerstein, A. H. (1972). On the so-called definite article in English. Language Inquiry, 3, 197209.Google Scholar
Strauss, S. (Ed.). (1982). U-shaped behavioral growth. New York: Academic.Google Scholar
Thomas, M. (1989). The acquisition of English articles by first- and second-language learners. Applied Psycholinguistics, 10, 335355.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Thorne, J. (1974). Notes on “Notes on ‘On the notion “definite.”’” Foundation of Language, 11, 111114.Google Scholar
Wells, G. (1986). Language development in the pre-school years. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Zehler, A. M., & Brewer, W. F. (1982). Sequence and principles in article system use: An examination of a, the and null articles. Child Development, 53, 12681274.Google Scholar