We use cookies to distinguish you from other users and to provide you with a better experience on our websites. Close this message to accept cookies or find out how to manage your cookie settings.
An abstract is not available for this content so a preview has been provided. Please use the Get access link above for information on how to access this content.
Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)
References
Carlson, G. N., & Tanenhaus, M. K. (1988). Thematic roles and language comprehension. Syntax and Semantics. 21, 263–288.Google Scholar
Carlson, G. N. & Tanenhaus, M. K. (1989). Linguistic structure in language processing. Dordrecht: Kluwer.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Graesser, A., & Bower, G. (Eds.). (1990). The psychology of learning and motivation: Vol. 25. Inferences and text comprehension. New York: Academic.Google Scholar
Just, M. A., & Carpenter, P. A. (1992). A capacity theory of comprehension: Individual differences in working memory. Psychological Review, 99, 122–149.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kintsch, W., & van Dijk, T. A. (1978). Toward a model of text comprehension and production. Psychological Review, 85, 363–394.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Miller, G. A. (1962). Some psychological studies of grammar. American Psychologist, 17, 748–762.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Morton, J. (1964). A preliminary functional model for language behavior. International Audiology, 3, 216–225.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Rumelhart, D. E., & McClelland, J. L. (1982). An interactive activation model of context effects in letter perception. Part II. Psychological Review, 89, 60–94.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed